Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 February 2017
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Triadic mother-father-child interactive processes in developmental research and clinical contexts View all 13 articles

The Desire of Parenthood: Intuitive Co-parental Behaviors and Quality of Couple Relationship among Italian and Belgian Same-Sex and Opposite-Sex Couples

  • 1Department of Psychology and Clinics of Human Systems, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 2Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy

Studies that focused on family issues have allowed a great understanding of the aspects related to its subsystems, such as parenting desire and its expectations, couples’ satisfaction and quality of child’s outcomes. All these aspects are greatly interconnected and contribute to the creation of specific family dynamics, such as the quality of family interactions. The present study focuses on intuitive co-parental behaviors and the quality of couple relationship observed during the decision process (intention and desire) to be (or become) parents. Our first goal was to explore these aspects in a cross-national sample made of Italian and Belgian heterosexual, lesbian and gay couples. We then aimed to evaluate if the degree of internalized homophobia affects co-parental alliance. The quality of couple relationship and co-parental behaviors have been evaluated through the recruitment of a group of 115 stable heterosexual, gay and lesbian couples (230 individuals, 20–50 years of age) without children, who wanted to become parents. We used the Prenatal Lausanne Trilogue Play to evaluate the Co-parental Alliance; the couple’s satisfaction was assessed with the Dyadic Adjustment Scale and the Internalized Homophobia with the MISS-LG. In line with the existent literature, the analysis did not find any difference between lesbian, gay and heterosexual couples in terms of co-parental alliance. High levels of couple adjustment lead to better parental performances among both Italian and Belgian couples. The results suggest also that sexual stigma differs from one country to another, and it has an impact on the capability of managing co-parenting. Clinical implications should be verified in further longitudinal studies in order to observe the impact on the inter-generational transmission of psychopathology.

Introduction

Over the past few years, we noticed an increase on LGBT (Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transsexual) family issues research, with a focus on family internal dynamics, in comparison with the first research field, which concentrated mostly on children’s outcomes (Patterson, 2005; Tasker, 2005; Peplau and Fingerhut, 2007), co-parental behaviors and couple’s adjustment (Farr and Patterson, 2013). These studies and reviews showed how lesbians and gay men could be parents as well as heterosexuals (Falk, 1989; Victor and Fish, 1995; Patterson and Chan, 1996; Brewaeys and Van Hall, 1997; Parks, 1998; Tasker, 1999; Patterson, 2000; Stacey and Biblarz, 2001; Anderssen et al., 2002; Perrin, 2002) and consequently, how motherhood and fatherhood do not relate to sexual orientation. Farr and Patterson (2013) examined differences between lesbian, gay and heterosexual couples studying correlations between self-reported division of the daily tasks, adopted children adjustment and co-parenting evaluation through an observational technique during a play session. All along the role-play, the authors assessed supportive (warmth, enjoyment, eyes contact) and undermining (to talk over the partner, to suggest different toys to the child) behaviors (McHale et al., 2001). Results have detected differences between the three groups in terms of labor division and co-parenting. Lesbian and gay parents were more likely than heterosexual parents to report sharing child-care. In addition, lesbian couples showed the most supportive interactions while gay couples showed the least. Heterosexual couples were intermediate between lesbian and gay couples in supportive behaviors. Despite those results, no differences have been found in terms of child adjustment between the three groups of families.

At the same time, several longitudinal studies, which were conducted with heterosexual-headed families, showed that early family interactions have a significant influence on a child’s emotional and cognitive development, in particular during the first years of the child’s life (Carneiro et al., 2006). Studies on the development of families, which include observations of parent-infant interactive behaviors, revealed that mother-father-infant interactions during infancy are predictive of emotional and cognitive outcomes in the child (Favez et al., 2012), especially for the development of the theory of mind assessed at age 5 (Frascarolo et al., 2008).

Yet, the interest shown for LGBT families’ issues concerning the quality of family interactions is quite recent. D’Amore et al. (2013) investigated differences between both healthy lesbian and heterosexual couples and a group formed by heterosexual couples with depressed mothers, using the Lausanne Trilogue Play paradigm (LTP; Fivaz-Depeursinge and Corboz-Warnery, 1999). The clinical approach detected no differences between healthy groups, independently from the sexual orientation, but rather between the two healthy groups and the group that included depressed mothers, explaining how much the maternal psychopathology could affect co-parental behaviors. In order to better understand on which levels it is necessary to intervene to encourage the healthy development of the family, future research is necessary to investigate which protective and risk factors are in common to all families, and which ones are specific of different families’ configuration.

The Desire of Parenthood

Changes that accompany the process to parenthood are so deep; their impacts vary over time, from couple to couple, and from individual to individual (Delmore-Ko et al., 2000). Sommer et al. (1993, p. 389) found that individuals who were more “cognitively ready” to become parents had lower levels of parenting stress and were more prone to adapt in their parenting style. Researchers demonstrated the considerable impact that the birth of a first child has on many aspects of family life, and the importance of couples’ expectations in predicting how they will adjust to these changes. The following factors are protective ones for the health of both the family and the child: a stable relationship, the support of the family of origin and an appropriate development of the individual (the future parent) and the couple desire.

