- 1Department of Sport, Health and Nutrition, Leeds Trinity University, Leeds, UK
- 2School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
The ability to regulate and subsequently change behavior is influenced by both reflective and implicit processes. Traditional theories have focused on conscious processes by highlighting the beliefs and intentions that influence decision making. However, their success in changing behavior has been modest with a gap between intention and behavior apparent. Dual-process models have been recently applied to health psychology; with numerous models incorporating implicit processes that influence behavior as well as the more common conscious processes. Such implicit processes are theorized to govern behavior non-consciously. The article provides a commentary on motivational and volitional processes and how interventions have combined to attempt an increase in positive health behaviors. Following this, non-conscious processes are discussed in terms of their theoretical underpinning. The article will then highlight how these processes have been measured and will then discuss the different ways that the non-conscious and conscious may interact. The development of interventions manipulating both processes may well prove crucial in successfully altering behavior.
Conscious Processes in Behavior Change
The dominant theories within health psychology aim to predict and explain behavior in order to guide the construction of interventions (Michie et al., 2009). A theoretical understanding is important, particularly as interventions based on theory are more likely to be effective (Michie and Johnston, 2012). With a focus on conscious, reflective processes, these cognitive models introspect attitudes, risk perceptions, and beliefs (Bandura, 1989; Ajzen, 1991; Schwarzer, 2008), typically through self-report. Such processes are theorized to encompass motivation which subsequently leads to the development of a behavioral intention. Thus, intention mediates the effect of motivation on behavior. Although studies have found that expectancy-value models, such as the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB: Ajzen, 1985), predict intention well (McEachan et al., 2011), there is still a large proportion in behavior left unexplained (Armitage and Conner, 2001). Unfortunately, the use of correlational data, which insufficiently validates a theory (Weinstein, 2007), far outweighs the number of studies attempting to change these processes through the development of interventions (Hardeman et al., 2002). This is despite the prevalence of clear methodological guidelines on belief alteration (Ajzen, 1991). Of those that have done so, however, successful motivational manipulations have not resulted in actual change (Sniehotta, 2009), thus suggesting people are generally motivated but struggle to transfer these positive intentions into action (Chatzisarantis and Hagger, 2005). Theoretical and methodological issues have been offered to explain the intention-behavior gap such as intention stability, lack of volitional control, and poor study design (Sheeran and Abraham, 2003; Ajzen, 2015). There are different explanations as to why individual’s fail at completing their previously stated intentions. Such explanations include forgetting, distraction, conflict, procrastination, action initiation, and control (Orbell et al., 1997; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Gollwitzer, 2015). Crucially, such reasons go beyond processes of motivation.
Self-Regulation and Planning
The transfer of motivation into action is dependent heavily on one’s ability to self-regulate. Self-regulation is the ability to overcome obstacles, get back on track, and ward off distractions from tempting stimuli (Baumeister et al., 1994). Thus, possessing such regulatory skills is crucial in fostering the enactment of a previously stated intention. Proceeding the motivational phase of behavior, action implementation occurs during the volitional phase of striving. As such, self-regulation is important during the phase of volition, with its purpose to facilitate positive motivational states. Goal-setting techniques and raising an individual’s level of self-efficacy are ways to regulate conscious processes. For example, a high belief in behavioral achievement will determine how one recovers from set-backs and thus decreases the chances of becoming derailed (Bandura, 1989). One of the widely used strategies to facilitate self-regulation involves planning (Schwarzer, 2008). One type of planning is implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999), which consists of precise descriptions of critical situations in which to perform a target behavior in terms of ‘if/then’ specifications (Gollwitzer, 1999). Such plans create a strong affiliation between a situational cue and the goal-directed behavior, so that this planned behavior may be triggered and initiated automatically when the cue signaling the specified situation is encountered. It is argued that intentions do not directly induce actions, but that they lead to highly specific plans, which in turn prompt action initiation (Gollwitzer, 1999). A similar strategy is action planning which is often erroneously conflated with implementation intentions, but includes more detail in the planning content (Hagger and Luszczynska, 2014). Included within the post-intentional stage of the Health Action Process Approach (Schwarzer, 2008), action planning involves precise specifications of ‘when,’ ‘where,’ and ‘how’ behavior will be performed. In addition to action planning, coping plans can also be used to facilitate self-regulation by identifying potential inhibitors and barriers to behavior (Sniehotta et al., 2005). Although action and coping plans have convergent validity, it is best to treat them as two separate strategies (Schwarzer, 2016). More specifically, as one can only identify potential obstacles when the behavior is first imagined, action plans precede coping plans.
