- 1EA 4430, CLIPSYD, University Paris Nanterre, Nanterre, France
- 2EA 7334 (REMES), Paris Sorbonne Cité, Patient-Centered Outcomes Research, University Paris Diderot, Paris, France
- 3Unité de Recherche Clinique en Economie de la Santé, Hôpital Hôtel-Dieu, URC ECO, Paris, France
- 4INSERM U894 CPN, LPMP, SHU, Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne, Paris, France
- 5EA2931 CERSM, UFR-STAPS, University Paris Nanterre, Nanterre, France
- 6INSERM U894 CPN, CMME Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne, Paris, France
- 7CESP, INSERM, University Paris-Sud, UVSQ, University Paris-Saclay, Villejuif, France
- 8Psychiatry Unit, Institut Mutualiste Montsouris, Paris, France
The concept of Emotional Eating (EE) is increasingly considered to be implicated in overeating and obesity, and in different subtypes of eating disorders. Among the self-report questionnaires assessing EE, the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ) includes recent advances in this area: it evaluates a broad range of emotions and situations both positive and negative, and the way they modulate food intake (decrease, stability, or increase). The main objective of our study was to further investigate the psychometric properties of the French version of the EMAQ in a large sample of students. Participants completed the EMAQ (n = 679), the DEBQ (Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire) (n = 75) and the CIDI-eating disorders screening (Composite International Diagnostic Interview) (n = 604). Factorial structure, reliability, and validity of the EMAQ were tested. Factorial analyses supported a two-factor (Positive and Negative) structure. The internal consistency indices were satisfactory and results suggest good test–retest reliability for the scale. Convergent and discriminant validity were confirmed from the significant correlations observed between the EMAQ scores and the DEBQ-EE subscale scores. Regarding associations with weight, whereas EMAQ negative scores were positively correlated with BMI, EMAQ positive scores were negatively correlated with BMI. Finally, EMAQ scores differed significantly depending on gender and risk for bulimia nervosa. This study supports the validity and the reliability of the EMAQ, which appears to be a promising instrument to better understand individual differences that could modulate food intake.
Introduction
Food consumption is considered an important mood regulating behavior (Heatherton and Baumeister, 1991; Polivy and Herman, 1993; Greeno and Wing, 1994; Macht, 1999, 2008). In this setting, Emotional Eating (EE) is generally conceived as eating in response to negative emotions rather than to feelings of hunger or satiety (Arnow et al., 1995; Lindeman and Stark, 2001). Indeed, some individuals appear to be more susceptible to unhealthy shifts in food choices (e.g., abnormal consumption of sweet, salty, high-fat, and energy-dense foods) in order to cope with negative emotions (Oliver et al., 2000; Nguyen-Michel et al., 2007; Macht, 2008; van Strien et al., 2012). Past research has shown that this eating behavior could place the individual at risk for overweight and obesity (Macht, 2008; Gibson, 2012; Singh, 2014). Moreover, both experimental and large scale epidemiological studies show that healthy and normal-weight persons use food to regulate negative emotions (Macht and Simons, 2000; Macht et al., 2005; Spoor et al., 2007; Camilleri et al., 2014; Finch and Tomiyama, 2015)1, which suggests that EE could be considered as a common phenomenon occurring in the general population. For instance, in a recent American survey among adults, 38% reported overeating unhealthy foods in the past month because of stress, and half of them reported engaging in these behaviors weekly or more (American Psychological Association, 2015). In a French national survey, 44.4% of respondents reported eating more under stress (Beck et al., 2007). Overeating is not the only stress-induced eating behavior. While few people report not changing their eating behaviors during stressful periods, there seem to be as many people who eat more (around 30–50% would present this a-typical stress response) as individuals who eat less (40–70% would present this typical distress response) (see Gibson, 2012). This disparity in either overeating or under-eating in the general population has been observed in clinical samples, and recent studies suggest that, besides obesity, EE could be involved in the entire spectrum of eating disorders: not only in binge eating episodes as in bulimia nervosa (BN) or binge eating disorder, but also in binge-purging and restrictive anorexia nervosa (Ricca et al., 2009, 2012). There is also growing evidence showing that positive emotions are considered to be an underestimated risk factor for food intake and overeating (Bongers et al., 2013a,b, 2016; Evers et al., 2013), but little is known about their effects on eating behaviors compared to those documented for negative emotions (Macht, 2008). However, recent studies suggest that eating in response to negative and to positive emotions could refer to different constructs (van Strien et al., 2013) and that only the desire to eat in response to negative emotions would be an ‘obese’ eating style (van Strien et al., 2016).
