Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 April 2017
Sec. Emotion Science

Automatic Processing of Emotional Words in the Absence of Awareness: The Critical Role of P2

  • 1College of Psychology and Sociology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Research Center for Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China
  • 3School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China
  • 4College of Education Science, Chengdu University, Chengdu, China

It has been long debated to what extent emotional words can be processed in the absence of awareness. Behavioral studies have shown that the meaning of emotional words can be accessed even without any awareness. However, functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have revealed that emotional words that are unconsciously presented do not activate the brain regions involved in semantic or emotional processing. To clarify this point, we used continuous flash suppression (CFS) and event-related potential (ERP) techniques to distinguish between semantic and emotional processing. In CFS, we successively flashed some Mondrian-style images into one participant's eye steadily, which suppressed the images projected to the other eye. Negative, neutral, and scrambled words were presented to 16 healthy participants for 500 ms. Whenever the participants saw the stimuli—in both visible and invisible conditions—they pressed specific keyboard buttons. Behavioral data revealed that there was no difference in reaction time to negative words and to neutral words in the invisible condition, although negative words were processed faster than neutral words in the visible condition. The ERP results showed that negative words elicited a larger P2 amplitude in the invisible condition than in the visible condition. The P2 component was enhanced for the neutral words compared with the scrambled words in the visible condition; however, the scrambled words elicited larger P2 amplitudes than the neutral words in the invisible condition. These results suggest that the emotional processing of words is more sensitive than semantic processing in the conscious condition. Semantic processing was found to be attenuated in the absence of awareness. Our findings indicate that P2 plays an important role in the unconscious processing of emotional words, which highlights the fact that emotional processing may be automatic and prioritized compared with semantic processing in the absence of awareness.

Introduction

Emotional words hold an important place in social communication in the modern world. When we see an emotional word, it transmits two main kinds of information. One is semantic information, which contains the meaning of the word, which activates the left lateral occipitotemporal sulci (Dehaene and Cohen, 2011), inferior frontal gyrus (Mestres-Missé et al., 2008; Chou et al., 2012), and angular gyrus (Horwitz et al., 1998; Seghier, 2013); the other is emotional information, which includes the biological value or social significance (Fox et al., 2001), which activates the amygdala (Isenberg et al., 1999; Garavan et al., 2001; Tabert et al., 2001; Hamann and Mao, 2002; Compton et al., 2003; Kensinger and Schacter, 2006; Herbert et al., 2009; García-García et al., 2016), orbitofrontal gyrus and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (Nakic et al., 2006), anterior cingulate gyrus (Posner et al., 2009), and lingual gyrus (Kuchinke et al., 2005). Although semantic and emotional information processing in the brain may largely overlap (Duncan and Barrett, 2007; Shackman et al., 2011; Raz et al., 2012, 2014), these results suggest that semantic and emotional processing of words activated different brain regions during a visible condition. However, it remains unclear to what extent the emotional and semantic processing of words can take place in the absence of conscious awareness.

In the invisible condition, to render the word stimuli invisible, we adopted an effective paradigm called continuous flash suppression (CFS). This interocular suppression technique has been known as a pivotal tool for exploring the visual awareness (Lin and He, 2009; Eo et al., 2016). In the CFS paradigm, some Mondrian-style images flash successively into the dominant eye steadily, which suppresses the experimental materials projected to the non-dominant eye (Kim and Blake, 2005; Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005).

At present, there is no consensus about unconscious processing of words, regardless of unconscious semantic processing or subliminal emotional processing. Firstly, there are conflicting findings for unconscious semantic processing. Some studies have reported the semantic processing of word stimuli in the unconscious conditions (Dehaene et al., 2006; Jiang et al., 2007; Wang and Yuan, 2008; Sklar et al., 2012; Ortells et al., 2016). However, other studies have provided the opposite evidence that the semantic processing of verbal stimuli cannot occur when they are rendered invisibly (Zimba and Blake, 1983; Kang et al., 2011; Heyman and Moors, 2014; Hesselmann et al., 2015). Secondly, the subliminal emotional processing of words remains questionable. For example, in a behavioral study, Sklar et al. (2012) reported that the negative word expression break suppression faster than the neutral word expression, while Yang and Yeh (2011) showed that the negative words took more time to break suppression than the neutral words. Using fMRI, Ortigue et al. (2007) showed that the masked lovers' names activated the fusiform and angular gyri, indicating that emotion-loaded word can activate the emotion-related and word-related areas. In contrast, Hoffmann et al. (2015) found that there was no significant difference of the relevant brain regions between masked emotional words and neutral words in the invisible condition. These fMRI results are inconsistent partly due to the fact that this technique has a low temporal resolution, which does not provide the temporal course for emotional word processing.

