- 1Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
- 2Reykjavík University, Reykjavík, Iceland
- 3Department of Neuromedicine and Movement Science, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
Literacy is the cornerstone of a primary school education and enables the intellectual and social development of young children. Letter-sound knowledge has been identified as critical for developing proficiency in reading. This study explored the development of letter-sound knowledge in relation to gender during the first year of primary school. 485 Norwegian children aged 5–6 years completed assessment of letter-sound knowledge, i.e., uppercase letters- name; uppercase letter -sound; lowercase letters- name; lowercase letter-sound. The children were tested in the beginning, middle, and end of their first school year. The results revealed a clear gender difference in all four variables in favor of the girls which were relatively constant over time. Implications for understanding the role of gender and letter-sound knowledge for later reading performance are discussed.
Introduction
Literacy is one of the main goals of primary education and is attained predominantly through reading, writing, speaking and listening (Rose, 2006). All of these domains facilitate children's intellectual, emotional and social development. A lack of basic reading skills at a young age is later associated with behavior problems and academic shortcomings (Adams, 1990; Elbaum et al., 2000; Tønnessen and Uppstad, 2015). At its most fundamental level, reading involves connecting vision to sound and attaching semantics toward these units of communication. Achieving a sufficient level of awareness and automaticity in reading requires a systematic knowledge of phonemes, i.e., knowing the sound of each letter in the alphabet (Hulme et al., 2012; Tønnessen and Uppstad, 2015).
Importance of Letter-Sound Knowledge for Reading Development
Literacy has a profound impact on the human brain (Dehaene, 2011). Recent brain imaging studies have identified specific locations in which symbols, such as letters, are processed. The importance of the visual word form area (VWFA) in the left lateral occipitotemporal sulcus is hypothesized to play a crucial role in human's processing of letters and words (Dehaene et al., 2002; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011). Dehaene and colleagues contend that the most influential factor for learning how to read is the attachment of meaning to symbols, of which letters and phonemes are the most crucial building block. In evolutionary term, reading is a relatively novel invention that relies on our finely tuned visual system to decode the meaning of symbols. This decoding process can be considered as a form of “neuronal recycling”: we use parts of the brain evolved for specific visual functioning in order to read. Connecting sound with these symbols, i.e., sublexical reading, is therefore of critical importance in order to decode whole words that compromise human language (Dehaene et al., 2010). As Tønnessen and Uppstad (2015) point out, the knowledge that words consist of a subset of sounds is an essential component of fruitful reading development. Furthermore, systematic phonics instruction seems to be essential in early reading skill acquirement (National Reading Panel, 2000; Ehri et al., 2001; Rose, 2006; Tønnessen and Uppstad, 2015). This includes learning all phonemes connected to each letter in the alphabet before learning to manipulate words, as this yields specific and advantageous cortical changes at an early stage in development (Dehaene et al., 2010; Dehaene, 2011). Hulme et al. (2012) found that letter-sound knowledge and phoneme awareness can both causally influence the development of early literacy skills. From a neurological perspective, the amount of systematic phonics teaching (i.e., learning letters one by one) in school seems to be the best predictor of reading comprehension in young children (Ehri et al., 2001). Similar results have been seen in adult readers, whereof grapheme- phoneme training yielded better results and more left lateralized specialization when learning to read texts with novel symbols (Yoncheva et al., 2015). Environmental factors seems not to be sufficient to explain the total variability in letter knowledge (Torppa et al., 2006), however, research concerning the cognitive basis of letter knowledge remain limited and largely unexplored (de Jong and Olson, 2004).
Gender Differences in Reading
Matthews et al. (2009) highlighted the growing gender gap in academic achievement. Girls tend to progress more efficiently academically and to attain higher levels of education than boys (Birch and Gary, 1998; Silverman, 2003). Results from PISA, PIRLS, and the Norwegian National Tests confirm this view by indicating a gender gap in reading performance (Mullis et al., 2012; Stoet and Geary, 2013; OECD, 2014). According to recent PISA results, there is a significant difference in reading of 15-year-old Norwegian boys and girls (OECD, 2014). Although both genders are above the OECD average in reading, Norwegian girls outperform boys with an average score of 528 vs. 481 respectively. The average difference between boys and girls across the OECD is 38.
