Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 October 2016
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Personality and cognition in economic decision making View all 20 articles

Creativity and Cognitive Skills among Millennials: Thinking Too Much and Creating Too Little

  • 1EMLYON Business School, Univ Lyon, GATE L-SE UMR 5824, Ecully, France
  • 2Economics Department, Middlesex University Business School, London, UK
  • 3Granada Lab of Behavioral Economics, Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 4Business School, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK

Organizations crucially need the creative talent of millennials but are reluctant to hire them because of their supposed lack of diligence. Recent studies have shown that hiring diligent millennials requires selecting those who score high on the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) and thus rely on effortful thinking rather than intuition. A central question is to assess whether the push for recruiting diligent millennials using criteria such as cognitive reflection can ultimately hamper the recruitment of creative workers. To answer this question, we study the relationship between millennials' creativity and their performance on fluid intelligence (Raven) and cognitive reflection (CRT) tests. The good news for recruiters is that we report, in line with previous research, evidence of a positive relationship of fluid intelligence, and to a lesser extent cognitive reflection, with convergent creative thinking. In addition, we observe a positive effect of fluid intelligence on originality and elaboration measures of divergent creative thinking. The bad news for recruiters is the inverted U-shape relationship between cognitive reflection and fluency and flexibility measures of divergent creative thinking. This suggests that thinking too much may hinder important dimensions of creative thinking. Diligent and creative workers may thus be a rare find.

Introduction

Evidence from a recent survey reports that managers are three times more likely to hire a mature worker than to hire a millennial (born between 1980 and 2000; Rainer and Rainer, 2011) despite desperately needing their creative talent1. Mature workers are appealing to recruiters because they are seen as more reliable and more committed than millennials. The dilemma for managers is thus to hire millennials that are both diligent and creative.

Recent studies have shown that firms can secure the hiring of diligent millennials by relying on measures of cognitive skills. For example, intelligence has been found to be the main predictor of overall work performance in a wide variety of occupations and across age and gender (e.g., Hunter and Hunter, 1984; Olea and Ree, 1994; see Schmidt, 2009 for a review). Standard measures of cognitive ability have been found to correlate positively with task performance (Schmidt et al., 1986; Murphy, 1989) and negatively with counterproductive work behaviors such as theft or absenteeism (Dilchert et al., 2007). Moreover, the results of a recent study suggest that these effects may be mediated by individuals' cognitive styles (Corgnet et al., 2015b). In particular, Corgnet et al. (2015b) find that millennials characterized by a more reflective style (as measured by the Cognitive Reflection Test; Frederick, 2005) are more diligent, displaying higher levels of task performance and lower levels of counterproductive work behaviors2. A crucial caveat is whether hiring millennials based on cognitive measures may ultimately select less creative workers. To address this point we need to assess the relationship between cognitive skills and creativity.

Traditionally, intelligence, and creativity have been considered to be unrelated (Getzels and Jackson, 1962; Wallach and Kogan, 1965; Batey and Furnham, 2006; Sawyer, 2006; Weisberg, 2006; Runco, 2007; Kaufman, 2009; Kim et al., 2010). In a meta-analysis, Kim (2005) finds that the correlation between creativity test scores and IQ varies widely and is, on average, small (r = 0.174).

However, a growing consensus has emerged in recent research stressing a close relationship between intelligence and creative performance (see Silvia, 2015, for a review). This emerging consensus heavily relies on recent studies that have employed more sophisticated statistical techniques and more robust assessment methods than prior research on the topic. For example, the use of latent variable models has allowed researchers to uncover a positive and significant relationship between creativity and intelligence using data from previous studies that reported non-significant correlations (Silvia, 2008b). The recent wave of research on intelligence and creativity has also improved upon traditional assessment of creativity that exclusively relied on scoring methods based on the originality and uniqueness of responses in creative tasks (such as finding unusual uses for an object). These traditional scoring methods are imprecise because they confound several factors, such as fluency and sample size (Hocevar, 1979; Silvia et al., 2008), and can thus lead to inaccurate estimates of the relationship between intelligence and creativity (Silvia, 2008a; Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011). The results of this new wave of research on creativity and intelligence have been taken as evidence that executive cognition is undoubtedly beneficial to creative thinking (Silvia, 2015).