Due to high temporal resolution, ERPs are able to reflect the temporal processing of emotional stimuli. We focused on the P2 component for the following reasons. First, P2 was related to attention and categorization around 150–300 ms post-stimulus onset (Antal et al., 2001; Crowley and Colrain, 2004). Second, a plethora of research has found that emotional effect takes place in the P2 time-window in visible conditions (Begleiter and Platz, 1969; Begleiter et al., 1979; Williamson et al., 1991; Schapkin et al., 2000; Ortigue et al., 2004; Kanske and Kotz, 2007; Wang and Bastiaansen, 2014). For instance, Herbert et al. (2006) observed that negative words elicited larger P2 component compared with neutral words. Finally, at the time window around 250 ms after stimulus onset, there exists a positive-going wave (P250) at the whole brain, which is related to both automatic semantic processing and early word recognition (García-Larrea et al., 1992; Kim et al., 2001; Hill et al., 2002; Liu et al., 2009). For example, Chung et al. (2010) have found the P250 reflects that semantic memory network is activated in the semantic processing of Chinese words.

Until now, few studies have distinguished the two types of processing simultaneously in one experiment in absence of conscious awareness (Yang and Yeh, 2011). In the current study, we used an electroencephalography (EEG) method to clarify the extent to which the semantic and emotional processing of words can occur in the absence of awareness within the time-window of P2. The participants observed negative, neutral, and scrambled word stimuli during the visible condition and the invisible condition. In order to distinguish the semantic and emotional processing, we adopted a kind of scrambled stimuli that removed the semantic information and preserved the spatial location features (Yang and Yeh, 2011). We compared scrambled words with neutral words as the semantic process and also compared negative words with neutral words as the emotional process. Based on previous results (Bernat et al., 2001; Herbert et al., 2006; Kang et al., 2011), we predict that emotional processing of words would occur even when the stimuli were presented unconsciously, but semantic processing would be suppressed in absence of awareness.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixteen right-handed students (10 female; mean age 22.9 years) from Liaoning Normal University participated in the experiment and were compensated $4.50 after the experiment. They were all native Chinese speakers and had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity with no psychiatric or neurological history. All participants provided written informed consent. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Liaoning Normal University.

Stimuli

We used 30 negative Chinese two-character words (e.g., ”悲伤,” sadness) and an equal number of neutral words (e.g., ”规则,” rule), which were selected from the Chinese Affective Words System (CAWS; Luo and Wang, 2004; Yi et al., 2015). All of the words were nouns (see Supplementary Material). Negative words and neutral words differed significantly in valence [mean: negative = 2.72, neutral = 5.11; t(58) = −34.79, p < 0.001] and arousal [mean: negative = 5.80, neutral = 4.54; t(58) = 5.75, p < 0.001]. However, they were matched in stroke numbers [mean: negative = 16.77, neutral = 16.37; t(58) = 0.362, p = 0.719], word frequency [mean frequency in 15 million words, negative = 0.002887%, neutral = 0.002357%; t(58) = −0.562, p = 0.578; http://www.cncorpus.org/] and concreteness (this dimension rating was obtained through the use of a seven-point scale by 27 new participants [−3 to 3: very abstract to very concrete; mean: negative 0.72, neutral 1.07; t(58) = −1.154; p = 0.253]). The scrambled word stimuli were constructed by dividing the negative and the neutral words into 5 × 10 blocks and then arranging them randomly. Separating the words removed the semantic information and preserved the effects of spatial location.

To create the interocular suppression, Mondrian images, and stimuli were projected onto each eye of the participant through a four-mirror stereoscope that included two intermediate mirrors (angled ± 45° orthogonally) between two adjustable mirrors. Mondrian patches (extended 7.16° × 10.03°, visual angle) were generated using Matlab 7.0 software and filled an outer frame (visual angle 8.60° × 11.36°) with the colors black and white. The stimuli were drawn using black characters at 28.6% contrast (the contrast of the stimuli was defined as the luminance difference between the background and the luminance of the words divided by the luminance of the background; Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005; Kang et al., 2011). The screen luminance of the word stimuli was set at 20% of the maximal screen luminance (dark gray), and the screen luminance of the background was set at 28% (light gray). Stimuli were presented on a 48 cm CRT (cathode ray tube) monitor (1024 × 768 resolution at 100 Hz frame rate) and controlled by E-Prime 2.0 software. Each participant was seated 60 cm in front of the computer screen with their head on a chin-rest, and responded by using a keyboard.