These, empirical observations raise questions as to how these gender differences arise. Maturation effects might explain some of the differences, for example, research has shown faster vocabulary growth in girls at an early age (Huttenlocher et al., 1991; McCune, 1992). Reznick and Goldfield (1989) found that vocabulary growth for children under 2 years old was faster for girls, and Hohm et al. (2007) found a significant difference between the genders at the age of 10 months in language in favor of the girls. Giedd et al. suggest that sex hormones might be an underlying factor (Giedd et al., 2012). The elevated levels of testosterone in the male fetal brain seem to slow the development of the left hemisphere, which could explain why boys are more likely to have reading difficulties (Geschwind and Galaburda, 1987; Tønnessen and Uppstad, 2015). Furthermore, boys seem to develop executive functions, such as processing speed and visuospatial working memory, at a slower rate than girls (Dekker et al., 2013; Stoet and Geary, 2013). Girls also seem to have deeper engagement and more motivation for reading (McKenna et al., 1995; Lynn and Mikk, 2009), as well as being more socially oriented; factors that could greatly affects language development from an early age (Halpern, 2012). Additionally, mothers tend to communicate verbally more frequently with girls than with boys (Halverson and Waldrop, 1970; Cherry and Lewis, 1978).
Based on the current evidence, development of letter-sound knowledge seems to have a multicausal explanation (Stoet and Geary, 2013). Regardless, the education system is the most prevalent environmental factor in the development of reading skills and, therefore, has a responsibility to create fruitful learning environments for both genders.
Letter-sound knowledge has been found to be crucial for later reading development (National Reading Panel, 2000; Piasta and Wagner, 2010; Hulme et al., 2012) and also shown to be the best predictor of children's future spelling and reading abilities (Scarborough, 1998; Hammill, 2004; Schatschneider et al., 2004). Research indicates a significant difference that favors girls, in letter-sound knowledge among Norwegian school-aged children (Sigmundsson et al., 2017a). However, research on gender differences in letter-sound knowledge is still limited (Dodd and Carr, 2003) and little is known on how these gender differences emerge in childhood. No studies have examined whether the gender gap in letter-sound knowledge changes longitudinally in a Norwegian speaking sample, this could be of importance since this language is considered to have a transparent orthography.
Broadening our understanding about this critical reading skill is important as it could influence and inform future teaching approaches. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine how letter-sound knowledge develops in girls and boys during their first year of primary school. Based on the existing literature, it is reasonable to hypothesize that the gender-gap in letter-sound knowledge would tend to exist in the first year of school.
Methods
Study Design and Participants
A total of 485 children between five and six-years old were included in this study. The participants completed an assessment of letter-sound knowledge (Bokstavtesten) (Ofteland, 1992) at the beginning, middle, and end of the first year of primary school.
The children (N = 485, 224 girls and 261 boys), were selected from 28 schools in Norway (convenience sampling as the schools were invited to participate). The mean chronological age of the entire group at the start of the project was 6.1 years (SD = 0.3); the overall range was 5.67–6.67 years.
The schools varied in size and location (from urban to suburban), included pupils with different sociocultural- and economic backgrounds, and was representative of Norwegian 1st grade students. Exclusions criteria included; uncorrected visual deficit; behavioral, neurological or orthopedic condition; a history of learning difficulties or any other medical condition that could potentially interfere with the ability to carry out the tests.
Measurements
Letter-Sound Knowledge
Letter-sound knowledge was assessed using the Norwegian version of the Letter-sound knowledge test (LSK test; “Bokstavtesten”; Ofteland, 1992). In the LSK test, participants are presented with the alphabet of printed letters, and verbally indicate how many of the uppercase- (e.g., “A, B, C,…”) and lowercase letter (e.g., “a, b, c,…”) they know the name and sound of. There are 29 letters in the Norwegian alphabet, which is based upon the Latin alphabet and is identical to the Danish alphabet.
The LSK test takes about 10 min to complete and consists of two sheets, one for the uppercase letters and one for lowercase letters.