Yet, although there is an obvious link between intelligence and executive cognition, from the point of view of modern dual-process theory (Evans, 2008, 2009; Stanovich, 2009, 2010; Evans and Stanovich, 2013), one should distinguish between algorithmic and reflective cognitive processes. Algorithmic processes are typically associated with computational efficiency and are measured by standard intelligence tests whereas reflective processing is associated with a disposition to employ the resources of the algorithmic mind, that is, to switch from autonomous “Type 1” thought to analytic “Type 2” (working memory-dependent) thought. The reflective mind thus has a disposition-based definition (“cognitive styles”, reflective vs. intuitive) and is not adequately measured by standard intelligence tests (which assess “cognitive ability”) but by tasks of cognitive reflection like the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Frederick, 2005). Individuals characterized by a more reflective mind tend to show higher levels of self-control and lower levels of “cognitive impulsivity” (Frederick, 2005; Kahneman and Frederick, 2007; Cokely and Kelley, 2009; Oechssler et al., 2009; Toplak et al., 2011; Brañas-Garza et al., 2012).

From this perspective, one can conjecture that cognitive reflection may relate negatively to creativity. This is the case because a number of studies suggest that the capacity to control one's attention and behavior may even be detrimental for creative thinking (for a review, see Wiley and Jarosz, 2012a). For example, creative problem solving has been shown to relate positively to moderate alcohol intoxication (Jarosz et al., 2012), which is known to impair inhibition and attentional control (Peterson et al., 1990; Kovacevic et al., 2012; Marinkovic et al., 2012). Similarly, an “experiential” thinking style (which maps onto Type 1 processing) has been found to correlate positively with creative performance (Norris and Epstein, 2011).

As mentioned, past literature arrived at conflicting conclusions regarding whether executive cognition favors (e.g., Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011; Beaty and Silvia, 2012; Silvia, 2015) or hampers (e.g., Eysenck, 1993; Kim et al., 2007; Ricks et al., 2007; Norris and Epstein, 2011; Jarosz et al., 2012; Wiley and Jarosz, 2012b) creative thinking. Dual-process theory can reconcile these apparently conflicting findings by positing that creativity may be generated by a mix of Type 1 and Type 2 processes (Allen and Thomas, 2011; Ball et al., 2015; Barr et al., 2015; see Sowden et al., 2015, for a review). It follows that the dual-process approach lays out a promising research agenda based on assessing the exact mix of Type 1 and Type 2 processes that bolsters creativity as well as analyzing separately the effect of algorithmic and reflective Type 2 processes on creative thinking.

Following a dual-process approach, Barr et al. (2015) find experimental evidence of an important effect of controlled Type 2 analytic processes on both convergent and divergent (Guilford, 1967) creative thinking. In particular, they find that both cognitive ability (measured as the combination of numeracy and verbal skills) and reflective cognitive style (average of scores in the CRT and base-rate problem tasks) covary positively with one's capacity to make remote associations, that is, with convergent creative thinking. Regarding divergent creative thinking, Barr et al. (2015) show that cognitive ability but not cognitive reflection predicts higher originality scores in an alternate uses task. Fluency in the latter task, however, was not correlated with either cognitive measure.

In this paper, we use a similar approach to Barr et al. (2015) and investigate how both types of cognitive processes affect creativity. In particular, we analyze how cognitive abilities (measured using Raven as a test of fluid intelligence) and cognitive styles (intuitive vs. reflective; as measured by the CRT) relate to convergent and divergent creative thinking. We extend Barr et al. (2015) by analyzing other measures of divergent thinking such as flexibility and elaboration and by exploring possible non-linearities between creativity and cognitive measures.

Given the conflicting results regarding whether executive cognition is beneficial or detrimental for creative thinking, we conjecture that there might exist a non-linear relationship between different measures of creativity and cognition. Specifically, it might be that a minimum level of executive cognition is necessary for creative performance but, beyond some level, the relationship disappears or even turns negative. This might explain why previous findings seem to be inconsistent. A related line of reasoning has been proposed in the so-called “threshold hypothesis” of the relationship between IQ and creativity (Guilford, 1967; Jauk et al., 2013). The threshold hypothesis states that intelligence is positively related to creative thinking for low IQ levels but the relationship blurs for high IQ levels. Similar arguments arise in recent accounts of the “mad genius hypothesis”: moderate levels of inhibitory or top-down control dysfunction, characteristic of subclinical psychiatric populations (e.g., mild ADHD and schizophrenia disorders), can spur creativity under some conditions whereas clinical-severe levels typically lead to impoverished creative thinking (Schuldberg, 2005; Abraham et al., 2007; Jaracz et al., 2012; Acar and Sen, 2013; Abraham, 2014).