Design and Procedure

We used a within-subject experimental design in which two factors—word type (negative, neutral, and scrambled words) and awareness states (visible and invisible light conditions)—were manipulated. To control participants' fatigue, neutral, negative, and scrambled word stimuli were presented randomly within one block consisting of 108 trials in the visible condition. With the invisible condition, each block included 60 trials. Each observer took part in five visible blocks and nine invisible blocks of words. The participants could take a rest between the blocks. The total experimental session lasted ~50 min. To assess their eye-dominance, the participants were asked to view an object through a hole made by their own fingers (the “Miles test”; Mendola and Conner, 2007; Axelrod et al., 2014). In the invisible condition, to make the word stimuli invisible, the Mondrian images were changed at a rate of 20 Hz (50 ms per image) and projected onto the dominant eye of each participant. At the same time, the negative, neutral, and scrambled words were presented to the non-dominant eye. At the beginning of each trial, the word stimuli and Mondrian images (extended 3.15° × 1.43°) were presented for 500 ms, and then followed by an 800–1200 ms randomized fixation (a dot, d = 0.38°) in an outer frame, representing an inter-trial interval (ITI; see Figure 1A). In the visible condition, in order to make the participants aware of the word stimuli, we replaced the Mondrian images that had been presented to the dominant eye with word stimuli. The other parts of the visible condition remained the same with the invisible condition (see Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure used in this study. (A) In the invisible condition, Mondrian-style images were presented to the dominant eye at a rate of 20 Hz while negative, neutral, and scrambled words were randomly presented to the non-dominant eye. (B) In the visible condition, negative, neutral, and scrambled words were randomly presented to both of the participant's eyes.

In both the invisible and visible condition, the participants were asked to pay attention to the stimuli and complete a feature detection task. If the participants saw the word stimuli (negative, neutral, and scrambled words), they were instructed to press the “f” key; if not, they pressed the “j” key. The buttons “f” and “j,” which were pressed by the right and left hands—associated with seen and unseen—were counterbalanced across participants. The sequences of the two conditions (invisible and visible) were also counterbalanced across participants. After completing the experiment, the participants were asked to recall the experience of each word when confronted with these words in the formal experiment, and rate the valences and arousals relating to each word using a 9-point Likert scale (valence 1–9; arousal 1–9). The results showed that an emotional word could effectively evoke an affective experience. The valence of the negative words (M ± SE, 3.28 ± 2821) were significantly lower than those of the neutral words (4.90 ± 0.11; t = −7.01, df = 30, p < 0.001). Moreover, the arousal induced by the negative words (5.26 ± 0.35) was much higher than that induced by the neutral words (3.60 ± 0.33, t = 3.451, df = 30, p = 0.002). Next, the participants completed a subjective report on whether the words presented in the experiment could be seen or not. All the participants reported that they did not see the word stimuli in the invisible condition but did distinguish the negative, neutral, and scrambled words in the visible condition. Moreover, the results of the feature detection task revealed that the participants reported not seeing any words in 99.89% of the trials.

Electrophysiological Recording and Analysis

Electroencephalographic (EEG) data were recorded from 64 electrodes using the Active Two system (BioSemi, the Netherlands). These electrodes were referenced online to averaged right and left mastoids. A horizontal electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from an electrode placed 1.5 cm from the external canthus of the right eye. A vertical EOG was recorded from an electrode placed 1.5 cm below the left eye. The signal was recorded at a sampling rate of 512 Hz, filtered at 0–104 Hz and stored for offline analysis. EEG data were edited with NeuroScan equipment (Compumedics) after data conversion using PolyRex software (as described by Graux et al., 2014). Semiautomatic correction of eye movements was applied using an ICA filter transform developed by Brain Products (BP, Germany). A 0.1–30 Hz IIR bandpass filter was applied (24-dB/oct slope). The ERP waveforms were time-locked to the onset of the stimulus and corrected with a −100 to 0 ms baseline before the stimuli were applied. The remaining artifacts with amplitudes higher or lower than +80 μV were automatically rejected.