The LSK test has proved to be a reliable and valid test of isolated word decoding proficiency (Ofteland, 1992). We estimated the convergent construct validity of the test battery by comparing the LSK ranking of the test scores of 20 Norwegian children (mean age 6.05 years, SD 0.28) with the rankings provided by the teacher of the same children. There was a moderate association between the rankings based on the teacher's evaluation and the rankings of test scores, with a Spearman rho correlation of 0.683.
The relative test-retest reliability of the test-battery was estimated using ICC (2, 1) (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979). The results were indicative of good reliability for individual test item scores, with ICCs between test and retest scores ranging from 0.985 to 0.992 (mean age 6.05 years, SD 0.28) in Norwegian children (N = 20) (Sigmundsson et al., 2017a).
Procedure
Full ethical review and approval were not required for this study in accordance with the national and institutional guidelines, however, the study was carried out in compliance with the recommendations of the Norwegian Centre for Research Data and the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed documents were obtained from the parents of all participants prior to the study initiation. Identification numbers were used to maintain confidentiality.
The assessment took place in a quiet room during school hours and was conducted according to the LSK manual. All of the participants were tested individually by teachers trained in the test protocols.
Each test item was thoroughly explained before the participants started.
Data Reduction and Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Version 19 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The occurrence of missing data was treated by listwise deletion. For the total score analysis and the analyses of lowercase letter-name and lowercase letter-sound the N was 411 (girls n = 186; boys n = 225). For the analyses of uppercase letter-name letter large name and uppercase letter-sound the N was 485 (girls n = 224; boys n = 261). Differences in letter-sound knowledge between girls and boys over time were assessed using the General Linear Model—repeated measure for the total scores of the four different measurements of letter-sound knowledge. Gender was used as the between-subjects factor, and time of testing was used as the repeated- measures factor. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Because the assumption of sphericity was violated in the data sets analyzed by repeated measures, we applied the Greenhouse–Geisser correction.
Results
Descriptive statistics of the scores for uppercase letters (name and sound), and lowercase letters (name and sound) for both genders over time are shown in Table 1. Higher scores indicate better performance (more knowledge of the letters and their sound).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of score for amount of uppercase letters name, uppercase letters sound, lowercase letters name, and lowercase letters sound for 5–6 year old girls and boys.
General linear model (repeated measures, mixed model analysis of variance) revealed significant main effect of gender [F(1, 409) = 13,636, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.032]. Thus, there was an overall difference in letter-sound knowledge between girls and boys. A significant main effect was obtained for time [F(1.553, 635) = 1,030, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.716], both groups scores significantly higher on the letter-sound knowledge measures after a period of 9 months. However, there was no significant time x gender interaction [F(1.553, 635) = 0.456, ns; partial η2 = 0.001]. These results demonstrate that, over time, the development in letter-sound knowledge were similar for both girls and boys, but that the relative difference in performance between genders tended to persist (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Letter–sound knowledge for girls and boys the first year at school. Mean score and standard error bars are presented. (A) Uppercase letter- name over time. (B) Uppercase letter-sound over time. (C) Lowercase letter- name over time. (D) Lowercase letter-sound over time. Time 1, September; Time 2, January/February. Time 3, May/June.
The detailed results of the analysis of variance are reported below for each test item.
Uppercase Letter–Name
A significant main effect was obtained for time [F(1.535, 741) = 776, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.617].
A significant main effect also was obtained for gender [F(1, 483) = 20.4, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.041]. There was no significant interaction effect [F(1.535, 741) = 0.717, p = ns; partial η2 = 0.001].
Uppercase Letter-Sound
A significant main effect was obtained for time [F(1.577, 761) = 1,007, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.676].
A significant main effect also was obtained for gender [F(1, 483) = 14.4, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.029]. There was no significant interaction effect [F(1.577, 761) = 0.738, p = ns; partial η2 = 0.002].
Lowercase Letter-Name
A significant main effect was obtained for time [F(1.736, 709) = 779, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.656].
A significant main effect also was obtained for gender [F(1, 409) = 15.1, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.036]. There was no significant interaction effect [F(1, 736, 709) = 0.442, p = ns; partial η2 = 0.001].