Methods

Participants and General Protocol

Participants were 150 students (46.67% female; age: mean ± SD = 20.23 ± 1.96) from Chapman University in the U.S. These participants were recruited from a database of more than 2000 students. Invitations to participate in the current study were sent to a random subset of the whole database. This study is part of a larger research program on cognition and economic decision making. The local Institutional Review Board approved of this research. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participating. We conducted a total of 12 sessions, nine had 12 participants and three had 14 participants. On average, sessions lasted for 45 min. All subjects completed the same tasks in the following order: (1) CRT, (2) Raven test, (3) Remote associates task, (4) Alternate uses task. Subjects had 6 min to complete each task and a 2-min break after completing the Raven test.

Measures

Cognitive Ability Assessment

Participants completed a subset of Raven progressive matrices test (Raven, 1936). Specifically, we used the odd number of the last three series of matrices (Jaeggi et al., 2010; Corgnet et al., 2015a). The number of matrices correctly solved in the Raven test (in our sample, ranging from 9 to 18, mean ± SD = 14.40 ± 2.42 for males and 14.47 ± 2.16 for females) is a conventional measure of cognitive ability. This test captures an important aspect of cognitive processing which is referred to as fluid intelligence and is closely related to algorithmic thinking (Stanovich, 2009, 2010).

Cognitive Style Assessment

We measured the participants' tendency to rely on intuition vs. reflection using the CRT introduced by Frederick (2005). The test is characterized by the existence of an incorrect response which automatically comes to mind but has to be overridden in order to find the correct solution. To the original CRT questions, we added four questions recently developed by Toplak et al. (2014). This extended task (see Text S1) will allow us to uncover potentially non-linear relationships that would be hard to observe using the classical three-item task (Frederick, 2005). In Table S1, we display the proportion of subjects answering each question correctly, split by gender. As expected, males performed better in the test than females (Frederick, 2005; Bosch-Domènech et al., 2014). Our measure of cognitive reflection is given by the total number of correct answers (from 0 to 7). The full distribution of correct answers by males (mean ± SD = 4.09 ± 2.31) and females (mean ± SD = 2.89 ± 2.03) is provided in Figure S1.

Convergent Creative Thinking

We used a subset of the Remote Associate Test (RAT; Mednick, 1962) to measure subjects' ability to make remote associations. In particular, subjects were shown 13 sets of three words (e.g., widow-bite-monkey) and asked to find a word which relates to all the three words provided (in this example the solution is “spider”). Our measure of convergent thinking is the number of problems correctly solved (from 0 to 13).

Divergent Creative Thinking

We measured divergent thinking using a variant of the Alternate Uses Task (AUT; Guilford, 1967). Participants were instructed to provide as many unusual uses of a pen as possible during 6 min. We construct four different measures of divergent thinking: fluency, originality, flexibility, and elaboration. We measured fluency as the total number of answers provided by a participant. Three raters were presented with a random list of answers and asked to score the degree of originality of each entry using a 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much) Likert scale. We computed originality as the sum of the average score of the three raters for all the entries provided by a participant, divided by the total number of answers. Following Troyer and Moscovitch (2006) and Gilhooly et al. (2007), all the answers were classified in broad differentiated categories (e.g., uses of the pen as cloth or hair accessories). Then, flexibility was measured as the number of different categories provided by each participant. Finally, elaboration refers to the average amount of detail (from 0 to 2) provided by each participant.

Statistical Analysis

For the data analysis, we start by showing the descriptive statistics of all the measures used and their zero-order correlations. To further assess the relationships between creativity and cognitive measures, we first provide a graphical representation using LOWESS smoothing (Cleveland, 1979; Cleveland and McGill, 1985). We then run ordinary least squares regressions which allow us to test the statistical significance of the linear and non-linear relationships which were shown in the LOWESS graphs. All the analyses were performed using Stata 14.0.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Means, standard deviations, and correlations are shown in Table 1. Unsurprisingly, we find moderate positive correlation between the number of correct answers in the CRT and Raven tests (r = 0.26, p < 0.01) which suggests that CRT and Raven are not entirely measuring the same cognitive skills (Frederick, 2005; Stanovich, 2009, 2010). Similarly, the different measures of divergent thinking (AUT) are significantly correlated (all p's < 0.01), except for originality and flexibility (p = 0.28).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations.