Motivation and Volition
Due to the importance of motivational and volitional processes in changing behavior, researchers have combined the two within interventions. White et al. (2012) successfully increased physical activity within chronic disease sufferers by targeting identified motivational beliefs from the TPB alongside planning strategies. French et al. (2012) found increases in walking behavior in a combined TPB motivation and volition intervention compared to those targeting motivation and volition separately. Studies have also used planning strategies with other theories of motivation to successfully change behavior including the Protection Motivation Theory (e.g., Milne et al., 2002; Zhang and Cooke, 2012), and the decisional balance sheet (e.g., Prestwich et al., 2003).
Although such work has been useful, a gap between planning and behavior has also been identified (Sniehotta, 2009), thus suggesting the presence of other mediators and moderators. This gap has been addressed through the recent integration of preparatory behaviors (Barz et al., 2016) and action control (Fernández et al., 2016). Preparatory behaviors are those formed a priori to the behavior, thus act as a sub-goal (Barz et al., 2016). Action control refers to the monitoring and evaluation of action with respect to the behavior (Fernández et al., 2016). Preparatory behaviors, like planning strategies, are prospective routes to change; whereas action control involves concurrent strategies. In placing these two strategies as moderators, that is planning is facilitated by the identification of preparatory behaviors and implementation of action control, other moderators may also influence their success such as self-efficacy (Bandura, 1989) and cognitive resource depletion (Baumeister et al., 2007). For example, Barz et al. (2016) found preparatory behaviors to be more useful for those with less efficacy beliefs. That is, the transfer of positive intentions via planning and preparatory behaviors was more prevalent in those who didn’t believe in their abilities.
In summary, it can be suggested that motivation results in the formation of a behavioral intention, which can lead to the development of volitional strategies such as action and coping plans. The identification of preparatory behaviors alongside action control then facilitates the transfer of these plans into action. The inclusion of volitional strategies can therefore significantly assist self-regulation.
Non-Conscious Processes
Aside from conscious processes and planning approaches, recent interest within health models of behavior change concerns the inclusion of implicit processes (Sheeran et al., 2013). Such processes operate at the non-conscious level without awareness, and are seen as being relatively automatic and fast. As such, it is argued that processes of which popular models of cognition such as the TPB target, are insufficient to explain and change behavior (Sniehotta et al., 2014), with the lack of explanatory power a resultant of processes such theories fail to take into consideration. The ability to self-regulate and thus enact behavior may be heavily dependent on implicit processes.
Measuring the Implicit
Despite the ease of which reflective processes can be assessed using self-report measures, it is far more difficult to measure implicit processes since individuals may not be able to accurately reflect such processes (Hagger et al., 2015). Implicit processes have been typically measured using the Go/No-Go Association Task (Nosek and Banaji, 2001), affective priming task (Klauer and Musch, 2003), and the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998). Such measures prevent socially desirable responses often found in self-report. Concerning the IAT, which is probably the most common, participants are presented with a set of stimulus on a computer which they then classify into one of two categories through the use of a key press. The response latencies are used to calculate implicit preferences. One problem with this measure is that it requires the use of opposite stimuli and there is no obvious dichotomy for certain behaviors, such as physical activity (Rebar et al., 2015). Moreover, it is unclear whether the test reveals a preference for a stimulus over another or a dislike for a stimulus over the alternative (Blanton et al., 2006). For example, does an individual prefer healthy foods more than unhealthy foods, or is their distaste for unhealthy foods more prevalent than their love for healthy foods. The Single-Category Implicit Association (Karpinski and Steinman, 2006), which only has one target category, has been developed to address this problem. Other concerns refer to the specificity of implicit measures and its internal validity. The TPB assesses reflective beliefs specified at the target, action, context, and time (TACT), whereas non-conscious measures appear to provide assessments on a broader level (Perugini and Richetin, 2011). Furthermore, response latencies may be a product of physical ability; not cognitive processes (Jaccard and Blanton, 2006).