Several self-report questionnaires have been developed to assess EE. The first and most commonly used questionnaires are the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) (Stunkard and Messick, 1985), the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) (van Strien et al., 1986) and the Emotional Eating Scale (EES) (Arnow et al., 1995)2. The TFEQ (Disinhibition subscale) and the TFEQ Revised 18-items version (EE subscale) (Karlsson et al., 2000), comprise only three items assessing increased food intake in response to negative emotions. The second revised version (TFEQ-R21) introduced three supplementary items in the EE subscale (Cappelleri et al., 2009). The DEBQ (van Strien et al., 1986) is a multidimensional instrument (see Materials and Methods) that includes a 13-items EE subscale, with questions assessing increased food intake in response to negative emotions. The EES (Arnow et al., 1995) evaluates the desire or urge to eat in response to 25 specific negative emotional states. The EES has recently been extended to positive emotions and the instructions have been modified to also evaluate decreased intake (EES-II, Kenardy et al., 2003), but this version has not been validated as such (no factorial, reliability, or validity analysis).
In summary, these instruments are not fully suited to measuring the complexity of the effects of these emotionally driven eating behaviors, i.e., the fact that food intake can either increase or decrease in response to negative as well as positive emotions.
Geliebter and Aversa (2003) developed the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ), a self-report questionnaire based on ratings of the tendency to eat less, equally or more in response to both positive and negative primary emotions. In comparison with the EES-II, the EMAQ also explores commonly encountered situations. Geliebter and Aversa (2003) showed among young adults the internal consistency and the reliability of the scale, as well as its sensitivity to BMI categories and gender. Later, Nolan et al. (2010) demonstrated in a convenience sample of young adults (n = 232 university students or employees; 73.71% of women) the construct validity of the EMAQ (convergent and discriminant with the DEBQ-EE subscale) and replicated Geliebter and Aversa (2003) findings on BMI and gender. However, neither studies tested the proposed four-factor structure of the scale (based on the presence of four subscores: positive emotions, negative emotions, positive situations, negative situations) and evidence of the EMAQ clinical sensitivity is limited to the demonstration that overweight individuals report higher scores than normal-weight and underweight individuals.
Hence, although the EMAQ has the potential advantage to take account of different factors that are now known to modulate food intake and so to be a useful screening tool for the assessment of emotionally driven, non-homeostasic eating behaviors, it needs to be further validated.
The aim of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of the French version of the EMAQ (F-EMAQ) in a large sample of students, as this population is considered at risk for developing EE. The students’ financial constraints can impact the healthiness of their diet (Gibson, 2012). Several studies showed that students’ weight (Anderson et al., 2003) and perceived stress (Tavolacci et al., 2013) increase, while at the same time physical activities decrease (Boujut and Koleck, 2009; Kern et al., 2013). Exam periods have been associated with an increased tendency to eat, with higher energy intake and a less healthy diet (Epel et al., 2004; Macht et al., 2005; Barker et al., 2015).
The study was designed to examine the factor structure, the internal consistency and the test–retest reliability of the F-EMAQ. Like in the Nolan et al. (2010) study, its convergent and discriminant validity were tested with the DEBQ-EE. Furthermore, we explored whether EMAQ scores discriminate individuals with versus without a risk for BN.
Materials and Methods
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Department of Psychological and Educational Sciences UFR-SPSE, University of Paris Nanterre ethics committee. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The first page of the questionnaire booklet included the information and consent form. The participation was voluntary and anonymous. The data were collected during students’ lectures or tutorials in restricted groups.
Participants
Two samples were used (Table 1): Sample 1 for the analyses of the EMAQ factorial structure and its associations with the risk for being diagnosed with disordered eating behaviors; Sample 2 for the analyses of the scale test–retest reliability and convergent and discriminant validity.
Sample 1 was derived from a survey on students’ physical and mental health (n = 750 students from the University of Paris Nanterre). Inclusion criteria for the present analyses were: 18 years old or more, with the EMAQ and the questionnaire assessing the risk of eating disorders fully completed. Sample 2 comprised 75 students in Psychology (Ecole des Psychologues Praticiens). These participants completed the EMAQ and DEBQ questionnaires. In addition, they completed the EMAQ a second time, at an interval of 4–6 weeks.
Measures
Socio-demographic information was collected (age, gender, academic discipline, level of education) as well as self-reported height and weight.
Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ) (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010)
The EMAQ contains 22 items assessing variations of food intake in response to emotional states and situations: nine items evaluate negative emotions (e.g., “When you are sad”), five evaluate positive emotions (e.g., “When you are happy”), five refer to negative situations (e.g., “When under pressure”) and three to positive situations (e.g., “When engaged in an enjoyable hobby”). On a nine point Likert-type scale, for each item respondents were asked to rate whether they ate less (from 1 to 4), the same (5), or more (from 6 to 9) compared to usual. Two optional responses (not included in the scoring) are also possible: Not applicable or Don’t know. A score is calculated for positive emotions (EMAQ-PE), negative emotions (EMAQ-NE), positive situations (EMAQ-PS) and negative situations (EMAQ-NS). A positive total score (EMAQ-P) is obtained by averaging the EMAQ-PE and EMAQ-PS scores. A negative total score (EMAQ-N) is obtained by averaging the EMAQ-NE and EMAQ-NS scores. In the two previous validation studies (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010), Cronbach’s alphas were respectively 0.78 and 0.79 for EMAQ-NE, 0.75 and 0.87 for EMAQ-PE, 0.65 and 0.75 for EMAQ-NS, 0.57 and 0.66 for EMAQ-PS. Cronbach’s alphas for the total Positive and Negative scores were not reported.
The French adaptation of the EMAQ was conducted following the standard procedure. The authors of the original version of the EMAQ gave their agreement (to SB) for the adaptation into French of this instrument. The EMAQ was translated into French independently by four senior researchers (SB, YM, LK, and LR). These different translations were then compared and for the few items for which the exact wording differed (one positive situation and one negative situation), the final wording was based on consensus. This version was then back-translated by an English native speaker with expertise in psychology (postdoctoral fellow at CESP-INSERM). This version was compared to the original EMAQ. Only one item was slightly different (negative situation: ‘After a heated argument’): since in French this can refer to either a quarrel or a discussion, these two slightly different meanings of the word ‘argument’ were kept in the final French version (available on request from the authors).
Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) (van Strien et al., 1986; French version: Lluch et al., 1996)
The DEBQ contains three subscales: Restrained Eating (DEBQ-R), External Eating (DEBQ-X) and Emotional Eating (DEBQ-EE). The DEBQ-EE has 13 items assessing eating more in response to negative emotions on a five-point Likert scale (from “never” to “very often”). Here, only DEBQ-EE scores were used (Cronbach alpha = 0.89).
Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (World Health Organization, 1990)
This is a diagnostic structured instrument designed to estimate the presence of mental disorders. Five “diagnostic entry” questions assess, with a four-point self-rating scale, the risk for BN and/or anorexia nervosa according to three different temporal criteria: present risk, risk in the past year, lifetime risk. Here, we used the scoring for the lifetime risk for BN.
Statistical Methods
Regarding descriptive statistics, continuous variables were summarized using mean, standard deviation and range, while categorical variables were summarized as counts and proportions. Two-group comparisons were performed using Student’s t-tests, while Pearson’s chi-squared tests were used to analyze cross-classification tables.
The descriptive statistics for the EMAQ items (i.e., response distribution across the nine response categories and ceiling/floor effects) were computed on Sample 1. To assess the factor structure of the EMAQ, Sample 1 was divided into two equal-sized subsamples, stratified by terciles for age and gender. A training sample was used to extract an interpretable factor structure using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with Mplus default ML estimator for continuous outcomes, and a confirmatory (CFA) model for the same factor structure was tested on the validation subsample. The same confirmatory analysis was replicated following inspection of modification indices by including correlated errors. Besides the χ2 of model fit, standard fit indices (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Jackson et al., 2009) were used to assess goodness-of-fit of the CFA model: a comparative fit index (CFI) greater than 0.90; a standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) below 0.10; a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) below 0.08. Based on the factor structure determined during exploratory and confirmatory approaches on the training/validation subsamples, a final CFA model was applied to the full sample.
Internal structure and consistency were evaluated using Pearson correlations and Cronbach’s Alpha respectively. Test–retest stability was assessed using intra-class correlation coefficients. Spearman correlations were used to evaluate monotonic associations of EMAQ scores with DEBQ scores and BMI. Group comparisons for Gender and Diagnosis (at risk or not for lifetime BN) on EMAQ-P and EMAQ-N scores were estimated by analyses of variance and associated effect sizes (partial eta-squared, ηp2). A value of ηp2 around 0.01 is associated with a small effect, around 0.06 with a medium effect, and around 0.14 with a large effect (Cohen, 1988).