Regarding our cognitive measures, we find that both Raven (p < 0.01) and CRT scores (p = 0.03) are positively correlated with convergent thinking (RAT). However, the relationship between cognitive skills and divergent thinking is more complicated. High levels of cognitive ability (Raven) relate positively with originality (p = 0.01) and elaboration (p < 0.01), but negatively with the number of answers provided (fluency; p = 0.04) and non-correlated with flexibility (p = 0.20). Finally, we do not find a significant correlation between cognitive styles (CRT scores) and any measure of divergent thinking (all p's > 0.26).

Non-linear Effects and Regression Analysis

We now turn to the study of possible non-linear relationships between our measures of cognition and creativity. Figure 1 displays all the relationships under study using LOWESS (bandwidth = 0.8; Cleveland, 1979; Cleveland and McGill, 1985). LOWESS is a model-free smoothing technique based on locally-weighted regressions which can detect both linear and non-linear relationships. In order to compare the effect sizes, we standardize all measures (standard deviations from the mean). We also ran ordinary least squares regressions to assess the statistical significance of the observed relationships. In Tables S2–S6, we present the results of a series of regressions in which we estimated both linear and quadratic effects of each of the predictors (Raven and CRT) separately on each creativity measure (columns [1] to [4]). From these regressions, we selected the models with the best fit, either linear or quadratic in each case, using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and report them in summary Table 2. In addition, we ran similar regressions in which both predictors (linear and quadratic terms) are included simultaneously (columns [5] and [6] in Tables S2–S6) in order to test for possible mediation or confounding effects. The interaction between CRT and Raven scores is never significant in predicting creativity (all p's > 0.3) and is thus not reported in the tables for the sake of brevity. The results remain qualitatively similar if we also control for gender and age.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Relationship between cognitive measures and creative thinking. The relationships are represented using locally weighted smoothing (LOWESS) techniques. All variables are standardized.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The effect of cognitive abilities and cognitive styles on creativity (best fitting models).

The models with the best fit (Table 2) report a positive linear relationship of convergent thinking (RAT) with both Raven (p < 0.01) and CRT scores (p = 0.03), which is consistent with the positive and significant correlations reported in the previous section. Effect sizes are substantial: in both cases, one SD increase in the predictor is associated with about 20% of one SD increase in RAT (0.22 and 0.17 for Raven and CRT, respectively; see coefficients in Table 2). Interestingly, the effect of Raven on RAT remains significant (p = 0.02) if we include both Raven and CRT scores as predictors (see column [5] in Table S2) whereas the effect of CRT becomes non-significant (p = 0.15). This result suggests that the significant effect of CRT scores on convergent thinking is driven more by cognitive ability (basic computational skills are also necessary for solving the CRT correctly) rather than by reflectiveness.

The relationship between our cognitive measures and divergent thinking is more complex. The models with the best fit report a linear and significant relationship between cognitive ability and all the measures of divergent thinking (all p's < 0.03), except for flexibility (p = 0.22; see Table 2). Subjects with a higher Raven score tend to generate less uses (lower fluency), although these are more elaborated and original. Again, for these three creativity measures, one SD increase in Raven produces a variation in the dependent variable of about 20% of one SD. The effect of Raven on flexibility appears to be slightly U-shaped in Figure 1 but the regressions do not report any significant linear or quadratic relationship (all p's > 0.22; see columns [1] and [2] in Table S5). As shown in columns [5] and [6] of Tables S3–S6, the effect of Raven on the divergent thinking measures remains virtually identical when controlling for CRT, which indicates that cognitive reflection does not mediate any of these relationships.

Contrary to the results observed with Raven, we do not find any significant linear relationship between cognitive styles and divergent thinking (all p's > 0.28; see column [3] in Tables S3–S6). These results hold when we control for Raven (all p's > 0.63; see column [5] in Tables S3–S6). However, we find a significant inverted U-shape relationship of CRT with both fluency and flexibility, as reported in Table 2 (p < 0.01 and p = 0.02, respectively). Subjects with an average level of cognitive reflection tend to produce more answers and use more categories than those subjects characterized by either a more intuitive or a more reflective cognitive style. Moreover, the fact that the coefficient of the linear term in the quadratic regression specification is not significantly different from zero in either case (p = 0.52 and p = 0.88, respectively) indicates that the maximum levels of fluency and flexibility are observed at the mean CRT score, as suggested by Figure 1. Effect sizes are comparable to those reported above insofar as, in both cases, moving one SD either above or below the mean CRT is associated with a decrease of about 20% of one SD in the dependent variable. Yet, the effects are larger for more extreme CRT values. Note that half of the observations fall outside the range mean ± one SD (see also Figure S1). Controlling for Raven does not alter these relationships (p = 0.01 and p = 0.02, respectively; see column [6] in Tables S4, S5), which again indicates an absence of mediation effects.