Dual Process Theories
The relationship between implicit and explicit processes can be framed using a variety of dual-processing models. On the whole, these models describe one level of processing that is deliberate and requires reflection, and another level of processing that is implicit, automatic and requires minimal cognitive resources. However, these definitions vary and have been mixed depending on the theoretical stance taken. For example, according to Hollands et al. (2016), non-conscious behavior can be ascribed to situations when the causal link between a stimulus and the behavior is not recognized, thus implying consciousness is not automatic and is under individual control. This sense of authorship may be illusionary (Wegner, 2002), however, as a strong subjective sense of a causal role isn’t sufficient to attribute causation (Bargh, 2008). In terms of the models, just like the plethora of change theories identifying reflective processes, a similar pattern has emerged integrating both types of processes. More specifically, these theories offer differing interactions between the processes, resulting in a confused understanding of the two (Payne and Gawronski, 2010). Understanding how the two processes interact and the type of interventions required will be discussed later.
Intervention Implications of Non-Conscious Processes
It is important to identify the role that the non-conscious plays in behavior change and self-regulation as this has important implications for intervention design. Papies (2016a) suggests that cues do not affect behavior directly, but do so by activating cognitive structures that have been formed by previous experiences in similar situations. Thus, there is a mediation of implicit processes, with the activation of implicit structures or schemata significant. This poses the question of how difficult such processes are to change as a consequence of prior learning and experiences (Wilson et al., 2000). If features of the situation can be changed so that they alter which of a person’s associations stored in memory becomes activated (Strack and Deutsch, 2004), this would provide avenues for target. Similarly, if such measures represent implicit processes responding to the environment, these external influencers can be used to target behavior via these processes. Of note here are prior situated conceptualizations and their interactions with environmental stimuli. If the environment is producing change wholly, the individual could be viewed as a blank state, ‘tabula rasa’ known to the behaviorists. An implicit change would, in turn, produce similar responses in each individual (Payne and Gawronski, 2010). Thus, there is a tension between whether processes are representative of the individual or their environment (Payne and Gawronski, 2010). Due to the processes generated from prior experiences, such a conclusion is unlikely. What is more likely is the activation of implicit processes, resulting in implicit processes that are still amendable to change (Gawronski and Bodenhausen, 2006). As the mere perception of objects activates stored memory associations (Hall and Fong, 2007), priming can be used to manipulate desired responses (Bargh et al., 2012). Such priming influences have been discussed recently by Papies (2016b). Planning can also be used to prime positive behavior. That is, when the situation is encountered, the cue enacts the behavior automatically (Gollwitzer and Brandstätter, 1997).
In addition to focusing on what is automatically activated through external affects, attention can be placed on how people respond to the stimuli. Such a process parallels the procedures involved in reflective models such as Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) whereby responses to automatic conscious processes bear the focus of intervention (Ellis, 1957; Beck, 1964). Changing how an individual responds to implicit processes can be done in a number of ways such as evaluative conditioning (Levey and Martin, 1975), approach/avoidance training (AAT; Chen and Bargh, 1999), Attention Bias Modification Treatment (ABMT; MacLeod et al., 2002), and mindfulness (Segal et al., 2002). Following the ideas of classical conditioning (Pavlov, 1927), evaluative conditioning refers to changes in liking or disliking that are due to the pairing of stimuli (De Houwer, 2007). AAT involves multiple trials wherein participants are taught to overcome automatic action tendencies by learning to approach one class of stimuli and avoid its counterpart. ABMT weakens attentional responses by substituting the anticipated stimulus with a neutral one (Sheeran et al., 2013). Within mindfulness, attention shifts from the content of thoughts and feelings to the experience encountered within the present moment (Baer et al., 2006). Thus, in comparison to the challenging and realignment of maladaptive thoughts apparent within CBT, the aim of mindfulness is a refocus of awareness, which decreases the influence of implicit processes. Responses to unwanted influences can also be controlled using plans (Gollwitzer et al., 2011). Furthermore, coping plans can be used to prevent the urge of implicit processes overriding conscious processes by intervening between responses. Specifically, precise descriptions of coping mechanisms automatically activate and trigger responses to decrease the unhelpful behaviors’ influence. In all of these approaches, the individual regains a sense of volition over implicit processes rather than succumbing to the activated schemata.