Exploratory factor analysis and CFA were carried out using R 2.15.2 (R Core Team, 2014) and Mplus 7 (Muthén and Muthén, 2013) software. The remaining analyses were carried out using SPSS 20.0 (Ibm Corp. Released, 2011). A fixed Type I error rate of 5% was retained for all statistical tests.
Results
Samples Characteristics
The characteristics of the study participants are summarized in Table 2.
Sample 1 included 604 students fulfilling the inclusion criteria. The men presented higher BMI (Mean = 22.3; SD = 2.6) than the women (Mean = 21.4; SD = 3.5, p = 0.002). Regarding academic discipline, 68% of the women were studying Psychology and 76% of the men were STAPS students (physical and sports activities faculty).
Sample 2 included 75 students. This sample differed from Sample 1 in terms of gender (93% women, p < 0.001) and age (Mean = 22.6; SD = 4.3, p = 0.020) but had similar BMI (Mean = 21.9; SD = 3.1, p = 0.757).
Factor Structure (Sample 1)
The analysis of the distribution of responses to each item revealed no evidence of ceiling or floor effect: the highest proportion of responses in either of the two extreme response categories did not exceed 45%. The proportion of Not Applicable and Don’t know responses per item was less than 1.6%, except for item 13 ‘Lonely’ (1.8%).
Two to four factor solutions were assessed using EFA with PROMAX rotation on the training subsample (n = 302). A two-factor structure unambiguously reflecting two major dimensions was found: positive (EMAQ-P, eight items) and negative (EMAQ-N, 14 items) valence. Only one item was found to load weakly on its hypothetical factor (‘Bored,’ λ = 0.176) (see Table 3). There was no evidence of item cross-loadings, and the RMSEA for this model was estimated at 0.094 (90% CI[0.086; 0.101]).
TABLE 3. Two to four factor solutions of the Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA on the training subsample, n = 302).
The CFA model for this bifactorial structure yielded model fit indices in the acceptable range (RMSEA = 0.089, 90% CI[0.082; 0.096]; CFI = 0.803; SRMR = 0.080) on the validation subsample (n = 302). Following analysis of modification indices, a second CFA model was tested on this validation sample by allowing items 16 and 17 to have correlated errors, but without much improvement in overall goodness-of-fit (RMSEA = 0.086, 90% CI[0.079; 0.093]; CFI = 0.818; SRMR = 0.079).
Based on the full sample (n = 604; see Table 4 for the factor loadings), the final goodness-of-fit indices were: χ2(208; n = 604) = 686.98, p < 0.000; RMSEA = 0.079, 90% CI[0.074; 0.084]; CFI = 0.832; SRMR = 0.067.
TABLE 4. Two-factor solution and item factor loadings of the French-EMAQ (Confirmatory Factor Analysis – CFA on the full sample, n = 604).
For the remaining analyses, besides EMAQ-P and EMAQ-N scores, we report but do not discuss the results for the EMAQ subscales in order to compare our results with those of the two previous validation studies (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010).
Internal Structure and Consistency (Sample 1, Table 5)
TABLE 5. Mean scores, reliability (internal consistency and test–retest stability) and Spearman’s correlation with BMI and DEBQ for each score of the EMAQ.
Pearson correlation between the EMAQ total positive score (EMAQ-P) and the EMAQ total negative score (EMAQ-N) was negative and significant (r = -0.130; p = 0.002). EMAQ Emotions and Situations subscale scores were positively significantly correlated (EMAQ-NE/EMAQ-NS: r = 0.685, p < 0.001; EMAQ-PE/EMAQ-PS: r = 0.482, p < 0.001).
Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the EMAQ-P and EMAQ-N scores were 0.750 and 0.850 respectively.
Test–Retest Reliability (Sample 2, Table 5)
The intra-class correlation coefficients for EMAQ-P and EMAQ-N were respectively 0.736 and 0.761.
Convergent and Discriminant Validity (Sample 2, Table 5)
The EMAQ Negative scores and the DEBQ-EE were strongly positively correlated (EMAQ-N: ρ = 0.761, p < 0.001). Conversely, the EMAQ positive scores and the DEBQ-EE were moderately negatively correlated (EMAQ-P: ρ = -.317, p = 0.006).
Associations with BMI (Sample 1, Table 5)
The EMAQ negative scores were modestly positively correlated with BMI (EMAQ-N: ρ = 0.180, p < 0.001), whereas the EMAQ positive scores were modestly negatively correlated with BMI (EMAQ-P: ρ = -0.143, p < 0.001).