Discussion

The dual-process approach of cognition has been recently suggested to reconcile previous conflictive findings on the relationship between creativity and executive cognition (Allen and Thomas, 2011; Ball et al., 2015; Barr et al., 2015; Sowden et al., 2015). We contribute to this literature by differentiating between the algorithmic and reflective minds (Evans and Stanovich, 2013), and by analyzing their separate effects on convergent thinking and four different dimensions of divergent thinking. We partially replicate the results of Barr et al. (2015) by finding that individuals' ability to make remote associations correlates positively with cognitive ability and cognitive reflection. However, we find that this effect on convergent thinking is mainly driven by cognitive ability. Similarly to Barr et al. (2015), we also find that higher levels of cognitive ability are related with higher originality scores and lower fluency scores in divergent thinking. Unlike Barr et al. (2015), we also analyze non-linear effects and find an inverted U-shape relationship between cognitive reflection and our measures of flexibility and fluency on the divergent thinking task. These new results suggest that individuals who are highly deliberative may have a disadvantage in producing a large number of new and creative ideas.

Dual-process models of creativity suggest that both generative and evaluative processes interact during the creative process (Finke et al., 1992; Basadur, 1995; Howard-Jones, 2002; Gabora, 2005; Nijstad et al., 2010; Gabora and Ranjan, 2013). Although these models do not have a straightforward mapping onto dual-process models of cognition, the interaction between Type 1 and Type 2 cognitive processes may play a different role in different phases of the creative process. In this line, Sowden et al. (2015) call for future research “…to investigate the extent to which creativity is determined by the ability to shift between Type 1 and Type 2 thinking processes as a function of the circumstances and the stage of the creative processes” (p. 55). Our results suggest that cognitive reflection, that is the disposition to override automatic responses related to Type 1 processing and engage in Type 2 controlled thought, has a complex effect on divergent thinking. To some extent, cognitive reflection may be necessary to shift between the generative and evaluative processes involved in the production of new ideas. However, individuals characterized by high levels of reflection may be less able to rely on their intuitive, autonomous mind which can also be needed for unleashing one's creative power (e.g., Dorfman et al., 1996; Norris and Epstein, 2011; Jarosz et al., 2012).

The finding of an inverted U-shape relationship between cognitive reflection (and, analogously, intuitive processing) and creativity is consistent with recent advances on the “mad genius hypothesis”: mild levels of top-down control dysfunction may be beneficial for creativity but severe impairment leads to poor creative performance (for a review, see Abraham, 2014).

Relatedly, neuropsychological research has shown an inverted-U shape relationship between spontaneous eye blink rates and flexibility in divergent creative thinking tasks (Chermahini and Hommel, 2010). To the extent that eye blink rates reflect dopaminergic activity (Karson, 1983), which is in turn linked to inhibitory control (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992), our results are in line with the finding of Chermahini and Hommel (2010).

Beyond its connection to basic cognitive research, our findings offer insights to managers in search for the creative talent of millennials. One essential implication of our study is that thinking too much may hamper important aspects of divergent creative thinking. This result is of primary relevance to hiring managers who may want to rely on cognitive reflection as the main criterion to recruit diligent (Corgnet et al., 2015b) and creative millennials. Our findings suggest that the cognitive tests used to recruit workers have to be adapted to the nature of the job offered. For example, recruiting for jobs that fundamentally require finding well-defined solutions to problems (such as accounting or actuarial jobs) can rely on a mix of cognitive ability and reflection tests which are good predictors of convergent creative thinking and diligence. However, recruiting for jobs that mainly require divergent creative thinking (such as marketing, industrial design, or psychology jobs) should not solely rely on cognitive measures. Recruiting based on cognitive reflection skills may actually prevent the hire of highly creative workers. These recommendations are becoming increasingly relevant as a growing number of jobs in modern economies require divergent creative thinking (Pink, 2005).

The current research has some necessary limitations that future research might remedy. To keep focus our study uses only one measure of fluid intelligence (Raven) and a single measure of cognitive style (CRT). Future research may assess the robustness of our findings to other measures of fluid intelligence and cognitive style, possibly extending the analysis to include crystallized intelligence. Also, our sample consisted entirely of undergraduates, with a limited age, education, and income range. Although this was a methodological choice that allowed us to study the workforce of the future, further studies may assess the robustness of our findings to different populations. Regarding our creativity measures, future research may attempt to extend our analysis to the case of practical creative tasks that are commonly encountered, for example, at the workplace. To that end, future research may embed the study of creativity in an organizational setting that allows for studying the relationship between workplace problem solving and cognitive skills.