The relationship quality has been shown to influence women’s feelings about childbearing. Fischer et al. (1999) found that women were more likely to want a pregnancy if they expressed positive feelings about their partner. The expectation about the duration of the relationship is another measure related to the stability of the relationship, and this measure has been found to influence the desire of having a baby (Wilson and Koo, 2006). Some studies suggested that women who do not have children with their current partner, even if they already have ones from previous romantic relationships, would want to have at least one child with him in order to achieve their concept of “family” (Thornton, 1978; Hoffman and Manis, 1979) or to strengthen the relationship (Westoff, 1977).

Riskind and Patterson (2010) published national representative data about parenting intentions and desires in a sample of American childless lesbian, gay and heterosexual people, and they found that 37% of childless lesbian participants expressed a desire for children, compared with 68% of heterosexuals women. On the contrary, 54% of childless gay men expressed a desire for children compared to 67% of heterosexual men. Despite this difference between gay men and lesbians, gay men who reported the desire to become parents were less likely to also express the intention of becoming parents than heterosexual men. While lesbians who expressed the desire of becoming parent expressed the intention of it as well (Riskind and Patterson, 2010).

D’Augelli et al. (2007), in an American sample of urban lesbian and gay youths, found strong expectations of parenthood among lesbian (91%) and gay youths (86%). Similarly, Gates et al. (2007) discovered that childless gay men were less likely to express a desire for children (52–67%), compared with childless heterosexual men.

Previous research showed that among gay- and lesbian-headed families the asymmetry between gender role (masculine and feminine) and parental role (paternal and maternal) requests a continuous redefinition of tasks, in particular concerning child-rearing, which does not reflect the parents’ biological genders (Coltrane, 2000). Lesbian and gay couples, for example, often report to divide child-care more equally than heterosexual couples, who, on the contrary, report role specialization (Goldberg, 2010). Despite this difference, same-sex couples and opposite-sex couples declare the same degree of satisfaction about their relationship (Sullivan, 1996; Tasker and Golombok, 1998; Johnson and O’Connor, 2002; Bos et al., 2004; Patterson, 2005). Patterson (1995a,b) revealed differences among lesbian couples with a biological mother and a non-biological mother. The author showed that co-mothers were not different in the quantity of involvement, but in the quality: biological mothers spent more time in the real child-caregiving, whereas non-biological mothers did it in activities and playtime. Bos et al. (2007) showed that biological mothers use more authority than non-biological mothers, and non-biological mothers happen to be less rigid and more involved than heterosexual fathers. Despite these qualitative differences, the involvement in caregiving is distributed more equally among lesbians (Chan et al., 1998; Ciano-Boyce and Shelley-Sireci, 2002). Other studies regarding lesbian motherhood showed that mother-child interaction patterns are similar with opposite-sex parents’ ones (Vanfraussen et al., 2003) or even better (Brewaeys and Van Hall, 1997).

In our opinion, the observation of the transition to parenthood, which shows the way a family develops, could be useful to extend the comprehension about protective and risk factors. The assumption is that family is an already created concept in couples’ minds even before a real family is formed.

Co-parental Behaviors and Quality of Couple Relationship

Studies about family system have stressed a distinction between “marital couple” and “co-parental couple” (Cowan and McHale, 1996; Katz and Gottman, 1996; McHale and Fivaz-Depeursinge, 1999; McHale, 2007; Simonelli et al., 2012). The first one refers to everything regarding the relationship between two adults partners mutually bound (Simonelli et al., 2012). Co-parenting describes the synchronization between adults in their parental roles (Minuchin, 1974) and it refers to their capacity to share work and responsibilities toward child caregiving. Co-parenting refers to the competence of mutual support and coordination between adults who are responsible for childcare and child rearing (McHale, 2007).

Researchers and theorists developed co-parenting notions, in studying the cooperation between parents-to-be by the use of observational techniques administered during the pregnancy (Carneiro et al., 2006), when both behavioral and mental aspects of parenting are triggered (Simonelli et al., 2012). According to Corboz-Warnery and Fivaz-Depeursinge (2001), the cooperation between parents appears during the pregnancy through intuitive behaviors (Papoušek and Papoušek, 1987), which anticipate the first encounter with the infant. Longitudinal studies that used the prenatal Lausanne Trilogue Play (prenatal LTP; Carneiro et al., 2006) showed that the interactions in the pregnancy period, with a fake baby (doll), were predictive of the future interactions with the real baby, after the childbirth (Favez et al., 2006). Marital and co-parental relationships are both important for the child’s outcomes (Belsky, 1984), but co-parenting has been found to be more strongly related to the child’s adjustment than other factors of couple relationship (Farr and Patterson, 2013). In fact, dysfunctional difficulties, at a co-parental level, could adversely affect the child’s outcomes (Minuchin and Minuchin, 1987) and promote the development of negative cognitive, social and emotional factors (McHale, 2007; Gatta et al., 2016a,b). Co-parenting could be one of the most important and influent mediator factors for functional family patterns, which ensures a good child’s development.

Aims

According to the literature references presented above, the aim was to investigate co-parental alliance in order to (i) observe similarities or differences concerning co-parenting among stable gay, lesbian and heterosexual couples without children; (ii) evaluate, among LG couples, the correlation between co-parental dimension, dyadic adjustment, internalized homophobia and social support.