P2 was investigated in a time-window of 220–280 ms and was calculated as mean amplitude (Chung et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2013). We defined a mid-line ROI to test the P2 component (from five mid-line electrode sites: Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz; Yun et al., 2011). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to analyze the ERPs with awareness states and word types as within-subject factors. Emotional effect was calculated by the differences in the P2 waves between the negative words and the neutral words. Semantic effect was calculated by the differences in the P2 waves between the neutral words and the scrambled words. We also used two-way repeated measures ANOVA to analyze the differences between waves, with awareness states and effects (emotional effect or semantic effect) as within-subject factors. When Mauchly's test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, Greenhouse–Geisser correction would be used to correct the degree of freedom. Bonferroni correction was used to adjust the p-value for all pairwise comparisons.

Results

Behavioral Data

We rejected the outliers of reaction time (RT) that were outside the range of ±2.5 standard deviations from the mean (<1%) and error trials (mean error rate = 1.38%). The results showed that the interaction of word type and awareness state was not significant [F(2, 30) = 2.679, p = 0.119, ηp2 = 0.152]. However, the main effect of awareness state was found to be significant [F(1, 15) = 20.480, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.563]. The main effect of word type was also significant [F(2, 30) = 7.354, p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.329]. The simple effect of word type in the visible condition [F(2, 30) = 4.656, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.237] revealed that negative words (mean RT, 394.169 ms) were faster than the scrambled words (435.437 ms) and neutral words (401.817 ms). The simple effect of word type in the invisible condition [F(2, 30) = 1.494, p = 0.241, ηp2 = 0.091, mean RT: negative word = 689.858 ms, neutral word = 691.949 ms, scrambled word = 695.799 ms] was not significant.

P2

A two-way ANOVA with awareness states (visible, invisible) and word types (negative, neutral, and scrambled) showed that the interaction of awareness states and word types was significant [F(2, 30) = 10.969, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.422]. The main effect of awareness states was also significant [F(1, 15) = 39.191, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.723], and so was the main effect of word type [F(2, 30) = 5.646, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.273]. Subsequent analyses revealed that P2 showed no difference between the negative words and the neutral words in the visible condition (p = 0.586; see Figures 2A,3). However, the negative words (1.202 ± 0.748 μV) elicited more positive P2 than the neutral words in the invisible condition (0.640 ± 0.715 μV, p < 0.01). The neutral words (5.164 ± 0.473 μV) elicited a larger P2 than the scrambled words (3.336 ± 0.583 μV) in the visible condition (p < 0.01; see Figures 2B,3). In the invisible condition, the P2 amplitudes of the scrambled words (1.189 ± 0.665 μV) were larger than those of the neutral words (0.640 ± 0.715 μV, p < 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Group Average differences in wave amplitudes from 220 to 280 ms. (A) Emotional effect: the P2 amplitudes of negative words (red line) minus the amplitudes of neutral words (black dotted line); the difference wave is represented as a thin black line. (B) Semantic effect: the P2 amplitudes of neutral words (black dotted line) minus the amplitudes of scrambled words (blue line); the difference wave also is again represented as a thin black line. (C) The difference wave (thin black line) between the emotional effect (red line) and the semantic effect (blue line).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A topographical representation of the difference waves shown in Figure 2. The asterisks indicate statistical significance (n.s., not significant; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

A 2 × 2 ANOVA with awareness states (visible and invisible) and effects (semantic and emotional) indicated that the interaction between semantic and emotional effects was also significant [F(1, 15) = 22.377, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.599]. The main effect of awareness states was significant [F(1, 15) = 7.037, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.319]. However, the main effect of all these effects was not significant [F(1, 15) = 1.288, p = 0.274, ηp2 = 0.079]. In the visible condition, the semantic effect (1.827 ± 0.561 μV) was larger than the emotional effect (−0.125 ± 0.225 μV, p = 0.006). In the invisible condition, the emotional effect (0.562 ± 0.186 μV) was larger than the semantic effect (−0.549 ± 0.197 μV, p = 0.006; see Figures 2C,3).

Discussion

In this research, we investigated the P2 responses to the emotional, neutral, and scrambled words in the visible and invisible conditions. Negative words elicited larger P2 amplitudes than neutral words only in the invisible condition. Neutral words elicited larger P2 amplitudes than scrambled words in the visible condition, but smaller P2 amplitudes than scrambled words in the invisible condition. The results showed that emotional processing of words can occur unconsciously, while semantic processing of words can take place both consciously and unconsciously.