On a methodological note, we used a fixed ordering of which may have influenced the results as, among other factors, fatigue may interfere with test results. While the 2-min break in the middle of the experiment might have mitigated spillover effects between the first and the second part of the experiment, concerns still remain. We encourage future research to explore possible ordering effects. In addition, future research focusing on state-level analyses of the role of intuition vs. reflection in creative performance is necessary to assess the robustness (and causality) of our trait-level findings as well as deepen our understanding of the cognitive basis of creativity. Along these lines, it would be interesting for future research to test the effect of cognitive manipulations such as cognitive load, ego depletion, priming, or time pressure/delay on creative performance. Our findings suggest that future research on the topic should attempt to capture potentially non-linear effects thus elaborating experimental designs that allow such effects to materialize. This can be done, for example, by considering at least three levels per treatment condition.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The authors acknowledge financial support from the International Foundation for Research in Experimental Economics, the Argyros School of Business and Economics at Chapman University, the Spanish Ministry of Education [Grant 2012/00103/001], Ministry of Economy and Competence [2016/00122/001], Spanish Plan Nacional I+D MCI [ECO2013-44879-R], 2014-17, and Proyectos de Excelencia de la Junta Andalucía [P12.SEJ.1436], 2014-18.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01626

Footnotes

1. ^See the following press release: http://www.forbes.com/sites/susanadams/2012/09/24/older-workers-theres-hope-study-finds-employers-like-you-better-than-millennials/#1f5799cb4aa6 (accessed September 21, 2016).

2. ^Positive effects of cognitive reflection on people's willingness to choose socially-efficient resource allocations (Lohse, 2016; Capraro et al., 2016) as well as to trust strangers (Corgnet et al., 2016) suggest other possible channels through which organizations may benefit from hiring individuals with a more reflective cognitive style. Cognitive reflection has also been found to play a key role in moral judgment (e.g., Paxton et al., 2012; Pennycook et al., 2014).

References

Abraham, A. (2014). Is there an inverted-U relationship between creativity and psychopathology? Front. Psychol. 5:750. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abraham, A., Windmann, S., McKenna, P., and Güntürkün, O. (2007). Creative thinking in schizophrenia: the role of executive dysfunction and symptom severity. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 12, 235–258. doi: 10.1080/13546800601046714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acar, S., and Sen, S. (2013). A multilevel meta-analysis of the relationship between creativity and schizotypy. Psychol. Aesthetics Creativity Arts 7:214. doi: 10.1037/a0031975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, A. P., and Thomas, K. E. (2011). A dual process account of creative thinking. Creativity Res. J. 23, 109–118. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2011.571183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, L. J., Marsh, J. E., Litchfield, D., Cook, R. L., and Booth, N. (2015). When distraction helps: evidence that concurrent articulation and irrelevant speech can facilitate insight problem solving. Thinking Reasoning 21, 76–96. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2014.934399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, N., Pennycook, G., Stolz, J. A., and Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). Reasoned connections: a dual-process perspective on creative thought. Thinking Reasoning 21, 61–75. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2014.895915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basadur, M. S. (1995). Optimal ideation-evaluation ratios. Creativity Res. J. 8, 63–75. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj0801_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batey, M., and Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 132, 355–429. doi: 10.3200/MONO.132.4.355-430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., and Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychol. Aesthetics Creativity Arts 6:309. doi: 10.1037/a0029171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch-Domènech, A., Brañas-Garza, P., and Espín, A. M. (2014). Can exposure to prenatal sex hormones (2D: 4D) predict cognitive reflection? Psychoneuroendocrinology 43, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brañas-Garza, P., García-Muñoz, T., and Hernán-González, R. (2012). Cognitive effort in the beauty contest game. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 83, 254–260. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capraro, V., Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., and Hernán-González, R. (2016). Deliberation favors social efficiency by helping people disregard their relative shares: evidence from US and India. Available online at: SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract = 2799850

Chermahini, S. A., and Hommel, B. (2010). The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition 115, 458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleveland, W. S. (1979). Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 74, 829–836. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1979. 10481038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleveland, W. S., and McGill, R. (1985). Graphical perception and graphical methods for analyzing scientific data. Science 229, 828–833. doi: 10.1126/science.229.4716.828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. D., and Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol. Rev. 99, 45–77.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cokely, E. T., and Kelley, C. M. (2009). Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: a protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgment Decision Making 4:20.