We suppose that there are no differences among the three types of couples when it comes to the quality of co-parental alliance. On the contrary, we hypothesize that couples with “better quality” of relationship and “high social support” show “better quality” in co-parental alliance, and that the internalized homophobia affects co-parental skills among LG couples.

This research also aimed to explore if the availability of legal marriage within a particular country influences the ways in which LG couples manage their transitions to parenthood and co-parental alliances. We also evaluate in what extent the degree of internalized homophobia affects co-parental alliance. We hypothesize that high levels of internalized homophobia lead to low scores of co-parental alliance.

We also assume that participants from two different countries, with different legislations and civil rights for LGBT people, show a different degree of internalized homophobia. For this purpose, this paper focuses on two groups of gay and lesbian couples living in two different European countries: Italy and Belgium.

Civil Rights for LGBT People

The choice to involve subjects from Italy and Belgium is motivated by the legal differences existing at the time of the recruitment (October 2014–December 2015) in terms of LG population’s civil rights. Belgium represents one of the leading countries in recognizing and protecting civil rights among LGBT people. The “Europe Annual Review of the Human Rights Situation of LGBTI People in Europe” (2014), published by the ILGA (International Lesbian and Gay Association [ILGA] Europe, 2014), sees Belgium in second position over 49 European countries for LGBTI (Lesbians, Gay, Bisexuals, Transsexuals, and Intersexuals) issues, whereas Italy ranks 32nd. Since 2003, Belgium legally recognizes same-sex couples by civil marriage, and, since 2006, it allows the adoption and the access to medically assisted procreation. On the contrary, Italian legislation is still incomplete in terms of legal protection of gay and lesbian community’s rights. Indeed, during the recruitment, no legislation regarding same-sex civil unions existed in Italy, and such status was not even legally recognized.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred and fifteen unmarried stable couples were recruited in Italy and Belgium (128 volunteers from Belgium with mean age = 24.3 years, SD = 3.7; 102 volunteers from Italy with mean age = 29.2 years, SD = 6.4). On Table 1 it can be noticed that groups are different in terms of age and duration of relationship. Observing the groups average, Italian participants are older than the Belgian ones and they have been in their relationships longer.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Mean and standard deviation of Age (years), duration of the relationship (in months) and coming-out age (years) of each group.

Inclusion criteria were: (i) being in a stable relationship for 1 year at least to be sure that the adult attachment was established, (ii) never been married, (iii) never had children [because we wanted to evaluate the relationship with the imaginary child (Lebovici, 1988) and not with a real baby], (iv) be supportive to the idea of becoming parents in the future.

Participants were recruited1 in northern Italy and Liège county in Belgium through web-posted advertisements, which informed that research was about “couples and parenting” without other explanations, in order to not induce biased answers. Thereupon, the sample is not randomized, but it could be defined as a “convenience” sample.

Each participant agreed to the informed consent and they gave their e-mails to allow follow-up contacts. After having submitted the answers, a researcher sent an e-mail to invite them in a specific laboratory2 for performing LTP and completing questionnaires.

Materials

Prenatal Lausanne Trilogue Play. Prenatal LTP (Carneiro et al., 2006) is a peculiar LTP (Fivaz-Depeursinge and Corboz-Warnery, 1999) version. Authors created this observational tool to evaluate representations of the child-to-be during the pregnancy, in particular through the seventh month. This is the reason why none of the couples picked for this study was pregnant. Parents-to-be are invited to sit by a facilitator in a triangular configuration, with a basket as a vertex. A “neutral” doll, with the typical size and shape of a newborn, represents the baby. The face has features and traits of a Caucasian baby, “neutral” in relation to sex or particular eye, skin, and hair color. Such “neutrality” should help parents-to-be to role-play the situation. A camera records the entire procedure, standing in front of the parents, it slightly varies from the original LTP during which there is another camera recording the infant’s facial expression. The facilitator asks the parents to imagine the moment when the three of them would encounter for the first time after delivery. He/she explains that the task has four parts: (a) One of them would play with the baby doll, (b) then the other, (c) then the parents would play together with the baby, and finally, (d) they would let the infant “sleep” and then talk together about the experience that they just had. The exercise lasts about 5 min.

Prenatal co-parenting was assessed in the prenatal LTP situation using five scales on a Likert Scale ranging from 0 to 5. Carneiro et al. (2006) specifically elaborated the first three scales to analyze the prenatal LTP. The five scales are: (1) Co-Parent Playfulness, which assesses the capacity of the couple to co-construct playful games. The evaluation also concerns the ability of the couple to understand that the situation is a simulation and not the reality. (2) Structure of the Play, assesses the couple’s capacity to structure the four play parts. In this scale, two dimensions are considered: the differentiation of the play into four discrete segments and the duration of the entire play sequence as well as of each segment. (3) Intuitive Parenting Behaviors, assesses the parents’ use of intuitive parenting behaviors. Six behaviors that relate to literature (Papoušek and Papoušek, 1987) are coded: holding and “en face” orientation, dialog distance, baby talk and/or smiles at baby, cuddling and/or rocking, exploration of baby’s body, and preoccupation with the baby’s well-being. These intuitive parenting behaviors are assessed as present or absent for each parent. These individual results are mixed together into a global score for the couple. (4) Couple Cooperation Scale, assesses the degree of active cooperation between the parents during the play, at a behavioral level. And finally, (5) Family Warmth, captures the affection and mood shared by the partners during the play; namely, whether they manifest affection and tenderness as a couple and toward their “baby.”