Between-Group Comparisons (Sample 1, Table 6)
TABLE 6. Emotional Appetite Questionnaire total and subscale scores according to gender and diagnosis, and post hoc t-test for EMAQ total scores (Sample 1, n = 604).
Seventy-six participants (54 women) presented a risk for BN. BMI differed significantly between men and women and between students at risk versus not at risk for BN (respectively Mean = 22.8; SD = 4.6 and Mean = 21.6; SD = 2.9; p = 0.003).
The analyses (BMI-adjusted) indicated a main effect of Gender [F(1,583) = 13.89; p < 0.001, = 0.023], of [Positive versus Negative] Valence [F(1,583) = 52.14; p < 0.000, = 0.082] and a significant Gender × Valence interaction [F(1,583) = 3.71; p = 0.050, = 0.006]. Post hoc t-test showed higher EMAQ-P scores among men, but no differences for the EMAQ-N.
Regarding the risk for BN, analyses (BMI-adjusted) indicated no main effect of Diagnosis [F(1,583) = 0.006; p = 0.972], a main effect of Valence [F(1,583) = 33.91; p < 0.001, = 0.055] and a significant Diagnosis × Valence interaction [F(1,583) = 12.81; p < 0.001, = 0.022]. Post hoc t-test revealed that students with a risk for BN had lower EMAQ-P but higher EMAQ-N scores.
Discussion
The present study describes the French adaptation of the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (F-EMAQ) and its validation among university students.
Exploratory factor analysis and CFA analyses revealed a two-factor structure reflecting two major dimensions in the F-EMAQ: the Positive versus Negative valence of the items, rather than the theorized Emotions versus Situations by valence (i.e., four-factor) structure. Our study is the first to have tested the factorial structure of the EMAQ. The original scoring scheme cannot be challenged based on the results of a single study and additional research is required, including in a clinical population, to determine the appropriateness and added value of using all the EMAQ subscale scores.
With regard to its psychometric properties, the F-EMAQ presents good reliability. The internal consistency indices were satisfactory and the Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients for the EMAQ-Positive and EMAQ-Negative total scores were above 0.70. Results also demonstrate adequate test–retest reliability for the Positive and Negative total scores.
In line with the findings by Nolan et al. (2010), the F-EMAQ showed satisfactory convergent and discriminant validity with the DEBQ-EE. Convergent validity was demonstrated by the positive association between the EMAQ negative scores and the DEBQ-EE scores, which reflect the same construct. In contrast, in favor of the discriminant validity of the scale, we observed negative associations between the EMAQ positive scores and the DEBQ-EE scores. Future studies, not only in larger and more diverse populations, but also with the EES-II, which includes positive emotions, should help to provide further arguments for the convergent and discriminant validity of the EMAQ.
In the present study, the positive association between EMAQ negative scores and BMI suggest that people who reported being prone to eating more in response to negative emotions and situations (emotional eaters) had higher BMI than non-emotional eaters. This is in line with the two previous validation studies (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010). It also adds to the literature incriminating EE in overeating and the obesity epidemic (Gibson, 2012). In our study, despite the small number of the students at risk for a lifetime diagnosis of BN, we found they had higher BMIs than the others and also appeared more prone to EE in response to negative emotions and situations. These results are coherent with the affect regulation model of BN suggesting that patients binge-eat in order to reduce negative affect (Polivy and Herman, 1993). They are also consistent with those of Ricca et al. (2012) who showed that BN patients reported eating more in response to negative emotional states (evaluated with the EES) than healthy controls. Nonetheless, in their study no significant differences were found between patients with anorexia nervosa (either restrictive or binge-purging type) and BN, but all the patients reported higher EES scores than the healthy control group. Yet, using the DEBQ-EE subscale, Vervaet et al. (2004) highlighted a continuum of EE scores along the BMI spectrum, whereby patients with anorexia nervosa restrictive-type had the lowest scores, patients with anorexia nervosa binge-purging type had intermediate scores, and those with BN had the highest scores.