Lowercase Letter-Sound
A significant main effect was obtained for time [F(1, 670, 683) = 798, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.661].
A significant main effect also was obtained for gender [F(1, 409) = 13.1, p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.031]. There was no significant interaction effect [F(1, 670, 683) = 0.351, p = ns; partial η2 = 0.001].
Discussion
The aim of this study was to examine how letter-sound knowledge develops in girls and boys during their first year of primary school. Four letter-sound knowledge factors were tracked over a 9-month period from September to June.
The main effect of gender revealed an overall difference in letter-sound knowledge between girls and boys, favoring girls on all the four factors. The boys performed worse than the girls in all four factors at all three-time points (Table 1 and Figure 1). These findings are in accordance with studies that report lower reading competency in 10-year-old (Mullis et al., 2003, 2007) and 14-year-old (OECD, 2014) boys.
A significant mean effect of time indicated that both groups scored significantly higher on the letter-sound knowledge test after a period of 9 months. There was no significant interaction effect, indicating that the relative differences in letter-sound knowledge outcomes between the genders were maintained over time (see Figure 1). The relative difference between uppercase letter- name/sounds and lowercase letter name/sounds followed the same pattern.
Firstly, the findings from our study indicate a gender gap in letter-sound knowledge that is already present when they first attend primary school, and secondly, that these differences tend to persist throughout their first year. Research has demonstrated the impact of letter-sound knowledge on future reading skills (Hulme et al., 2012; Torppa et al., 2018). The gender gap, already observed in 5-to-6-year-old children, accumulates and may be one of several factors that explain the gender differences found in PISA 2015. PISA 2015, which presents the performance of 15 year-old student's, reported that about 20% of students in OECD countries do not achieve baseline levels of reading proficiency. This percentage has remained stable since 2009. That study also revealed that girls outperform boys in all countries and economies (OECD, 2016). “In 2012, 14% of boys and 9% of girls did not attain the PISA baseline level of proficiency in any of the three subjects measured in PISA – reading, mathematics and science” (p. 13) (OECD, 2015).
Since dropping out is a dynamic developmental process with various influencing factors such as social equalities, early action should be taken at multiple levels. However, it is possible to argue that gender differences contribute to this complex phenomenon. Early childhood literacy is fundamental for almost all school subjects and the substantial amount of boys who fail to attain proficient reading skills represents a major challenge for education systems (Stanovich, 1986; Entwisle et al., 2007). Furthermore, it is easy to imagine how poor performance in these basic skills leads to decreased motivation for further practicing and learning, thereby perpetuating the vicious cycle of demotivation. The education system might also be exacerbating these gender differences if adapted teaching methods are not initiated.
Specific training and systematic practice (Kleim and Jones, 2008; Sigmundsson et al., 2013, 2017b) are required to effectively learn the letters of an alphabet and their phonetic usage. Therefore, one could reason that boys have less training/experience than girls at this age. In this respect, it seems wise to advocate for systematic and thorough learning of letters and their sounds as early as possible, at least by the first year of school. To close the gender gap, we need to firstly find out the level of each child in letter-sound knowledge and secondly provide each child with the right challenges for their intervention/training (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975, 2008). Thirdly we need to have good follow-up of children in relation to new challenges/training (Kleim and Jones, 2008). This is in line with Dehaene (2011) who argues that: “Grapheme-phoneme correspondences must be systematically taught, one by one: the amount of such teaching is the best predictor of reading performance…” (p. 26). Hulme et al. (2012) also supports the importance of letter-sound knowledge and phonemic skills and suggest that these factors should be directly taught to all children in the early stages of their academic careers. The arguments for using synthetic phonics are supported by a number of researches in both experimental studies and large scale assessments (National Reading Panel, 2000; Ehri et al., 2001; Levin et al., 2006; Rose, 2006).