Scores of the five scales are added to obtain a global score between 5 and 25. The higher is the score, the more the prenatal alliance is considered to be functional.

Measures of Internalized Sexual Stigma for Lesbians and Gay Men. MISS-LG (Lingiardi et al., 2012) is a doubled version scale, one for each gender, formed by 17 items on Likert Scale with five points (from “I disagree” to “I agree”). It quantifies internalized stigma degree, defined as the feelings expressed by gay men and lesbians toward homosexuality at large and toward themselves (ibidem). Two versions (L and G) are the same in 11 items, but differ in six that reflect gender differences. Scoring gives back four scores3 regarding three dimensions of the internalized sexual stigma and a total score that results from the average of all items. The higher is the total score, the more is the Internalized Sexual Stigma degree.

Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976; Italian version translated and validated by Gentili et al., 2002; French version validated by Vandeleur et al., 2003). The 32 items of the scale assess several aspects of the couple’s life, such as the frequency and intensity of disagreements and/or agreements on the marital emotions, actions, and activities. The total of the answers display a score between 0 and 151: the higher the score is, the higher the couple is satisfied with its relationship. An average score is computed for every couple.

The Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL; Cohen and Hoberman, 1983; Italian version translated and validated by Moretti et al., 2012) evaluates how people behave when they need help during negative events. ISEL consists of a list of 40 statements concerning the perceived availability of potential social resources. The items are counter-balanced for desirability: half of the items are positive statements about social relationships while negative statements form the other half. Items fall into four 10-item subscales: tangible support, appraisal support, self-esteem support, and belonging support.

Results

Comparison between Belgian and Italian Groups

Our first aim was to explore if the availability of legal marriage influences the ways in which LG couples manage their transitions to parenthood and their co-parental alliances (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Lausanne Trilogue Play (LTP) prenatal mean scores in the three groups (Gay men, Lesbian and Heterosexuals people) divided by country of residence.

For the purpose of observing differences in the subgroups we have performed a MANOVA between the prenatal LTP scales scores of the three groups (Gay men, Lesbians and Heterosexuals) for each Country. Given the difference of age and duration of relationship between the two groups, we introduce the two variables into the model as a covariate. Results showed no difference related to “Sexual orientation” (Λ = 0.890; F = 1.265(5,51); p = 0.294) and a difference between the “Country” (Λ = 0.805; F = 2.473(5,51); p < 0.05) and “Duration of Relationship” (Λ = 0.750; F = 3.393(5,51); p < 0.05) also confirmed by the Bonferroni Post hoc; our first hypothesis has been confirmed.

The Between-Subjects Effects showed an effect of the “Country” variable on two LTP variables: “Structure of the Play” (F = 6.121; p = 0.016) and “Couple Cooperation” (F = 4.543; p = 0.038).

Looking at the means in the above table, Italians participants seem to observe the most suitable structure and timing of the play, and a more adequate couple cooperation.

In order to verify our second hypothesis, we performed an univariate two-ways ANOVA for the dyadic adjustment, social support and internalized homophobia (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Mean scores and standard deviations of Dyadic Adjustment and Social Support for each group and mean scores of Internalized Homophobia for the LG (Lesbian and Gay) participants divided in the two country groups.

About the dyadic adjustment, we discovered important differences by country of residence (F = 15.79; p = 0.000), sexual orientation (F = 11.43; p < 0.001) and their interaction (F = 11.01; p < 0.001). Bonferroni Post hoc showed a significant difference between Italian lesbian and heterosexual participants (p < 0.001) and Italian lesbians and gay men (p = 0.002). Regarding country of residence, Italian participants had more dyadic adjustments than Belgian. When running the same ANOVA for social support, no differences were revealed.

About the internalized homophobia, the two-ways ANOVA was only relevant for the country (F = 252.87; p < 0.001) and the interaction between country and sexual orientation (F = 9.72; p = 0.002). A deeper analysis showed that Italian lesbians and gay men had less internalized homophobia than Belgian ones (1.54 vs. 0.22 of average): in this context our hypothesis has not been confirmed.

Correlation between Co-parenting Alliance, Dyadic Adjustment, Social Support and Internalized Homophobia in Homosexuals Participants (Lesbian and Gay Couples)

In order to observe the connection between the co-parental dimension, the couple adjustment, the social support and the degree of internalized homophobia among LG participants, we performed one Pearson’s correlation at a time (Table 4), per country of residence.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Pearson correlations between co-parental alliance, dyadic adjustment, social support and internalized homophobia.

Analyses showed two different profiles. On one hand, we have the Belgian group for which the dyadic satisfaction significantly relates to couple cooperation: the couples who are involved in the “play of parenting” and share playfulness have higher level of social support.

On the other hand, the Italian group’s results showed that the Italian LG participants with better performances in co-parental alliance have higher score of dyadic satisfaction, and it seems to be connected with a lower level of internalized homophobia and a higher social support.

Predictor of Co-parental Alliance in Lesbian and Gay Couples

So as to test the moderating role of dyadic satisfaction and the degree of internalized homophobia on the co-parental alliance, we used a hierarchical multiple regression analysis.