While one would expect that increasing food intake, no matter the reason, leads to weight gain, here we observed negative associations between BMI and EMAQ positive scores: students who reported eating more in response to positive emotions and situations had the lowest BMI. This replicates the findings of the two previous validation studies (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010). Hence, eating in response to positive emotions or situations seems to be not associated with weight gain. Of interest too is the observation that the students at risk for a lifetime diagnosis of BN (who had high BMIs and EMAQ negative scores) also appeared to be less prone to eat in response to positive emotions and situations than the others. Our results are consistent with the raising idea that people who eat in response to negative emotions differ from those who eat in response to positive emotions (Bongers et al., 2013a,b) and add to the recent discussion that they may represent two different constructs (van Strien et al., 2013, 2016). Similar studies, but with the EMAQ, should help to better understand the relationships between different eating disorders, concerns about weight and eating behaviors and modes of modulating food intake in response to unpleasant as well as pleasant experiences.
Besides exercising, which was not measured here, another factor that could influence the links between eating and body weight concerns the type of food consumed in relation to the valence of emotions (pleasure or displeasure) as previous studies demonstrated that negative emotions increase the consumption of junk food and positive emotions increase the consumption of healthy foods (Lyman, 1982; Macht, 1999; Macht et al., 2002). Additional comparative studies of underweight, normal and overweight people, coupling the evaluation of both positive and negative EE with the EMAQ and the type of food consumed [using daily food diaries or ecological sampling assessments (Adriaanse et al., 2011; Haedt-Matt and Keel, 2011)] should help to increase our knowledge on this issue.
As in the two previous studies on the EMAQ (Geliebter and Aversa, 2003; Nolan et al., 2010), men had higher EMAQ positive scores than women. These results support the hypothesis that men may be more likely to eat comfort foods to maintain or enhance positive emotions than women (Dubé et al., 2005). However, in our study, men and women did not differ as regards eating more in response to negative situations. This result contrasts with what is traditionally reported in the literature about EE, which is that EE in response to negative states or situations is a more frequent characteristic of women’s eating behaviors (Gibson, 2012; Camilleri et al., 2014). To correctly interpret the effects of gender, we need first to determine whether the underlying psychometric properties of the EMAQ are invariant (i.e., equivalent) across gender (Gregorich, 2006). The same holds true for age. Thus if measurement invariance across gender/age does not hold, total score differences across gender/age groups are difficult to interpret, as they could result either from measurement differences or from genuine differences in emotionally driven eating behaviors. Accordingly, additional data are being collected to investigate this issue in a larger and more diverse sample.
The current study presents some limitations. First, it was conducted among students, so there are limits to the generalizability of the findings. Moreover, the range of academic disciplines represented was relatively restricted (and included many psychology students) and some factors that could influence EE, such as ethno-culture, religion, socio-economic background, incomes or the level of physical activity were not taken into account (Dubé et al., 2005; Diggins et al., 2015). As regards the biases inherent to self-report questionnaires, further attention should be devoted to how factors that are particularly critical in the area of weight control and eating behaviors, such as the ability for introspection or the social desirability bias, impact responding to the EMAQ.
Conclusion
Today, EE is considered as a real risk factor for eating disorders and obesity. The literature and our findings highlight that, beyond their intensity, the valence of emotions has an influence on eating and potentially on weight. To few instruments assessing emotionally driven eating habits in French are available and validated. The EMAQ appears to be a promising instrument evaluating subjective variations in food intake (eating less, equally, or more) in response to positive and negative emotional states and situations. It could help clinicians to better understand the individual differences that could impact food intake and to tailor specific therapeutic interventions.
Author Contributions
LK, LR, YM, and SB designed and performed the study. LB and SB conducted the literature searches. LB, CL, and SB undertook the statistical analyses. LB, CL, and SB wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors significantly participated in interpreting the results and revising the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
LB is a Ph.D. fellow financed by the University Paris Nanterre (ED 139 – Ph.D. research allocation). We thank Gayatri Kotbagi, Damien Fouques, and Dana Castro for their help with data collection.
Footnotes
- ^ See also Bongers and Jansen (2016) for a discussion on the validity of self-reported emotional eating.
- ^ See Gibson (2012) and Bongers and Jansen (2016) for a full overview of EE questionnaires.