The emotional effect was significant in the invisible condition, whereas this effect was not significant in the visible condition. In the visible condition, consistent with the present study, Carretié et al. (2008) found that there was no significant difference between the neutral words and negative words. In our study, participants observed the negative, neutral, and scrambled words. They found the scrambled words much easier to distinguish than the intact words. This strong categorization effect may have suppressed the emotional effect. In contrast, the emotional effect of the P2 component occurred in the invisible condition. This result was similar to previous findings. For example, Bernat et al. (2001) found that in the supraliminal condition, no significant differences in the P2 component were exhibited between unpleasant and pleasant words, while in the subliminal condition, unpleasant words evoked larger P2 amplitudes than pleasant words. Moreover, Yun et al. (2011) also found that the masked threat words elicited a larger P250 amplitude than the neutral words in PTSD (posttraumatic stress disorder) patients. Thus, the P2 component was sensitive to emotional processing in the unconscious (invisible) condition, but not in the visible condition. This result suggested that P2 might play an important role in unconscious emotional word processing.

We found a difference in the P2 component (neutral words > scrambled words) in the visible condition. This result was similar to that of previous studies (Liu et al., 2009; Chung et al., 2010). Interestingly, when the participants were unaware of the stimuli, a P2 reversal (scrambled words > neutral words) was exhibited. Moreover, the semantic effect in the invisible condition was weaker than that in the visible condition. One possibility is that a reverse mode was operating between the different types of attentional processes adopted between the visible and the invisible condition. The participants adopted a “bottom-up” attentional style in the invisible condition, yet a “top-down” style in the visible condition. Another explanation is that the observed early semantic processing effect of the P250 component was related to the inferior frontal gyrus (Liu et al., 2013), which was activated by syntactic violations in the absence of conscious awareness (Batterink and Neville, 2013; Axelrod et al., 2014). Thus, the P2 reversal effect might imply that some early semantic processing was still operating in the invisible condition, but that this effect was much weaker than that in the visible condition. Further, studies are needed to verify our interpretation of the P2 reversal effect. In short, semantic processing appears to be suppressed in the invisible condition, but not in the visible condition.

Why was the emotional effect greater than the semantic effect in the invisible condition, yet the semantic effect was stronger than the emotional effect in the visible condition? Owing to the biological value of emotional stimuli, emotional processing holds a prioritized place (Yokoyama et al., 2013). Previous studies have provided evidence of two distinct networks of emotional processing: the cortical pathway and the subcortical pathway (Whalen et al., 1998; Morris et al., 1999; Damasio et al., 2000; Adolphs, 2004; Williams et al., 2006). For example, Naccache et al. (2005) recorded the amygdala-mediated reactions of three epilepsy patients when they observed threat words and neutral words in masked and unmasked paradigms by intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG). The brain regions relating to emotional memory were also activated, such as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC, Cato et al., 2004) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC, Kuchinke et al., 2006). The cortical pathway of emotional processing was found to be reduced or absent in CFS, while the subcortical pathway—which included the amygdala and pulvinar—was still in operation. This is because the prefrontal cortex is related to “top-down” parieto-frontal networks, which bind conscious sensory processing (Dehaene et al., 2006). Although some researches hold the notion that cortical semantic processing still occurs in the absence of awareness (Dehaene et al., 2006; Axelrod et al., 2014), in our study the degree of semantic processing was definitely impaired. Therefore, the emotional effect was greater than the semantic effect in the invisible condition, which suggested that emotional processing is less dependent on conscious awareness than on semantic processing.

We also found the main effect of the awareness state to be significant: the amplitudes of P2 in the visible condition were larger than those in the invisible condition. This result reflected the fact that the brain reactions to the word stimuli in the visible condition were stronger than those in the invisible condition. The “unconscious binding” hypothesis suggests that, in the unconscious condition, registered and attentively grouped information can be integrated (Lin and He, 2009). However, compared with conscious binding (Crick and Koch, 1990), “unconscious binding” is fragile and weak. Thus, we found a larger P2 component in the visible condition than in the invisible condition. Moreover, the main effect of the word type was significant, which reflected that the emotional and semantic processing might exist in the visible and invisible conditions.