Google Scholar

Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., and Hernán-González, R. (2015a). The cognitive basis of social behavior: cognitive reflection overrides antisocial but not always prosocial motives. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:287. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., Hernán-González, R., Kujal, P., and Rassenti, S. (2016). To trust, or not to trust: cognitive reflection in trust games. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 64, 20–27. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2015.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corgnet, B., Hernán-González, R., and Mateo, R. (2015b). Cognitive reflection and the diligent worker: an experimental study of millennials. PloS ONE 10:e0141243. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dilchert, S., Ones, D. S., Davis, R. D., and Rostow, C. D. (2007). Cognitive ability predicts objectively measured counterproductive work behaviors. J. Appl. Psychol. 92:616. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorfman, J., Shames, V. A., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). “Intuition, incubation, and insight: implicit cognition in problem solving,” in Implicit Cognition, ed G. Underwood (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 257–296.

Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 255–278. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, J. S. B. T. (2009). “How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many?” in In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond, eds Evans J. S. B. T. and K. Frankish (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 33–55.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Evans, J. S. B. T., and Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing the debate. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 223–241. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: suggestions for a theory. Psychol. Inquiry 4, 147–178. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0403_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., and Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative Cognition: Theory, Research and Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 19, 25–42. doi: 10.1257/089533005775196732

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabora, L. (2005). Creative thought as a non-Darwinian evolutionary process. J. Creat. Behav. 39, 262–283. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2005.tb01261.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabora, L., and Ranjan, A. (2013). “How insight emerges in distributed, content-addressable memory,” in The Neuroscience of Creativity, eds A. Bristol, O. Vartanian, and J. Kaufman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 19–44.

Getzels, J. W., and Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and Intelligence: Explorations with Gifted Students. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., and Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. Br. J. Psychol. 98, 611–625. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Hocevar, D. (1979). Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in the measurement of originality. J. Educ. Psychol. 71:191. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.71.2.191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard-Jones, P. A. (2002). A dual-state model of creative cognition for supporting strategies that foster creativity in the classroom. Int. J. Technol. Design Educ. 12, 215–226. doi: 10.1023/A:1020243429353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, J. E., and Hunter, R. F. (1984). Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job performance. Psychol. Bull. 96, 72–98. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.96.1.72

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y. F., Jonides, J., and Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning – implications for training and transfer. Intelligence 38, 625–635. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaracz, J., Patrzala, A., and Rybakowski, J. K. (2012). Creative thinking deficits in patients with schizophrenia: neurocognitive correlates. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 200, 588–593. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31825bfc49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarosz, A. F., Colflesh, G. J. H., and Wiley, J. (2012). Uncorking the muse: alcohol intoxication facilitates creative problem solving. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 487–493. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41, 212–221. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Frederick, S. (2007). Frames and brains: elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 45–46. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karson, C. N. (1983). Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain 106, 643–653.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kaufman, J. C. (2009). Creativity 101. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? A meta-analysis. J. Secondary Gifted Educ. 16, 57–66. doi: 10.4219/jsge-2005-473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H., Cramond, B., and VanTassel-Baska, J. (2010). “The relationship between creativity and intelligence,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, eds J. C. Kaufman and R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 395–412.

Kim, S., Hasher, L., and Zacks, R. T. (2007). Aging and a benefit of distractibility. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 14, 301–305. doi: 10.3758/BF03194068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovacevic, S., Azma, S., Irimia, A., Sherfey, J., Halgren, E., and Marinkovic, K. (2012). Theta oscillations are sensitive to both early and late conflict processing stages: effects of alcohol intoxication. PloS ONE 7:e43957. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohse, J. (2016). Smart or selfish–when smart guys finish nice. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 64, 28–40. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2016.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinkovic, K., Rickenbacher, E., Azma, S., and Artsy, E. (2012). Acute alcohol intoxication impairs top–down regulation of stroop incongruity as revealed by blood oxygen level?dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Map. 33, 319–333. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychol. Rev. 69, 220. doi: 10.1037/h0048850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, K. R. (1989). Is the relationship between cognitive ability and job performance stable over time? Human Perform. 2, 183–200. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup0203_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., and Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 21, 34–77. doi: 10.1080/10463281003765323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norris, P., and Epstein, S. (2011). An experiential thinking style: its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. J. Personal. 79, 1043–1080. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00718.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nusbaum, E. C., and Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence 39, 36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oechssler, J., Roider, A., and Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 72, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olea, M. M., and Ree, M. J. (1994). Predicting pilot and navigator criteria: not much more than g. J. Appl. Psychol. 79, 845–851. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxton, J. M., Unger, L., and Greene, J. D. (2012). Reflection and reasoning in moral judgement. Cogn. Sci. 36, 163–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01210.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., and Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking Reasoning 20, 188–214. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.865000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, J. B., Rothfleisch, J., Zelazo, P. D., and Pihl, R. O. (1990). Acute alcohol intoxication and cognitive functioning. J. Stud. Alcohol 51, 114–122. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1990.51.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pink, D. (2005). A Whole New Mind. New York City, NY: Riverhead Books.