First, we included the dyadic satisfaction and internalized homophobia finding that both predict the co-parental alliance (R2 = 0.077, p = 0.022), as Dyadic Satisfaction and Dyadic Consensus show positive effect on the co-parental alliance (Dyadic Satisfaction β = 0.334, p < 0.001; Dyadic Consensus β = 0.309, p = 0.028). Then, we included interactions between variables and the influence of the country of residence: the model is still predictive and it explains 15% of the variance (R2 = 0.147, p = 0.003). This analysis displayed a specific effect on the Internalized Homophobia Identity scale linked to the country of residence (β = -0.341, p = 0.042).

Discussion

This study purpose was to observe intuitive co-parental behaviors and relationship quality among LG couples who aim to become parents, compared with heterosexual peers. Our intent was to evaluate in which way the degree of internalized homophobia affects co-parental alliance, and if living in a country which has no legal system in favor of the LG community operates as a risk factor for the family-to-be.

Our sample cannot be defined as representative of the population because of its size and convenient nature. Most of Italian LG couples were members of LGBT associations, and probably more keen to show a positive image of lesbians and gay men. Nevertheless, it was complicated to reach couples out of this circuit without using specific Social Networks or accessing LGBT associations’ newsletters. Indeed, because of the absence of legal standards concerning the desire of parenthood for LG people, couples who want to become parents firstly contact these communities rather than “official” places (such as hospitals or social services) to get information. Listing results previously displayed, the first evidence is the difference of age and duration of relationship between Italian and Belgian participants. However, our statistical analyses with “age” as a covariate showed that the difference of age between the two groups did not explain the differences we observed between the groups. In regards of this absence of age effect, it is necessary to analyze this result in the light of the Italian socio-demographic context. As a matter of fact, according to an ISTAT report (?), Italy is characterized by a low and belated fertility, as the mean age of the first childbirth is over the age of 30. Thus, this difference between Belgian and Italian participants could illustrate the situation of couples who start to express their first desire for parenthood.

According to literature (Patterson, 2000; D’Amore et al., 2013), our preliminary analysis confirmed that there were no differences between lesbian, gay and heterosexual couples in terms of co-parental alliance. The only significant difference regards the Structure of Play, which evaluates the compliance to play by the game rules rather than expressing proper parental skills. The “Structure of Play” is also a variable that seems to be linked to the country of residence unlike the others LTP variables (Simonelli et al., 2012; D’Amore et al., 2013). The differences highlighted in our study appear to focus on the quality of the couple relationship, which varies from one group to another. Indeed, it seems that Italian lesbian couples show a higher level of dyadic adjustment than heterosexual and gay couples; this result confirms the data emerged from literature (Farr and Patterson, 2013). The degree of internalized homophobia of Italian participants is significantly lower than for the Belgian ones. A previous study (Lorenzi et al., 2015) already showed that Belgian lesbians and gay men express more internalized homophobia, despite Belgians live in a country with a more favorite legal environment for LG rights. Besides, the same study demonstrated that internalized homophobia had a strong relation with mental health disorders, such as anxiety and depression. Thus, it is an important variable to be evaluated, as it could be an indirect factor that could affect parental skills and then co-parental behaviors. In addition, from the beginning of the transition to parenthood, both of LG future parents have to face several stressful situations both inside their family (e.g., who the biological parent will be between them in case of artificial techniques), and out of it (e.g., the context in which they live in). They would not have the same custodial rights on their child, unlike heterosexual parents. Focusing on correlations, high levels of couple adjustment lead to better parental performances among both Italian and Belgian couples. At the same time, more social support helps couples to carry out the task with happiness rather than boredom. These positive influences correspond to Belsky’s (1984) theory about the multi-factorial vision of parenting. The incoherence among correlations by country is highlighted by the opposite influence that internalized homophobia has on parental alliance: in Belgium, better performances are characterized by higher levels of internalized homophobia, even if it is a risk factor for several negative issues. It seems that Belgian LG couples with high internalized homophobia have more cooperative parental behaviors and better adjustment to the game. It is possible to claim, without generalizing (because of the sample size), that Belgian LG couples with a high level of internalized homophobia invest themselves in their own relationship and they strictly comply with the LTP roles and rules to offer the best performance. On the contrary, Italian performances seem to confirm positive patterns between a low level of internalized homophobia and parental behaviors. These discordances between Belgian and Italian participants may also be explained by their difference of age. Indeed, as Belgian participants were younger, we can also assume that they were less mature and more stereotypical than Italian participants. Further research should evaluate if the link between internalized homophobia and parental alliance persists when Belgians get older and when couples become actual parents.

Besides, this study adds information about the negative influence of the internalized homophobia on co-parental alliance. Even if they must be carefully read into, these results are of great interest: they suggest that sexual stigma has an impact on the capability of managing co-parenting. Indeed, internalized homophobia is a mandatory step that every gay and lesbian person has to face across the construction of his/her own identity (Montano, 2007). The identity component denotes the attitude to express a negative consideration of oneself as gay or lesbian, and it is important to assess how much this passage is overrun during the evaluation of parental skills among LG couples.