When the Conscious/Non-Conscious Do Battle
In addition to the influence of implicit processes, reflective processes also have a role to play in behavior. The development of the reflective system, which is seen as evolutionary new, performs certain tasks that the non-conscious cannot (Baumeister and Bargh, 2014). As stated earlier, the exact relation between the processes varies within each dual model. The following are potential interactions and their impact on behavior.
Both processes can influence behavior independently or act as moderators (i.e., the influence of the non-conscious is moderated by consciousness or the influence of conscious processes is moderated by the non-conscious) (Rebar et al., 2016). The limited space allocated for consciousness is facilitated by the implicit processes triggered automatically whilst providing adaptive responses to the environment (Baumeister and Bargh, 2014). Reflective processes may develop an intention and, once repeatedly linked with a stimulus, become under the guidance of the non-conscious through automatic activation (Gardner, 2015). Thus, once activated, action is induced through non-conscious processes instead of prior reflective states (Gardner, 2015). Targeting intention and habits may therefore be fruitful (Allom et al., 2016). Automatic processing may be the default mode, guiding behavior in accordance with one’s implicit attitudes unless an individual is prompt into deliberation (Evans and Stanovich, 2013). When motivation and opportunity is high, reflective processes may be in control compared to the automated influences when such motives and opportunity are low (Fazio, 1990). Due to its finite resource and susceptibility to depletion, a weakened cognitive state could hand self-control over to non-conscious processes despite having motivation and opportunity (Friese et al., 2009). Engaging in persistent decision making, complex experiences, and constant restraints is likely to drain the battery of self-control (Baumeister et al., 2007), leading to mental contamination (Wilson and Brekke, 1994). Tempting, unhealthy hedonic outcomes may be in contrast with the reflective, healthier outcomes (Strack and Deutsch, 2004). Affective attitudes, distinct from instrumental processes, could be seen as implicit routes to behavior (Conner et al., 2011). Such affective beliefs have been found to better predict short-term choices compared to instrumental beliefs governing long-term outcomes (Morris et al., 2016). Thus, prior positive long-term motivational intentions developed from the careful consideration of behavioral pros and cons could be overridden in the wake of affective outcomes. Finally, in utilizing a neurobiological approach, the influence of intentions on behavior can be moderated by executive function and behavioral prepotency (Hall and Fong, 2015). Those possessing higher inhibitory ability are more likely to suppress and inhibit urges and thus enable control to remain in the reflective system.
Although their relation is manifold, what is clear is the stronger the influence of the non-conscious, the more one has to consciously overcome it (Bargh, 2014). The tug-of-war between the processes, encapsulated well within the horse rider metaphor (Friese et al., 2011), determines whether the reflective or implicit prevails (Hofmann et al., 2008). The strength of the competing schemas resulting from boundary conditions will determine the ‘winner’ (Strack and Deutsch, 2004). Although the concordance between implicit and explicit processes in health psychology remains unclear and some assert that reflective processes still account for implicit factors through mediation (e.g., Jaccard and Blanton, 2006; Ajzen and Dasgupta, 2015), research has moved from simply acknowledging non-conscious existence to examining their interactions and the situations that influence different responses (Hagger, 2016).
Conclusion
Although research in behavior change has primarily concerned conscious processes, it is important to account for implicit processes in addition to its more common reflective counterpart. Such influences offer an explanation as to why there appears to be a significant gap between what one intends to do and what is actually performed, thus taps into problems of self-regulation. Despite the inclusion of implicit processes in several theories of behavior change, there remains a lack of effective interventions (Hollands et al., 2016). Due to the rapid progression in establishing a science of behavior change (Michie et al., 2008), linking techniques with determinants of change (Michie and Abraham, 2004) and establishing which combine best to facilitate change (Michie et al., 2009) would prove valuable. The number of techniques required could be of particular use (Dombrowski et al., 2012); specifically as different techniques are needed to change the different processes. Although there are concerns surrounding intervention delivery and reporting (Michie and Prestwich, 2010; Dombrowski et al., 2016), interventions and techniques should be included within the taxonomies currently dominated by reflective processes (Sheeran et al., 2013). Such additions may satisfy recent criticisms of popular reflective theories (e.g., Sniehotta et al., 2014).