Rainer, T. S., and Rainer, J. W. (2011). The Millennials: Connecting to America's Largest Generation. Nashville, TN: BandH Publishing Group.

Google Scholar

Raven, J. C. (1936). Mental Tests used in Genetic Studies: The Performances of Related Individuals in Tests Mainly Educative and Mainly Reproductive. Unpublished master's thesis, University of London.

Ricks, T. R., Turley-Ames, K. J., and Wiley, J. (2007). Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge. Mem. Cogn. 35, 1456–1462. doi: 10.3758/BF03193615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity. San Diego, CA: Acade.

Google Scholar

Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, F. L. (2009). “Select on intelligence,” in Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior: Indispensable Knowledge for Evidence-Based Management, ed E. A. Locke (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd.), 3–18.

Schmidt, F. L., Hunter, J. E., and Outerbridge, A. N. (1986). Impact of job experience and ability on job knowledge, work sample performance, and supervisory ratings of job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 71:432. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.71.3.432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuldberg, D. (2005). Eysenck Personality Questionnaire scales and paper-and-pencil tests related to creativity. Psychol. Rep. 97, 180–182. doi: 10.2466/pr0.97.5.180-182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2008a). Another look at creativity and intelligence: exploring higher-order models and probable confounds. Pers. Individ. Diff. 44, 1012–1021. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2008b). Creativity and intelligence revisited: a latent variable analysis of Wallach and Kogan (1965). Creativity Res. J. 20, 34–39. doi: 10.1080/10400410701841807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2015). Intelligence and creativity are pretty similar after all. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 599–606. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9299-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., et al. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychol. Aesthetics Creativity Arts 2, 68–85. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., and Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: connections to dual-process theory. Thinking Reasoning 21, 40–60. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2014.885464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E. (2009). “Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: is it time for a tri-process theory,” in In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond, eds J. S. B. T. Evans and K. Frankish (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 55–88.

Stanovich, K. E. (2010). Rationality and the Reflective Mind. Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The cognitive reflection test as a predictor of performance on heuristics and biases tasks. Mem. Cogn. 39, 1275–1289. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0104-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: an expansion of the cognitive reflection test. Thinking Reasoning 20, 147–168. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.844729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, A. K., and Moscovitch, M. (2006). “Cognitive processes of verbal fluency tasks,” in The Quantified Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment, ed A. M. Poreh (New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Group), 143–157.

Wallach, M. A., and Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of Thinking in Young Children: A Study of the Creativity-Intelligence Distinction. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Google Scholar

Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding Innovation in Problem Solving, Science, Invention, and the Arts. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Wiley, J., and Jarosz, A. F. (2012a). “How working memory capacity affects problem solving,” in Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 56, ed B. H. Ross (Burlington, MA: Academic Press), 185–228.

Wiley, J., and Jarosz, A. F. (2012b). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 21, 258–262 doi: 10.1177/0963721412447622

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: creativity, cognitive reflection, intelligence, cognition, intuition

Citation: Corgnet B, Espín AM and Hernán-González R (2016) Creativity and Cognitive Skills among Millennials: Thinking Too Much and Creating Too Little. Front. Psychol. 7:1626. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01626

Received: 20 July 2016; Accepted: 05 October 2016;
Published: 25 October 2016.

Edited by:

Nikolaos Georgantzis, University of Reading, UK

Reviewed by:

Noelia Sánchez-Pérez, University of Murcia, Spain
Conny Ernst-Peter Wollbrant, University of Gothenburg, Sweden

Copyright © 2016 Corgnet, Espín and Hernán-González. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonio M. Espín, a.espin@mdx.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.