References
Adriaanse, M. A., de Ridder, D. T. D., and Evers, C. (2011). Emotional eating: eating when emotional or emotional about eating? Psychol. Health 26, 23–39. doi: 10.1080/08870440903207627
American Psychological Association (2015). Stress in America: Paying with our Health, 29. Available at: https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2014/stress-report.pdf
Anderson, D. A., Shapiro, J. R., and Lundgren, J. D. (2003). The freshman year of college as a critical period for weight gain: an initial evaluation. Eat. Behav. 4, 363–367. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00030-8
Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., and Agras, W. S. (1995). The emotional eating scale: the development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 18, 79–90. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(199507)18:1<79::AID-EAT2260180109>3.0.CO;2-V
Barker, M. E., Blain, R. J., and Russell, J. M. (2015). The influence of academic examinations on energy and nutrient intake in male university students. Nutr. J. 14, 98. doi: 10.1186/s12937-015-0088-y
Bongers, P., de Graaff, A., and Jansen, A. (2016). “Emotional”does not even start to cover it: generalization of overeating in emotional eaters. Appetite 96, 611–616. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.004
Bongers, P., and Jansen, A. (2016). Emotional eating is not what you think it is and emotional eating scales do not measure what you think they measure. Front. Psychol. 7:1932. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01932
Bongers, P., Jansen, A., Havermans, R., Roefs, A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2013a). Happy eating. The underestimated role of overeating in a positive mood. Appetite 67, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.03.017
Bongers, P., Jansen, A., Houben, K., and Roefs, A. (2013b). Happy eating: the single target implicit association test predicts overeating after positive emotions. Eat. Behav. 14, 348–355. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.06.007
Boujut, E., and Koleck, M. (2009). La santé mentale chez les étudiants: suivi d’une cohorte en première année d’université. Ann. Med. Psychol. 167, 662–668. doi: 10.1016/j.amp.2008.05.020
Camilleri, G. M., Méjean, C., Kesse-Guyot, E., Andreeva, V. A., Bellisle, F., Hercberg, S., et al. (2014). The associations between emotional eating and consumption of energy-dense snack foods are modified by sex and depressive symptomatology. J. Nutr. 144, 1264–1273. doi: 10.3945/jn.114.193177
Cappelleri, J. C., Bushmakin, A. G., Gerber, R. A., Leidy, N. K., Sexton, C. C., Lowe, M. R., et al. (2009). Psychometric analysis of the three-factor eating questionnaire-R21: results from a large diverse sample of obese and non-obese participants. Int. J. Obes. 33, 611–620. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.74
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2 Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Routledge.
Diggins, A., Woods-Giscombe, C., and Waters, S. (2015). The association of perceived stress, contextualized stress, and emotional eating with body mass index in college-aged Black women. Eat. Behav. 19, 188–192. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.09.006
Dubé, L., LeBel, J. L., and Lu, J. (2005). Affect asymmetry and comfort food consumption. Physiol. Behav. 86, 559–567. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.023
Epel, E., Jimenez, S., Brownell, K., Stroud, L., Stoney, C., and Niaura, R. (2004). Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1032, 208–210. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.022
Evers, C., Adriaanse, M., de Ridder, D. T. D., and de Witt Huberts, J. C. (2013). Good mood food. Positive emotion as a neglected trigger for food intake. Appetite 68, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.007
Finch, L. E., and Tomiyama, A. J. (2015). Comfort eating, psychological stress, and depressive symptoms in young adult women. Appetite 95, 239–244. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.017
Geliebter, A., and Aversa, A. (2003). Emotional eating in overweight, normal weight, and underweight individuals. Eat. Behav. 3, 341–347. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(02)00100-9
Gibson, E. L. (2012). The psychobiology of comfort eating: implications for neuropharmacological interventions. Behav. Pharmacol. 23, 442–460. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328357bd4e
Greeno, C. G., and Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Psychol. Bull. 115, 444–464. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444
Gregorich, S. E. (2006). Do self-report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the confirmatory factor analysis framework. Med. Care 44(11 Suppl. 3), S78–S94. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000245454.12228.8f
Haedt-Matt, A. A., and Keel, P. K. (2011). Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychol. Bull. 137:660. doi: 10.1037/a0023660
Heatherton, T. F., and Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychol. Bull. 110, 86–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86
Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118
Jackson, D. L., Gillaspy, J. A., and Purc-Stephenson, R. (2009). Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis: an overview and some recommendations. Psychol. Methods 14, 6–23. doi: 10.1037/a0014694
Karlsson, J., Persson, L. O., Sjöström, L., and Sullivan, M. (2000). Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 24, 1715–1725. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801442
Kenardy, J., Butler, A., Carter, C., and Moor, S. (2003). Eating, mood, and gender in a noneating disorder population. Eat. Behav. 4, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00019-9
Kern, L., Romo, L., Kotbagi, G., and Muller, I. (2013). Résultats de l’étude sur l’activité physique et les comportements alimentaires chez les étudiants de Bordeaux. Paper Presented at the 10ème Assisses Sport, Prévention & Santé, Bordeaux.