This study showed that the civil rights for Belgian LGBT people seem not to be enough to ensure their well-being; homophobia and sexual stigma are still often expressed in an implicit, subtle and indirect way. Our data offer some valuable indications about implications of internalized homophobia in the process of the identity construction and of the impact on the inter-generational transmission of psychopathology. As a matter of fact, the identity dimension of the MISS-LG, which “corresponds to an enduring propensity to have a negative self-attitude as homosexual and to consider sexual stigma as a part of a value system and identity” (Lingiardi et al., 2012, p. 1196), correlates with lower levels of co-parental alliance. This suggests that an individual, with a negative self-esteem due to her/his homosexuality, may find it difficult to manage the role of parent, because he/she always considers that role through a heteronormative perspective.

These themes need a further and closer examination, in particular in these early periods of lifecycle and with a strong collaboration among the countries.

Ethics Statement

Ethics committee of “Faculté de Psychologie, Logopédie et Sciences de l’Education” University of Liege. Project accepted the 10/31/2013. Participants sign a consent about the research protocol explaining the project details as well as the relative finalities, nature of data required, information’s about participants rights.

Author Contributions

MM: Study conception and design, data collection and analysis, interpretation of results and write up. AB: Data analysis, interpretation of results and write up. PP: Data collection, interpretation of results and write up. AS: Study conception and design, data analysis, interpretation of results and write up.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This paper is published with the support of the Belgian University Foundation. Publié avec le concours de la Fondation Universitaire de Belgique. A special thanks to Prof. JM Gauthier for supporting this project.

Footnotes

  1. ^This research has obtained the approval of the Ethics Committee of the University of Liège (Project accepted the 10/31/2013).
  2. ^Service de Psychologie clinique de l’enfant et de l’adolescent, University of Liège and Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua.
  3. ^Sexuality: The sexuality dimension describes the pessimistic evaluation of intimate gay or lesbian relationships’ quality and duration and a negative conception of gay or lesbian sexual behaviors (e.g., “I don’t believe in love between homosexuals”; “When I have sex with a woman, I feel awkward”). Identity: The identity dimension corresponds to an enduring propensity to have a negative self-attitude as homosexual and to consider sexual stigma as a part of a value system and identity (self-stigma; e.g., “I’d prefer to be heterosexual”; “If it were possible, I’d do anything to change my sexual orientation”). Social Discomfort: Social discomfort is the fear of public identification as a lesbian or gay man in the social context, disclosure in private and professional life, and negative internalized beliefs regarding religious, moral, and political acceptability of homosexuality (e.g., “At University (and/or at work), I pretend to be heterosexual”; “It’s difficult for me to say that I’m lesbian/gay, even to someone I know”).

References

Anderssen, N., Amlie, C., and Ytterøy, E. A. (2002). Outcomes for children with lesbian or gay parents. A review of studies from 1978 to 2000. Scand. J. Psychol. 43, 335–351. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev. 55, 83–96. doi: 10.2307/1129836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bos, H. M., Van Balen, F., and Van Den Boom, D. C. (2004). Experience of parenthood, couple relationship, social support, and child-rearing goals in planned lesbian mother families. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 755–764. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00269.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bos, H. M., Van Balen, F., and Van Den Boom, D. C. (2007). Child adjustment and parenting in planned lesbian-parent families. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 77, 38–48. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.1.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewaeys, A., and Van Hall, E. V. (1997). Lesbian motherhood: the impact on child development and family functioning. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 18, 1–16. doi: 10.3109/01674829709085563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carneiro, C., Corboz-Warnery, A., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (2006). The prenatal Lausanne Trilogue Play: a new observational assessment tool of the prenatal co-parenting alliance. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 207–228. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, R. W., Brooks, R. C., Raboy, B., and Patterson, C. J. (1998). Division of labor among lesbian and heterosexual parents: associations with children’s adjustment. J. Fam. Psychol. 12, 402–419. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.12.3.402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciano-Boyce, C., and Shelley-Sireci, L. (2002). Who is mommy tonight? Lesbian parenting issues. J. Homosex. 43, 1–13. doi: 10.1300/J082v43n02_01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Hoberman, H. M. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 13, 99–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1983.tb02325.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coltrane, S. (2000). Research on household labor: modeling and measuring the social embeddedness of routine family work. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 1208–1233. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01208.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corboz-Warnery, A., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, É (2001). Du Couple à la Famille: L’alliance Parentale Prénatale Annonce-t-elle le Devenir de la Famille? Louvain-la-Neuve: De Boeck Supérieur. doi: 10.3917/ctf.027.0017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, P. A., and McHale, J. P. (1996). Coparenting in a family context: emerging achievements, current dilemmas, and future directions. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 74, 93–106. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219967408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Amore, S., Simonelli, A., and Miscioscia, M. (2013). La qualità delle interazioni triadiche nelle famiglie lesbo-genitoriali: uno studio pilota con la procedura del Lausanne Trilogue Play. Infanz. Adolesc. 12, 113–127. doi: 10.1710/1328.14701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R., Rendina, H. J., Sinclair, K. O., and Grossman, A. H. (2007). Lesbian and gay youth’s aspirations for marriage and raising children. J. LGBT Issues Couns. 1, 77–98. doi: 10.1300/J462v01n04_06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delmore-Ko, P., Pancer, S. M., Hunsberger, B., and Pratt, M. (2000). Becoming a parent: the relation between prenatal expectations and postnatal experience. J. Fam. Psychol. 14, 625–640. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.4.625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, P. J. (1989). Lesbian mothers: psychosocial assumptions in family law. Am. Psychol. 44, 941–947. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.6.941