The behavioral data showed no differences between the RTs of negative words and those of neutral words in the invisible condition; negative words were faster than neutral words and scrambled words in the visible condition. To date, ample studies have been conducted looking at the differences in the processing of emotional vs. neutral words on the behavioral level (see Jończyk, 2016). Our findings support previous studies reporting facilitative processing of negative compared to neutral words (Kousta et al., 2009; Vinson et al., 2014; Yap and Seow, 2014). However, we did not find the behavioral differences between negative words and neutral words in the invisible condition. It was probably because the behavioral data was not as sensitive as the electrophysiological data. Moreover, in our experiment the time needed (500 ms) to break the CFS was not sufficient to elicit a behavioral reaction; Yang and Yeh (2011) found that the mean RT needed to break suppression was more than 1,600 ms. Therefore, the behavioral data in our study might be insufficient to elucidate the unconscious emotional word processing.

A limitation of our research might be the participants' anticipation or some response strategies in our task. It appears to be better if the visible and invisible conditions are mixed within one block (see Yang et al., 2014). Therefore, we suggest that future studies should further investigate this line of thought.

In conclusion, our findings provide evidence that emotional words can be processed even when the stimuli are rendered invisible, which may contribute to the ongoing debate (Zimba and Blake, 1983; Dehaene et al., 2006; Jiang et al., 2007; Hesselmann et al., 2015; Ortells et al., 2016). Furthermore, emotional information is more sensitive to unconscious processing than semantic information; a semantic effect weakly occurs only in the absence of awareness. Besides, P2 plays an important role in unconscious emotional word processing. These findings appear to suggest that emotional processing remains automatic and prioritized compared with semantic processing in the unconscious condition. Since the issue is rather complex (context- and task-dependent), the debate on emotional vs. cognitive primacy remains ongoing (see Lai et al., 2012), and further studies are needed to clarify this issue.

Author Contributions

HD, QL, YL, and HL designed performed this study. HD performed the study. HD and ZZ analyzed the data. YL, HD, and WZ wrote the paper. YL and HD contributed equally to this work.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC31470997, NSFC31571153, and NSFC81171289).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00592/full#supplementary-material

References

Adolphs, R. (2004). Emotional vision. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1167–1168. doi: 10.1038/nn1104-1167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Kéri, S., Kovács, G., Liszli, P., Janka, Z., and Benedek, G. (2001). Event-related potentials from a visual categorization task. Brain Res. Protoc. 7, 131–136. doi: 10.1016/S1385-299X(01)00055-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelrod, V., Bar, M., Rees, G., and Yovel, G. (2014). Neural correlates of subliminal language processing. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2160–2169. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batterink, L., and Neville, H. J. (2013). The human brain processes syntax in the absence of conscious awareness. J. Neurosci. 33, 8528–8533. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0618-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begleiter, H., and Platz, A. (1969). Cortical evoked potentials to semantic stimuli. Psychophysiology 6, 91–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1969.tb02887.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., and Garozzo, R. (1979). “Visual evoked potentials and affective ratings of semantic stimuli,” in Evoked Brain Potentials and Behavior, ed H. Begleiter (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 127–143.

Google Scholar

Bernat, E., Bunce, S., and Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 11–34. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00133-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carretié, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., De La Gándara, B. S., Igoa, J. M., et al. (2008). Modulation of ongoing cognitive processes by emotionally intense words. Psychophysiology 45, 188–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00617.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cato, M. A., Crosson, B., Gökçay, D., Soltysik, D., Wierenga, C., Gopinath, K., et al. (2004). Processing words with emotional connotation: an fMRI study of time course and laterality in rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 167–177. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, T. L., Lee, S. H., Hung, S. M., and Chen, H. C. (2012). The role of inferior frontal gyrus in processing Chinese classifiers. Neuropsychologia 50, 1408–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, K. K., Tong, X., Liu, P. D., McBride-Chang, C., and Meng, X. (2010). The processing of morphological structure information in Chinese coordinative compounds: an event-related potential study. Brain Res. 1352, 157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M. P., Herrington, J., Miller, G. A., et al. (2003). Paying attention to emotion. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 81–96. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.2.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, F., and Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Semin. Neurosci. 2, 263–275.