Author Contributions
HS and MH: idea, analysis, and writing; AD: analysis and writing; GO: idea and writing.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Birch, S. H., and Gary, W. (1998). Children's interpersonal behaviours and the teacher-child relationship. Dev. Psychol. 34, 934–946. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.934
Cherry, L., and Lewis, M. (1978). “Differential socialization of girls and boys: implications for sex differences in language development,” in The Development of Communication, eds N. Waterson and C. Snow (New York, NY: Wiley), 189–197.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2008). “Flyt og læring (Flow and Learning),” in Læring og Ferdighetsutvikling (Learning and Skill Development), ed H. Sigmundsson (Trondheim: Tapir), 119–129.
Dehaene, S. (2011). The massive impact of literacy on the brain and its consequences for education. human neuroplasticity and education. Pontic. Acad. Sci. 2011, 19–32.
Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends. Cogn. Sci. 15, 254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003
Dehaene, S., Le Clec, H. G., Poline, J. B., Le Bihan, D., and Cohen, L. (2002). The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus. Neuroreport 13, 321–325. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200203040-00015
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Brag, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., et al. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330, 1359–1364. doi: 10.1126/science.1194140
de Jong, P. F., and Olson, R. K. (2004). Early predictors of letter knowledge. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 88, 254–273. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.007
Dekker, S., Krabbendam, L., Aben, A., de Groot, R., and Jolles, J. (2013). Coding task performance in early adolescence: a large-scale controlled study into boy-girl differences. Front. Psychol. 4:550. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00550
Dodd, B., and Carr, A. (2003). Young children's letter-sound knowledge. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 34, 128–137. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2003/011)
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., and Willows, D. M. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the national Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 71, 393–447. doi: 10.3102/00346543071003393
Elbaum, B., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M. T., and Moody, S. W. (2000). How effective are one-to-one tutoring programs in reading for elementary students at risk for reading failure? A metaanalysis of the intervention research. J. Educ. Psychol. 92, 605–619. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.605
Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., and Olsen, L. S. (2007). Early Schooling: the handicap of being poor and male. Sociol. Educ. 80, 114–138. doi: 10.1177/003804070708000202
Giedd, J. N., Raznahan, A., Mills, K. L., and Lenroot, R. K. (2012). Review: magnetic resonance imaging of male/female differences in human adolescent brain anatomy. Biol. Sex Differ. 3:19. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-19
Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Halverson, C. F., and Waldrop, M. F. (1970). Maternal behavior toward own and other preschool children: the problem of “ownness”. Child Devel. 41, 839–845. doi: 10.2307/1127229
Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Except. Child. 70, 453–469. doi: 10.1177/001440290407000405
Hohm, E., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Schmidt, M. H., and Laucht, M. (2007). Language development at ten months: predictive of language outcome and school achievement ten years later? Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 16, 149–156. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0567-y
Hulme, C., Bowyer-Crane, C., Carroll, J. M., Duff, F. J., and Snowling, M. J. (2012). The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in learning to read: combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychol. Sci. 23, 572–577. doi: 10.1177/0956797611435921
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236
Kleim, J. A., and Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. J. Speech Lang Hear. Res. 51, S225–S239. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/018)
Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., and Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 93, 139–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.08.002
Lynn, R., and Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. TRAMES 13, 3–13. doi: 10.3176/tr.2009.1.01
Matthews, J. S., Morrison, F. J., and Ponitz, C. C. (2009). Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. J. Edu. Psychol. 101, 689–704. doi: 10.1037/a0014240
McCune, L. (1992). “First words: a dynamic systems view,” in Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications, eds C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn, and C. Stoel-Gammon (Parkton, MD: York Press), 331–336.
McKenna, M. C., Kear, D. J., and Ellsworth, R. A. (1995). Children's attitudes toward reading: a national survey. Read. Res. Q. 30, 934–956.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., and Drucker, K. T. (2012). TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., and Kennedy, A. M. (2003). PIRLS 2001 International Report: IEA's Study of Reading Literacy Achievement in Primary Schools in 35 Countries. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Kennedy, A. M., and Foy, P. (2007). PIRLS 2006 International Report: IEA's Progress in International Reading Literacy Study in Primary Schools in 40 Countries. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS and PIRLS International Study Center, Lynch School of Education, Boston College.