Author Contributions
The article was written by the first author, TSQ. The second author, JB, provided guidance, drafting and final approval.
Funding
This research was supported by a Leeds Trinity University Ph.D. Studentship Award.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). “From intentions to action: a theory of planned behavior,” in Action Control: From Cognitions to Behaviours, eds J. Kuhl and J. Beckman (New York, NY: Springer), 11–39.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
Ajzen, I. (2015). The theory of planned behaviour is alive and well, and not ready to retire: a commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau, and Araújo-Soares. Health Psychol. Rev. 9, 131–137. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.883474
Ajzen, I., and Dasgupta, N. (2015). “Explicit and implicit beliefs, attitudes, and intentions: the role of conscious and unconscious processes in human behavior,” in The Sense of Agency, eds P. Haggard and B. Eitam (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 115–144. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190267278.003.0005
Allom, V., Mullan, B., Cowie, E., and Hamilton, K. (2016). Physical activity and transitioning to college: the importance of intentions and habits. Am. J. Health Behav. 40, 280–290. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.40.2.13
Armitage, C. J., and Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour : a meta-analytic review. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 471–499. doi: 10.1348/014466601164939
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. Am. Psychol. 44, 1175–1184. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175
Bargh, J. A. (2008). “Free will is unnatural,” in Psychology and Free Will, eds J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman, and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).
Bargh, J. A., Schwader, K. L., Hailey, S. E., Dyer, R. L., and Boothby, E. J. (2012). Automaticity in social–cognitive processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 593–605. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.002
Barz, M., Lange, D., Parschau, L., Lonsdale, C., Knoll, N., and Schwarzer, R. (2016). Self-efficacy, planning, and preparatory behaviours as joint predictors of physical activity: a conditional process analysis. Psychol. Health 31, 65–78. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2015.1070157
Baumeister, R. F., and Bargh, J. A. (2014). “Conscious and unconscious: toward an integrative understanding of human life and action,” in Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind, eds J. Sherman, B. Gawronski, and Y. Trope (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 33–49.
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., and Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression: theory and therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 10, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1964.01720240015003
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzales, P. M., and Christie, C. (2006). Decoding the implicit association test: implications for criterion prediction. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42, 192–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.07.003
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., and Hagger, M. S. (2005). Effects of a brief intervention based on the theory of planned behavior on leisure time physical activity participation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 27, 470–487. doi: 10.1123/jsep.27.4.470
Chen, M., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 215–224. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002007
Conner, M., Prestwich, A., and Ayres, K. (2011). Using explicit affective attitudes to tap impulsive influences on health behaviour: a commentary on Hofmann et al. (2008). Health Psychol. Rev. 5, 145–149. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.539969
De Houwer, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. Span. J. Psychol. 10, 230–241. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600006491
Dombrowski, S. U., O’Carroll, R. E., and Williams, B. (2016). Form of delivery as a key ‘active ingredient’ in behaviour change interventions. Br. J. Health Psychol. 21, 733–740. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12203
Dombrowski, S. U., Sniehotta, F. F., Avenell, A., Johnston, M., MacLennan, G., and Araujo-Soares, V. (2012). Identifying active ingredients in complex behavioural interventions for obese adults with obesity-related co-morbidities or additional risk factors for co-morbidities: a systematic review. Health Psychol. Rev. 6, 7–32. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.513298
Evans, J. B. T., and Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 223–241. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685
Fazio, R. H. (1990). “Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: the MODE model as an integrative framework,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed. M. P. Zanna (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 75–109.