Google Scholar

Crowley, K. E., and Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 732–744. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., and Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, S., and Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: a neurobiological analysis. Cogn. Emot. 21, 1184–1211. doi: 10.1080/02699930701437931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eo, K., Cha, O., Chong, S. C., and Kang, M. S. (2016). Less is more: semantic information survives interocular suppression when attention is diverted. J. Neurosci. 36, 5489–5497. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3018-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., and Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130:681. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garavan, H., Pendergrass, J. C., Ross, T. J., Stein, E. A., and Risinger, R. C. (2001). Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Neuroreport 12, 2779–2783. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-García, I., Kube, J., Gaebler, M., Horstmann, A., Villringer, A., and Neumann, J. (2016). Neural processing of negative emotional stimuli and the influence of age, sex and task-related characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 773–793. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Larrea, L., Lukaszewicz, A. C., and Mauguiére, F. (1992). Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia 30, 723–741. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90042-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graux, J., Gomot, M., Roux, S., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., and Bruneau, N. (2014). Is my voice just a familiar voice? An electrophysiological study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 101–105. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, S., and Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. Neuroreport 13, 15–19. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, C., Ethofer, T., Anders, S., Junghofer, M., Wildgruber, D., Grodd, W., et al. (2009). Amygdala activation during reading of emotional adjectives—an advantage for pleasant content. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 35–49. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, C., Kissler, J., Junghöfer, M., Peyk, P., and Rockstroh, B. (2006). Processing of emotional adjectives: evidence from startle EMG and ERPs. Psychophysiology 43, 197–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00385.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesselmann, G., Darcy, N., Sterzer, P., and Knops, A. (2015). Exploring the boundary conditions of unconscious numerical priming effects with continuous flash suppression. Conscious. Cogn. 31, 60–72. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyman, T., and Moors, P. (2014). Frequent words do not break continuous flash suppression differently from infrequent or nonexistent words: implications for semantic processing of words in the absence of awareness. PLoS ONE 9:e104719. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H., Strube, M., Roesch-Ely, D., and Weisbrod, M. (2002). Automatic vs. controlled processes in semantic priming—differentiation by event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 197–218. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00202-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, M., Mothes-Lasch, M., Miltner, W. H., and Straube, T. (2015). Brain activation to briefly presented emotional words: effects of stimulus awareness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 655–665. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., and Donohue, B. C. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 8939–8944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.15.8939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., Beattie, B. A., et al. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 10456–10459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.18.10456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Costello, P., and He, S. (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychol. Sci. 18, 349–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jończyk, R. (2016). Affect-Language Interactions in Native and Non-Native English Speakers. Poznań: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Kang, M. S., Blake, R., and Woodman, G. F. (2011). Semantic analysis does not occur in the absence of awareness induced by interocular suppression. J. Neurosci. 31, 13535–13545. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1691-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., and Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Res. 1148, 138–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2006). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J. Neurosci. 26, 2564–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, C. Y., and Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M. S., Kim, J. J., and Kwon, J. S. (2001). The effect of immediate and delayed word repetition on event-related potential in a continuous recognition task. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 11, 387–396. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00011-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kousta, S.-T., Vinson, D. P., and Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition 112, 473–481. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuchinke, L., Jacobs, A. M., Grubich, C., Võ, M. L., Conrad, M., and Herrmann, M. (2005). Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 28, 1022–1032. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuchinke, L., Jacobs, A. M., Võ, M. L. H., Conrad, M., Grubich, C., and Herrmann, M. (2006). Modulation of prefrontal cortex activation by emotional words in recognition memory. Neuroreport 17, 1037–1041. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000221838.27879.fe

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, V. T., Hagoort, P., and Casasanto, D. (2012). Affective primacy vs. cognitive primacy: dissolving the debate. Front. Psychol. 3:243. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Z., and He, S. (2009). Seeing the invisible: the scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry. Prog. Neurobiol. 87, 195–211. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, B., Wang, Z., and Jin, Z. (2009). The integration processing of the visual and auditory information in videos of real-world events: an ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 461, 7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.05.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Tao, R., Wang, W., You, W., Peng, D., and Booth, J. R. (2013). Chinese dyslexics show neural differences in morphological processing. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Sheng, F., Woodcock, K. A., and Han, S. (2013). Oxytocin effects on neural correlates of self-referential processing. Biol. Psychol. 94, 380–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Y. J., and Wang, Y. N. (2004). Chinese Affective Words System (CAWS). Beijing: Institute of Psychology, CAS.