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farr, R. H., and Patterson, C. J. (2013). Coparenting among lesbian, gay, and heterosexual couples: associations with adopted children’s outcomes. Child Dev. 84, 1226–1240. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favez, N., Frascarolo, F., Carneiro, C., Montfort, V., Corboz-Warnery, A., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (2006). The development of the family alliance from pregnancy to toddlerhood and children outcomes at 18 months. Infant Child Dev. 15, 59–73. doi: 10.1002/icd.430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favez, N., Lopes, F., Bernard, M., Frascarolo, F., Lavanchy Scaiola, C., Corboz-Warnery, A., et al. (2012). The development of family alliance from pregnancy to toddlerhood and child outcomes at 5 years. Fam. Process 51, 542–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01419.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, R. C., Stanford, J. B., Jameson, P., and DeWitt, M. J. (1999). Exploring the concepts of intended, planned and wanted pregnancy. J. Fam. Pract. 48, 117–122.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fivaz-Depeursinge, E., and Corboz-Warnery, A. (1999). The Primary Triangle: A Developmental Systems View of Mothers, Fathers, and Infants. New York, NY: Basic books.

Google Scholar

Frascarolo, F., Favez, N., and Lavanchy Scaiola, C. (2008). “The development of family alliance and inner states understanding at age five,” in Proceedings of the 20th Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development: Family Relations. A Developmental Context for Theory of Mind and Emotion Understanding in Preschoolers, ed. N. Favez (Würzburg: International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development).

Google Scholar

Gates, G. J., Badgett, M. V. L., Macomber, J. E., and Chambers, K. (2007). Adoption and Foster Care by Gay and Lesbian Parents in the United States. Los Angeles, CA: The William Institute, University of California.

Google Scholar

Gatta, M., Balottin, L., Mannarini, M., Chesani, G., Del Col, L., Spoto, A., et al. (2016b). Familial factors relating to alexithymic traits in adolescents with psychiatric disorders. Clin. Psychol. 1–11. doi: 10.1111/cp.12098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatta, M., Sisti, M., Sudati, L., Miscioscia, M., and Simonelli, A. (2016a). The Lausanne Trilogue Play within the outcome evaluation in infant mental health: a preliminary report. Res. Psychother. 19, 19–30. doi: 10.4081/ripppo.2016.198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentili, P., Contreras, L., Cassaniti, M., and D’Arista, F. (2002). La Dyadic adjustment scale: una misura dell’adattamento di coppia. Minerva Psichiatr. 43, 107–116.

Google Scholar

Goldberg, A. E. (2010). Lesbian and Gay Parents and Their Children: Research on the Family Life Cycle. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Hoffman, L. W., and Manis, J. D. (1979). The value of children in the United States: a new approach to the study of fertility. J. Marriage Fam. 41, 583–596. doi: 10.2307/351628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

International Lesbian and Gay Association [ILGA] Europe (2014). ILGA-Europe Annual Review of the Human Rights Situation of LGBTI People in Europe. Available at: http://www.certidiritti.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/05/Annual-Review-2014-web-version.pdf

]BR29147 Italian National Institute of Statistics [ISTAT] (2015). Natalitá e Feconditaá della Popolazione Residente. Available at: https://www.istat.it/it/files/2015/11/Natalità_fecondita_2014.pdf?title=Natalità+e+fecondità+-+27%2Fnov%2F2015aaaTesto+integrale.pdf

Johnson, S., and O’Connor, E. (2002). The Gay Baby Boom: The Psychology of Gay Parenthood. New York, NY: New York University Press.

Google Scholar

Katz, L. F., and Gottman, J. M. (1996). Spillover effects of marital conflict: in search of parenting and co-parenting mechanisms. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 74, 57–76. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219967406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebovici, S. (1988). Fantasmatic interaction and intergenerational transmission. Infant Ment. Health J. 9, 10–19. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(198821)9:1<10::AID-IMHJ2280090104>3.0.CO;2-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lingiardi, V., Baiocco, R., and Nardelli, N. (2012). Measure of internalized sexual stigma for lesbians and gay men: a new scale. J. Homosex. 59, 1191–1210. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2012.712850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi, G., Miscioscia, M., Ronconi, L., Pasquali, C. E., and Simonelli, A. (2015). Internalized Stigma and Psychological Well-Being in Gay Men and Lesbians in Italy and Belgium. Soc. Sci. 4, 1229–1242. doi: 10.3390/socsci4041229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHale, J., Kuersten-Hogan, R., and Lauretti, A. (2001). “Evaluating co-parenting and family-level dynamics during infancy and early childhood: the co-parenting and family rating scale,” in Family Observation Coding Systems. Resources for Systemic Research, eds P. K. King and K. M. Lindhal (London: Erlbaum), 151–170.

McHale, J. P. (2007). When infants grow up in multiperson relationship systems. Infant Ment. Health J. 28, 370–392. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHale, J. P., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (1999). Understanding triadic and family group interactions during infancy and toddlerhood. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2, 107–127. doi: 10.1023/A:1021847714749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minuchin, P., and Minuchin, S. (1987). “Family as the context for patient care,” in Primary Care in the Home, eds L. Bernstein, A. Grieco, and M. Dete (Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & CO.), 83–94.