Fernández, B. R., Knoll, N., Hamilton, K., and Schwarzer, R. (2016). Social-cognitive antecedents of hand washing: action control bridges the planning–behaviour gap. Psychol. Health 31, 993–1004. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2016.1174236
French, D. P., Stevenson, A., and Michie, S. (2012). An intervention to increase walking requires both motivational and volitional components: a replication and extension. Psychol. Health Med. 17, 127–135. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2011.592843
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., and Wänke, M. (2009). Control me or I will control you: impulses, trait self-control, and the guidance of behavior. J. Res. Pers. 43, 795–805. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.07.004
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., and Wiers, R. (2011). On taming horses and strengthening riders: recent developments in research on interventions to improve self-control in health behaviors. Self Identity 10, 336–351. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2010.536417
Gardner, B. (2015). A review and analysis of the use of ‘habit’ in understanding, predicting and influencing health-related behaviour. Health Psychol. Rev. 9, 277–295. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2013.876238
Gawronski, B., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol. Bull. 132, 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. Am. Psychol. 54, 493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). “Setting one’s mind on action: planning out goal striving in advance,” in Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, eds R. Scott and S. Kosslyn (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage).
Gollwitzer, P. M., and Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 186–199. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.186
Gollwitzer, P. M., Sheeran, P., Trötschel, R., and Webb, T. L. (2011). Self-regulation of behavior priming effects. Psychol. Sci. 22, 901–907. doi: 10.1177/0956797611411586
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
Hagger, M. S. (2016). Non-conscious processes and dual-process theories in health psychology. Health Psychol. Rev. 10, 375–380. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1244647
Hagger, M. S., and Luszczynska, A. (2014). Implementation intention and action planning interventions in health contexts: state of the research and proposals for the way forward. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 6, 1–47. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12017
Hagger, M. S., Rebar, A. L., Mullan, B., Lipp, O. V., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2015). The subjective experience of habit captured by self-report indexes may lead to inaccuracies in the measurement of habitual action. Health Psychol. Rev. 9, 296–302. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.959728
Hall, P. A., and Fong, G. T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory: a model for individual health behaviour. Health Psychol. Rev. 1, 6–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.043
Hall, P. A., and Fong, G. T. (2015). Temporal self-regulation theory: a neurobiologically informed model for physical activity behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:117. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00117
Hardeman, W., Johnston, M., Johnston, D. W., Bonetti, D., Wareham, N. J., and Kinmonth, A. L. (2002). Application of the theory of planned behaviour in behaviour change interventions: a systematic review. Psychol. Health 17, 123–158. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2015.1019011
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., and Wiers, R. W. (2008). Impulsive versus reflective influences on health behavior: a theoretical framework and empirical review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2, 111–137. doi: 10.1080/17437190802617668
Hollands, G. J., Marteau, T. M., and Fletcher, P. C. (2016). Non-conscious processes in changing health-related behaviour: a conceptual analysis and framework. Health Psychol. Rev. 10, 381–394. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2015.1138093
Jaccard, J., and Blanton, H. (2006). “A theory of implicit reasoned action: the role of implicit and explicit attitudes in the prediction of behavior,” in Prediction and Change of Health Behavior: Applying the Reasoned Action Approach, eds I. Ajzen, D. Albarracin, and J. Hornik (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 69–94.
Karpinski, A., and Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 16–32. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16
Klauer, K. C., and Musch, J. (2003). “Affective priming: findings and theories,” in The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion, eds J. Musch and K. C. Klauer (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum).
Levey, A. B., and Martin, I. (1975). Classical conditioning of human ‘evaluative’ responses. Behav. Res. Ther. 13, 221–226. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(75)90026-1
MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., and Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causa basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111, 107–123. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.1.107
McEachan, R. R. C., Conner, M., Taylor, N. J., and Lawton, R. J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 5, 97–144. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.613995
Michie, S., and Abraham, C. (2004). Identifying techniques that promote health behaviour change: evidence based or evidence inspired? Psychol. Health 19, 29–49. doi: 10.1080/0887044031000141199
Michie, S., Abraham, C., Wittington, C., McAteer, J., and Gupta, S. (2009). Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 28, 690–701. doi: 10.1037/a0016136
Michie, S., and Johnston, M. (2012). Theories and techniques of behaviour change: developing a cumulative science of behaviour change. Health Psychol. Rev. 6, 1–6. doi: 10.3310/hta19990
Michie, S., Johnston, M., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., and Eccles, M. (2008). From theory to intervention: mapping theoretically derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change techniques. Appl. Psychol. 57, 660–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00341.x
Michie, S., and Prestwich, A. (2010). Are interventions theory-based? Development of a theory coding scheme. Health Psychol. 29, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/a0016939
Milne, S. E., Orbell, S., and Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining motivational and volitional interventions to promote exercise participation: protection motivation theory and implementation intentions. Br. J. Health Psychol. 7, 163–184. doi: 10.1348/135910702169420
Morris, B., Lawton, R., McEachan, R., Hurling, R., and Conner, M. (2016). Changing self-reported physical activity using different types of affectively and cognitively framed health messages, in a student population. Psychol. Health Med. 21, 198–207. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2014.997762
Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Soc. Cogn. 19, 625–664. doi: 10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886
Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., and Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 945–954. doi: 10.1177/0146167297239004
Papies, E. K. (2016a). Health goal priming as a situated intervention tool: how to benefit from nonconscious motivational routes to health behaviour. Health Psychol. Rev. 10, 408–424.