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
OECD (2014). PISA 2012 Results: What Students Know and Can Do – Student Performance in Mathematics, Reading and Science, Vol. 1. Revised edition, February 2014, PISA, OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264201118-en
OECD (2015). The ABC of Gender equality in Education: Aptitude, Behaviour, Confidence, PISA, OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264229945-en
OECD (2016) PISA 2015 Results, Vol. 1: Excellence and Equity in Education, Paris: PISA, OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264266490-en.
Ofteland, G. S. (1992). Ein tekst veks Fram. Kva Kjennetegner Elevtekster på Barnesteget 1.-3. Klasse? Hovedoppgåve ved Avdeling for Nordisk Språk og Litteratur. Universitet i Oslo.
Piasta, S. B., and Wagner, R. K. (2010). Developing Early Literacy skills: a meta-analysis of alphabet learning and instruction. Read. Res. Q. 45, 8–38. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.45.1.2
Reznick, J. S., and Goldfield, B. A. (1989). Rapid Change in Language Acquisition during the Second Year: Naming Explosion or Knowing Explosion. Kansas City, MO: Society for Research in Child Development Poster Presentation.
Rose, J. (2006). Independent Review of the Teaching of Early Reading. Final Report. Department for Education and Skills, UK.
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). “Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: phonological awareness and some other promising predictors,” in Specific Reading Disability: A View of the Spectrum, eds B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, and A. J. Capute (Timonium, MD: York Press), 75–119.
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., and Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: a longitudinal comparative analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 96:265. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.2.265
Shrout, P. E., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol. Bull. 86, 420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
Sigmundsson, H., Eriksen, A. D., Ofteland, G. S., and Haga, M. (2017a). Letter-sound knowledge: exploring gender differences in children when they start school regarding knowledge of large letters, small letters, sound large letters and sound small letter. Front. Psychol. 8:1539. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01539
Sigmundsson, H., Polman, R. C., and Lorås, H. (2013). Exploring individual differences in children's mathematical skills: a correlational and dimensional approach. Psychol. Rep. 113, 23–30. doi: 10.2466/04.10.PR0.113x12z2
Sigmundsson, H., Trana, L., Polman, R., and Haga, M. (2017b). What is trained develops! theoretical perspective on skill learning. Sports 5:38. doi: 10.3390/sports5020038
Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: a meta–analysis. Sex Roles 49, 451–463. doi: 10.1023/A:1025872421115
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Read. Res. Q. 21, 360–406. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1
Stoet, G., and Geary, D. C. (2013). Sex differences in mathematics and reading achievement are inversely related: within- and across-Nation Assessment of 10 Years of PISA Data. PLoS ONE 8:e57988. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057988
Tønnessen, F. E., and Uppstad, P. H. (2015). Can We Read Letters? Reflections on Fundamental Issues in Reading and Dyslexia Research. Rotterdam; Boston, MA; Taipei: Sense Publishers.
Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Sulkunen, S., Niemi, P., and Ahonen, T. (2018). Why do boys and girls perform differently on PISA Reading in Finland? The effects of reading fluency, achievement behaviour, leisure reading and homework activity. J. Res. Read. 41, 122–139. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12103
Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A. M., Laakso, M. L., Eklund, K., and Lyytinen, H. (2006). Prediciting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Dev. Psychol. 42, 1128–1142. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1128
Keywords: children, gender differences, uppercase, lowercase, letter-name, letter-sound, longitudinal
Citation: Sigmundsson H, Dybfest Eriksen A, Ofteland GS and Haga M (2018) Gender Gaps in Letter-Sound Knowledge Persist Across the First School Year. Front. Psychol. 9:301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00301
Received: 24 November 2017; Accepted: 22 February 2018;
Published: 08 March 2018.
Edited by:
Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United KingdomReviewed by:
Dexin Shi, University of South Carolina, United StatesJulia Mary Carroll, Coventry University, United Kingdom
Julia Ai Cheng Lee, Universiti Malaysia Sarawak, Malaysia
Marina Leite Puglisi, Federal University of São Paulo, Brazil
Copyright © 2018 Sigmundsson, Dybfest Eriksen, Ofteland and Haga. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hermundur Sigmundsson, hermundurs@svt.ntnu.no