Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 19 December 2017
Sec. Psychology of Language

Commentary: Ancient genomes show social and reproductive behavior of early Upper Paleolithic foragers

  • Department of Spanish, Linguistics, and Theory of Literature (Linguistics), Universidad de Sevilla, Seville, Spain

A commentary on
Ancient genomes show social and reproductive behavior of early Upper Paleolithic foragers

by Sikora, M., Seguin-Orlando, A., Sousa, V. C., Albrechtsen, A., Korneliussen, T., Ko, A., et al. (2017). Science 358, 659–662. doi: 10.1126/science.aao1807

Confident reconstructions of prehistoric languages are precluded by the widespread occurrence of borrowing resulting from language contact, which blurs the phylogenetic links between languages resulting from common ancestry. Because languages are cultural systems that can be learned and shifted (even when they are modeled by the cognitive faculty that enables us to acquire and use them), attempts to reconstruct language phylogenies based purely on the dynamics of human gene pools and particularly, on human genetic phylogenies have also failed to a great extent. As noted by Pakendorf (2014), although linguistic boundaries sometimes act as barriers to gene flow, genetic admixture frequently takes place irrespective of linguistic affiliations.

These circumstances explain why until now characterizations of prehistoric languages and models of language dynamics in the remote past have largely relied on linguistic evidence and theories. Typological surveys of present-day languages, as well as some celebrated depictions of how language is implemented in our brains (e.g., Chomskyan linguistics), have crystallized in a uniformitarian view of the nature of languages (Fromkin and Rodman, 1983; Dixon, 1997), according to which the languages spoken in the past were roughly equal to the languages we speak today in terms of overall complexity. Likewise, favorite theories in historical linguistics, like grammaticalization theory (which accounts for the emergence of functional words in the grammars of languages), have been used to circumvent the limits of linguistic reconstructions and to infer how languages (and language) might have been in remote prehistory (e.g., Heine and Kuteva, 2007). Nonetheless, the resulting picture, even if plausible, is difficult to prove, as no remains of the languages spoken at that times are available.

Recent research by anthropological and evolutionary linguists suggests that structural aspects of languages correlate with (and might depend on) environmental and social factors (Lupyan and Dale, 2016). In particular, small, close-knit human communities tend to speak languages with complex, opaque, and redundant morphologies, reduced semantic transparency, and limited syntactic devices, to the point that core aspects of human languages, like recursion, might be absent (Everett, 2005). On the contrary, increased cross-cultural exchanges, and ultimately, widespread language contact, are hypothesized to regularize morphological paradigms, augment semantic transparency, and trigger compositionality and more elaborated syntaxes (Bolender, 2007; Wray and Grace, 2007; Lupyan and Dale, 2010; Trudgill, 2011). Overall, this suggests that we might achieve a better understanding of the nature of prehistoric languages if we knew more about how social dynamics were in the past.

Until recently, our understanding of the socio-cultural milieu of Paleolithic humans was limited to what can be inferred from the archeological record. It seems, for instance, that Magdalenian humans (from ca. 15,000 years before present) lived in small bands with quite elaborated social systems and extensive long-distance contacts (Weniger, 1989; Schwendler, 2012). Not surprisingly, we know more about recent times than about distant epochs. Now Sikora et al. (2017) have shown that humans from around 34,000 years before present were already organized in small groups with limited within-band kinship and inbreeding, and with wide social, mating networks, resembling the way in which many present-day hunter-gatherers live today. The paper by Sikora et al. is important because of two reasons. First, it shows that certain human behaviors and socializing patterns are quite old, and provided that these are confident proxies for aspects of language design, we are allowed to suggest that humans from that remote period spoke languages close, but not identical, to the first type we described above [what Wray and Grace (2007) call esoteric languages]. Second, in their characterization of social dynamics in early Upper Paleolithic, Sikora et al. have not relied on archaeological remains, but on ancient genome sequences. Because protocols for using ancient DNA have been significantly improved over the last years, enabling to reveal snapshots of genetic variation in past populations (Slatkin, 2016; Key et al., 2017), we can anticipate sharp pictures of social dynamics (and accordingly, of language features) from more distant periods of our history, provided that suitable human remains are available. Likewise, knowing more about social dynamics in the remote past, particularly, kinship systems, should help better understand the interrelation between gene and language phylogenies throughout our history (Lansing et al., 2017).