Lindeman, M., and Stark, K. (2001). Emotional eating and eating disorder psychopathology. Eat. Disord. 9, 251–259. doi: 10.1080/10640260127552
Lluch, A., Kahn, J. P., Stricker-Krongrad, A., Ziegler, O., Drouin, P., and Méjean, L. (1996). Internal validation of a french version of the dutch eating behaviour questionnaire. Eur. Psychiatry 11, 198–203. doi: 10.1016/0924-9338(96)88391-X
Lyman, B. (1982). The nutritional values and food group characteristics of foods preferred during various emotions. J. Psychol. 112, 121–127. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1982.9923544
Macht, M. (1999). Characteristics of eating in anger, fear, sadness and joy. Appetite 33, 129–139. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0236
Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite 50, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002
Macht, M., Haupt, C., and Ellgring, H. (2005). The perceived function of eating is changed during examination stress: a field study. Eat. Behav. 6, 109–112. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.09.001
Macht, M., Roth, S., and Ellgring, H. (2002). Chocolate eating in healthy men during experimentally induced sadness and joy. Appetite 39, 147–158. doi: 10.1006/appe.2002.0499
Macht, M., and Simons, G. (2000). Emotions and eating in everyday life. Appetite 35, 65–71. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0325
Muthén, L., and Muthén, B. (2013). Mplus User’s Guide, Seventh Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Nguyen-Michel, S. T., Unger, J. B., and Spruijt-Metz, D. (2007). Dietary correlates of emotional eating in adolescence. Appetite 49, 494–499. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.03.005
Nolan, L. J., Halperin, L. B., and Geliebter, A. (2010). Emotional appetite questionnaire. Construct validity and relationship with BMI. Appetite 54, 314–319. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.004
Oliver, G., Wardle, J., and Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom. Med. 62, 853–865. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016
Polivy, J., and Herman, C. P. (1993). “Etiology of binge eating: psychological mechanisms,” in Binge Eating: Nature, Assessment, and Treatment, eds C. G. Fairburn and G. T. Wilson (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 173–205.
R Core Team (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Ricca, V., Castellini, G., Fioravanti, G., Lo Sauro, C., Rotella, F., Ravaldi, C., et al. (2012). Emotional eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 245–251. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.04.062
Ricca, V., Castellini, G., Lo Sauro, C., Ravaldi, C., Lapi, F., Mannucci, E., et al. (2009). Correlations between binge eating and emotional eating in a sample of overweight subjects. Appetite 53, 418–421. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.07.008
Spoor, S. T. P., Bekker, M. H. J., van Strien, T., and van Heck, G. L. (2007). Relations between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite 48, 368–376. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.005
Stunkard, A. J., and Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J. Psychosom. Res. 29, 71–83. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8
Tavolacci, M. P., Ladner, J., Grigioni, S., Richard, L., Villet, H., and Dechelotte, P. (2013). Prevalence and association of perceived stress, substance use and behavioral addictions: a cross-sectional study among university students in France, 2009–2011. BMC Public Health 13:724. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-724
van Strien, T., Cebolla, A., Etchemendy, E., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., Ferrer-García, M., Botella, C., et al. (2013). Emotional eating and food intake after sadness and joy. Appetite 66, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.02.016
van Strien, T., Donker, M. H., and Ouwens, M. A. (2016). Is desire to eat in response to positive emotions an “obese” eating style: Is Kummerspeck for some people a misnomer? Appetite 100, 225–235. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.035
van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E., Bergers, G., and Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 295–315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T
van Strien, T., Herman, C. P., Anschutz, D. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., and de Weerth, C. (2012). Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores. Appetite 58, 277–284. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.10.005
Keywords: emotional eating, scale validity, students, Body Mass Index, bulimia nervosa
Citation: Bourdier L, Lalanne C, Morvan Y, Kern L, Romo L and Berthoz S (2017) Validation and Factor Structure of the French-Language Version of the Emotional Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ). Front. Psychol. 8:442. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00442
Received: 18 January 2017; Accepted: 09 March 2017;
Published: 23 March 2017.
Edited by:
Jens Blechert, University of Salzburg, AustriaReviewed by:
Peggy Bongers, Maastricht University, NetherlandsLaurence James Nolan, Wagner College, USA
Tatjana Vanstrien, Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands
Copyright © 2017 Bourdier, Lalanne, Morvan, Kern, Romo and Berthoz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Léna Bourdier, Ym91cmRpZXIubGVuYUBnbWFpbC5jb20=