Google Scholar

Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and Family Therapy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Montano, A. (2007). L’omofobia interiorizzata come problema centrale del processo di formazione dell’identità omosessuale. Riv. Sessuol. 31, 25–33.

Google Scholar

Moretti, M., Simonelli, A., Melloni, M., and Ronconi, L. (2012). Interpersonal Support Evalutation List (ISEL): un contributo alla validazione e applicazione nel contesto italiano. Psicol. Soc. 3, 447–469.

Papoušek, H., and Papoušek, M. (1987). “Intuitive parenting: a dialectic counterpart to the infant’s integrative competence,” in Handbook of Infant Development, ed. J. D. Osofsky (Oxford: John Wiley & Sons), 669–720.

Google Scholar

Parks, C. A. (1998). Lesbian parenthood: a review of the literature. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 68, 376–389. doi: 10.1037/h0080347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, C. J. (1995a). Families of the baby boom: parents’ division of labor and children’s adjustment. Dev. Psychol. 31, 115–123. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.1.115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, C. J. (1995b). Lesbian Mothers, Gay Fathers, and Their Children. Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities Over the Lifespan: Psychological Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Patterson, C. J. (2000). Family relationships of lesbians and gay men. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 1052–1069. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01052.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, C. J. (2005). Lesbian and Gay Parents and Their Children: Summary of Research Findings. Lesbian and Gay Parenting. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 5–23.

Patterson, C. J., and Chan, R. W. (1996). “Gay fathers and their children,” in Textbook of Homosexuality and Mental Health, eds R. P. Cabaj and T. S. Stein (Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association), 371–393.

Google Scholar

Peplau, L. A., and Fingerhut, A. W. (2007). The close relationships of lesbians and gay men. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 405–424. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrin, E. C. (2002). Sexual Orientation in Children and Adolescents: Implications for Health Care. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic Plenum.

Google Scholar

Riskind, R. G., and Patterson, C. J. (2010). Parenting intentions and desires among childless lesbian, gay, and heterosexual individuals. J. Fam. Psychol. 24, 78–81. doi: 10.1037/a0017941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonelli, A., Bighin, M., and De Palo, F. (2012). Coparenting interactions observed by the prenatal lausanne trilogue play: an Italian replication study. Infant Ment. Health J. 33, 609–619. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommer, K., Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Schellenbach, C., Maxwell, S., and Keogh, D. (1993). Cognitive readiness and adolescent parenting. Dev. Psychol. 29, 389–398. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.2.389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38, 15–28. doi: 10.2307/350547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stacey, J., and Biblarz, T. J. (2001). (How) does the sexual orientation of parents matter? Am. Sociol. Rev. 66, 159–183. doi: 10.2307/2657413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, A. (1996). Virtually Normal: An Argument about Homosexuality. New York, NY: Random House, Inc.

Google Scholar

Tasker, F. (1999). Children in lesbian-led families: a review. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 4, 153–166. doi: 10.1177/1359104599004002003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasker, F. (2005). Lesbian mothers, gay fathers, and their children: a review. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 26, 224–240. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200506000-00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasker, F. L., and Golombok, S. (1998). Growing Up in a Lesbian Family: Effects on Child Development. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Thornton, A. (1978). Marital dissolution, remarriage, and childbearing. Demography 15, 361–380. doi: 10.2307/2060656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandeleur, C. L., Fenton, B. T., Ferrero, F., and Preisig, M. (2003). Construct validity of the French version of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Swiss J. Psychol. 62, 167–175. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.62.3.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanfraussen, K., Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, I., and Brewaeys, A. (2003). Family functioning in lesbian families created by donor insemination. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 73, 78–90. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.73.1.78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, S. B., and Fish, M. C. (1995). Lesbian mothers and the children: a review for school psychologists. School Psychol. Rev. 24, 456–479.

Google Scholar

Westoff, L. A. (1977). The Second Time Around: Remarriage in America. New York, NY: Viking Adult.

Google Scholar

Wilson, E. K., and Koo, H. P. (2006). The relationship context: its effects on low-income women’s desire for a baby. J. Marriage Fam. 68, 1326–1340. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00331.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: parenting desires/intentions, same-sex couples, co-parenting, dyadic adjustment, Prenatal Lausanne Trilogue Play

Citation: Miscioscia M, Blavier A, Pagone PR and Simonelli A (2017) The Desire of Parenthood: Intuitive Co-parental Behaviors and Quality of Couple Relationship among Italian and Belgian Same-Sex and Opposite-Sex Couples. Front. Psychol. 8:110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00110

Received: 21 July 2016; Accepted: 16 January 2017;
Published: 14 February 2017.

Edited by:

Chris J. Gibbons, University of Cambridge, UK

Reviewed by:

Douglas L. Delahanty, Kent State University, USA
Michelle Dow Keawphalouk, Harvard University and Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), USA

Copyright © 2017 Miscioscia, Blavier, Pagone and Simonelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marina Miscioscia, bWFyaW5hLm1pc2Npb3NjaWFAdW5pcGQuaXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.