In sum, cutting-edge research in the domain of population genetics and paleogenomics is expected to allow linguists to apply what they have learnt about the languages spoken by present-day human groups and the social conditions favoring their distinctive structural features to a period of our history that is far beyond the limits of the best linguistic reconstructions, and ultimately, to provide with more accurate characterizations of prehistoric languages. Needless to say that caution is in order, particularly, to avoid the temptation of plainly equating modern hunter-gatherers' languages to the languages spoken by prehistoric peoples. No present-day human group is frozen in prehistoric conditions. Change is connatural to language, and subtle modifications of social dynamics and environmental conditions have seemingly occurred over time. The story of click sounds nicely illustrates this: often considered a distinctive feature of the first languages spoken by humans (Knight et al., 2003), linguistic and genetic evidence is also compatible with the view that they might be a recent episode in the diversification of human speech (Güldemann and Stoneking, 2008). That said, and assuming that human cognition has remained substantially the same from the emergence of our species, the patterns of population dynamics and socialization behaviors revealed by paleogenomic studies like the one conducted by Sikora et al. seem a reliable window to the nature of the languages spoken in deep prehistory. These are good news for the fields of historical linguistics and language evolution, but also for anyone interested in human cultural evolution.

Author Contributions

AB-B conceived and wrote the paper. The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Funding

Preparation of this work was supported by funds from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (grant FFI2016-78034-C2-2-P [AEI/FEDER, UE]).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bolender, J. (2007). Prehistoric cognition by description: a Russellian approach to the upper Paleolithic. Biol. Philos. 22, 383–399. doi: 10.1007/s10539-006-9058-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, R. M. W. (1997). The Rise and Fall of Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Everett, D. (2005). Cultural constrains on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Curr. Anthropol. 46, 621–646. doi: 10.1086/431525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fromkin, V., and Rodman, R. (1983). An Introduction to Language. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Google Scholar

Güldemann, T., and Stoneking, M. (2008). A historical appraisal of clicks: a linguistic and genetic population perspective. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 37, 93–109. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.37.081407.085109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heine, B., and Kuteva, T. (2007). The genesis of Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Key, F. M., Posth, C., Krause, J., Herbig, A., and Bos, K. I. (2017). Mining metagenomic data sets for ancient DNA: recommended protocols for authentication. Trends Genet. 33, 508–520. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2017.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, A., Underhill, P. A., Mortensen, H. M., Zhivotovsky, L. A., Lin, A. A., Henn, B. M., et al. (2003). African Y chromosome and mtDNA divergence provides insight into the history of click languages. Curr. Biol. 13:464–473. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00130-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansing, J. S., Abundo, C., Jacobs, G. S., Guillot, E. G., Thurner, S., Downey, S. S., et al. (2017). Kinship structures create persistent channels for language transmission. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 12910–12915. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706416114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupyan, G., and Dale, R. (2010). Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE 5:e8559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupyan, G., and Dale, R. (2016). Why are there different languages? The role of adaptation in linguistic diversity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 649–660. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pakendorf, B. (2014). Coevolution of languages and genes. Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 29, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2014.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwendler, R. H. (2012). Diversity in social organization across Magdalenian Western Europe ca. 17–12,000 BP. Quatern. Int. 272, 333–353. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2012.03.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sikora, M., Seguin-Orlando, A., Sousa, V. C., Albrechtsen, A., Korneliussen, T., Ko, A., et al. (2017). Ancient genomes show social and reproductive behavior of early Upper Paleolithic foragers. Science 358, 659–662. doi: 10.1126/science.aao1807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slatkin, M. (2016). Statistical methods for analyzing ancient DNA from hominins. Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 41, 72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trudgill, P. (2011). Sociolinguistic Typology: Social Determinants of Linguistic Complexity. Oxford: Oxford University Press OUP.

Google Scholar

Weniger, G. C. (1989). The Magdalenian in Western Central Europe: Settlement pattern and regionality. J. World Prehist. 3, 323–372. doi: 10.1007/BF00975326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wray, A., and Grace, G. W. (2007). The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua 117, 543–578. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: language evolution, language complexity, ancient genomes, hunter-gatherers, historical linguistics

Citation: Benítez-Burraco A (2017) Commentary: Ancient genomes show social and reproductive behavior of early Upper Paleolithic foragers. Front. Psychol. 8:2247. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02247

Received: 12 November 2017; Accepted: 11 December 2017;
Published: 19 December 2017.

Edited by:

Niels Janssen, Universidad de La Laguna, Spain

Reviewed by:

Philip R. Nigst, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2017 Benítez-Burraco. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonio Benítez-Burraco, YWJlbml0ZXo4QHVzLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.