Mendola, J. D., and Conner, I. P. (2007). Eye dominance predicts fMRI signals in human retinotopic cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 414, 30–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mestres-Missé, A., Camara, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Rotte, M., and Münte, T. F. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of meaning acquisition from context. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 2153–2166. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 1680–1685. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naccache, L., Gaillard, R., Adam, C., Hasboun, D., Clémenceau, S., Baulac, M., et al. (2005). A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 7713–7717. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500542102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakic, M., Smith, B. W., Busis, S., Vythilingam, M., and Blair, R. J. R. (2006). The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: the role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage 31, 1752–1761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortells, J. J., Kiefer, M., Castillo, A., Megías, M., and Morillas, A. (2016). The semantic origin of unconscious priming: behavioral and event-related potential evidence during category congruency priming from strongly and weakly related masked words. Cognition 146, 143–157. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. D. C., and Grafton, S. T. (2007). The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1218–1230. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortigue, S., Michel, C. M., Murray, M. M., Mohr, C., Carbonnel, S., and Landis, T. (2004). Electrical neuroimaging reveals early generator modulation to emotional words. Neuroimage 21, 1242–1251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, J., Russell, J. A., Gerber, A., Gorman, D., Colibazzi, T., Yu, S., et al. (2009). The neurophysiological bases of emotion: an fMRI study of the affective circumplex using emotion-denoting words. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 883–895. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, G., Jacob, Y., Gonen, T., Winetraub, Y., Flash, T., Soreq, E., et al. (2014). Cry for her or cry with her: context-dependent dissociation of two modes of cinematic empathy reflected in network cohesion dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 30–38. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, G., Winetraub, Y., Jacob, Y., Kinreich, S., Maron-Katz, A., Shaham, G., et al. (2012). Portraying emotions at their unfolding: a multilayered approach for probing dynamics of neural networks. Neuroimage 60, 1448–1461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schapkin, S. A., Gusev, A. N., and Kuhl, J. (2000). Categorization of unilaterally presented emotional words: an ERP analysis. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 60, 17–28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular gyrus multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 19, 43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., and Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sklar, A. Y., Levy, N., Goldstein, A., Mandel, R., Maril, A., and Hassin, R. R. (2012). Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 19614–19619. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211645109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabert, M. H., Borod, J. C., Tang, C. Y., Lange, G., Wei, T. C., Johnson, R., et al. (2001). Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 39, 556–573. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00157-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1096–1101. doi: 10.1038/nn1500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinson, D., Ponari, M., and Vigliocco, G. (2014). How does emotional content affect lexical pro- cessing? Cogn. Emot. 28, 737–746. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.851068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., and Bastiaansen, M. (2014). Oscillatory brain dynamics associated with the automatic processing of emotion in words. Brain Lang. 137, 120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., and Yuan, J. (2008). N400 lexicality effect in highly blurred Chinese words: evidence for automatic processing. Neuroreport 19, 173–178. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f3e3f2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., and Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. J. Neurosci. 18, 411–418.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Bryant, R. A., Meares, R. A., Peduto, A. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala–prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 652–661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J., and Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology 28, 260–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb02192.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, E., Brascamp, J., Kang, M. S., and Blake, R. (2014). On the use of continuous flash suppression for the study of visual processing outside of awareness. Front. Psychol. 5:724. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y. H., and Yeh, S. L. (2011). Accessing the meaning of invisible words. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yap, M. J., and Seow, C. S. (2014). The influence of emotion on lexical processing: insights from RT distributional analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 526–533. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0525-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, S., He, W., Zhan, L., Qi, Z., Zhu, C., Luo, W., et al. (2015). Emotional noun processing: an ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. PLoS ONE 10:e0118924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, T., Noguchi, Y., and Kita, S. (2013). Unconscious processing of direct gaze: evidence from an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 51, 1161–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yun, X., Li, W., Qiu, J., Jou, J., Wei, D., Tu, S., et al. (2011). Neural mechanisms of subliminal priming for traumatic episodic memory: an ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 498, 10–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.04.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimba, L. D., and Blake, R. (1983). Binocular rivalry and semantic processing: out of sight, out of mind. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 9:807. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.9.5.807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional word, continuous flash suppression, P2, unconscious, semantic processing

Citation: Lei Y, Dou H, Liu Q, Zhang W, Zhang Z and Li H (2017) Automatic Processing of Emotional Words in the Absence of Awareness: The Critical Role of P2. Front. Psychol. 8:592. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00592

Received: 08 November 2016; Accepted: 30 March 2017;
Published: 20 April 2017.

Edited by:

Michael Noll-Hussong, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Paula Goolkasian, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, USA
Rafał Jończyk, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań, Poland
Felix R. Dreyer, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany

Copyright © 2017 Lei, Dou, Liu, Zhang, Zhang and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wenhai Zhang, endoMjAxMjAxMDZAMTYzLmNvbQ==
Hong Li, bGlob25nd3JtQHZpcC5zaW5hLmNvbQ==

Co-first authors.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.