Papies, E. K. (2016b). Goal priming as a situated intervention tool. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 12, 12–16.
Payne, B. K., and Gawronski, B. (2010). “A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going?,” in Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications, eds B. Gawronski and B. K. Payne (New York, NY: Guilford Press).
Perugini, M., and Richetin, J. (2011). Time matters. Health Psychol. Rev. 5, 154–157. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.529635
Prestwich, A. J., Lawton, R. J., and Conner, M. T. (2003). The use of implementation intentions and the decision balance sheet in promoting exercise behavior. Psychol. Health 18, 707–721. doi: 10.1080/08870440310001594493
Rebar, A. L., Dimmock, J. A., Jackson, B., Rhodes, R. E., Kates, A., Starling, J., et al. (2016). A systematic review of the effects of non-conscious regulatory processes in physical activity. Health Psychol. Rev. 10, 395–407. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1183505
Rebar, A. L., Ram, N., and Conroy, D. E. (2015). Using the EZ-diffusion model to score a single-category implicit association test of physical activity. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 16, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.09.008
Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: how to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Appl. Psychol. 57, 1–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x
Schwarzer, R. (2016). Coping planning as an intervention component: a commentary. Psychol. Health 31, 903–906. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2016.1158260
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., and Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York, NY: Guilford.
Sheeran, P., and Abraham, C. (2003). Mediator of moderators: temporal stability of intention and the intention-behavior relation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 205–215. doi: 10.1177/0146167202239046
Sheeran, P., Gollwitzer, P. M., and Bargh, J. A. (2013). Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychol. 32, 460–473. doi: 10.1037/a0029203
Sniehotta, F. (2009). An experimental test of the theory of planned behavior. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 1, 257–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01013.x
Sniehotta, F. F., Presseau, J., and Araújo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to retire the theory of planned behavior. Health Psychol. Rev. 8, 1–7. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2013.869710
Sniehotta, F. F., Schwarzer, R., Scholz, U., and Schüz, B. (2005). Action planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35, 565–576. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.258
Strack, F., and Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
Webb, T. L., and Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender bahaviour change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol. Bull. 132, 249–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249
Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Ann. Behav. Med. 33, 1–10. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3301_1
White, K., Terry, D. J., Troup, C., and Kenardy, J. (2012). An extended theory of planned behavior intervention for older adults with type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease. J. Aging Phys. Act. 20, 281–299. doi: 10.1123/japa.20.3.281
Wilson, T., Lindsey, S., and Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychol. Rev. 107, 101–126. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101
Wilson, T. D., and Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychol. Bull. 116, 117–142. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.117
Keywords: self-regulation, implicit, conscious, non-conscious, dual-process
Citation: St Quinton T and Brunton JA (2017) Implicit Processes, Self-Regulation, and Interventions for Behavior Change. Front. Psychol. 8:346. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00346
Received: 13 December 2016; Accepted: 23 February 2017;
Published: 08 March 2017.
Edited by:
Michael Noll-Hussong, University of Ulm, GermanyReviewed by:
Alicia Abundis-Gutierrez, University of Guadalajara, MexicoBerangere Thirioux, Unité de Recherche Clinique Pierre Deniker, Centre Hospitalo-Universitaire Henri Laborit, France
Copyright © 2017 St Quinton and Brunton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Tom St Quinton, t.stquinton@leedstrinity.ac.uk