Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2016
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic High-level adaptation and aftereffects View all 11 articles

The Importance of Formalizing Computational Models of Face Adaptation Aftereffects

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA
  • 2Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA

Face adaptation is widely used as a means to probe the neural representations that support face recognition. While the theories that relate face adaptation to behavioral aftereffects may seem conceptually simple, our work has shown that testing computational instantiations of these theories can lead to unexpected results. Instantiating a model of face adaptation not only requires specifying how faces are represented and how adaptation shapes those representations but also specifying how decisions are made, translating hidden representational states into observed responses. Considering the high-dimensionality of face representations, the parallel activation of multiple representations, and the non-linearity of activation functions and decision mechanisms, intuitions alone are unlikely to succeed. If the goal is to understand mechanism, not simply to examine the boundaries of a behavioral phenomenon or correlate behavior with brain activity, then formal computational modeling must be a component of theory testing. To illustrate, we highlight our recent computational modeling of face adaptation aftereffects and discuss how models can be used to understand the mechanisms by which faces are recognized.

Introduction

Adaptation aftereffects have been used as a tool for studying low-level vision (e.g., Gibson and Radner, 1937; Blakemore and Sutton, 1969; Frisby, 1979; Wade, 1994; Anstis et al., 1998; Kohn, 2007; Thompson and Burr, 2009; Webster, 2015) and there has been of growing interest in their potential for informing high-level vision. Like low-level aftereffects (color and tilt) just a few seconds of adaptation are needed to bias the perception of high-level stimuli (faces, bodies, and objects; Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007; Javadi and Wee, 2012; Rhodes et al., 2013). For example, brief adaptation to a face with a wide jaw and narrow eye separation will cause an aftereffect, biasing perception toward a psychologically opposite stimulus, such that a subsequently presented face may appear to have a narrower jaw and wider eye separation (Leopold et al., 2001; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006; Robbins et al., 2007; Tangen et al., 2011).

The face recognition literature has used adaptation to address debates about the nature of face representations. Unlike low-level visual features, such as color or orientation, faces are complex with high dimensionality. The concept of face space has pervaded theorizing about face recognition, providing a framework for understanding how such multidimensional stimuli are represented in the brain (Valentine, 1991). Faces are represented as points along a collection of psychological dimensions, with the distance between points reflecting the similarity between faces. Each dimension of face space is assumed to represent some component of variation across the population of know faces; dimensions could correspond to physical differences, such as the distances between the eyes, or more holistic sources (O’Toole et al., 1993; Burton et al., 1999; Dailey and Cottrell, 1999; Wilson et al., 2002).

Debate has centered on the nature of the face representations that reside within face space (see Valentine, 2001; Rhodes et al., 2005; Rhodes and Leopold, 2011; Valentine et al., 2015). Norm models propose that faces are represented with respect to their deviation from a prototypical or average face, the norm (Valentine, 1991; Byatt and Rhodes, 1998; Loffler et al., 2005; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006), whereas exemplar models propose that faces are represented with respect to their similarity to individually encoded faces, exemplars (Valentine, 1991; Lewis, 2004).

Although representations assumed by norm and exemplar models are fundamentally different, both turn out to make similar predictions about key aspects of face recognition, such as effects of typicality and distinctiveness (e.g., Valentine and Bruce, 1986; Valentine, 1991) and effects of caricature (e.g., Lewis and Johnston, 1998, 1999; Lewis, 2004). One seemingly promising approach to differentiating predictions of norm and exemplar models relies on face adaptation (e.g., Leopold et al., 2001; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006; Robbins et al., 2007).

Face Adaptation Aftereffects

In a now classic paper, Leopold et al. (2001) had participants first learn to identify four novel target faces (Adam, Jim, John, and Henry) and then tested their ability to identify test faces as one of the four studied targets. Test faces were positioned along morph lines that projected from each of the target identities, through the norm, to the opposite side of face space (Figure 1). On some trials, participants were first briefly shown an adaptor face for a few seconds and then identified a test face. Without adaptation, the average face at the center of the space was equally likely to be identified as one of the four targets. But with exposure to an adaptor located on the opposite side of the norm from a target, call it “anti-Adam,” the average face was more likely identified as “Adam.” Relative to baseline without adaptation, the psychometric function for identification as a function of the distance of a test face from the average (identity strength), is significantly shifted to the left by adaptation to a matching anti-face (adaptation to anti-Adam, testing on morphs of Adam); this contrasts with the psychometric function for non-matching anti-faces (adaptation to anti-Jim, testing on morphs of Adam), which is slightly shifted to the right.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) Schematic face-space representation of the stimuli used in anti-face adaptation paradigms (adapted from Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006). In both Leopold et al. (2001) and Rhodes and Jeffery (2006), an opposite adaptor (anti-face) was constructed to lie on the opposite side of the norm from each of four target faces (only one shown here), with the opposite test trajectory lying in between. In addition, Rhodes and Jeffery (2006) included additional four non-opposite (control) trajectories (only one trajectory is shown here) that lay between each target face and a corresponding non-opposite adaptor (matched to the opposite adaptor for distance from the target). (B) Sensitivity to face identity in baseline (open circles), matched anti-face (closed circles) and non-matched anti-face (open circles) conditions. The proportion of correct responses has been averaged across the four identity trajectories and a best-fitting four parameter logistic function is shown for each condition. Left panel: behavioral data from Leopold et al. (2001). Right panel: simulated data from Ross et al. (2014) for the exemplar-based (left), traditional norm-based (center), and two-pool norm-based (right) models. (C) Sensitivity to face identity on baseline (open circles) and adaptation (closed circles) trials along both the opposite morph-trajectory (top) and non-opposite morph trajectory (bottom). Left panel: behavioral data from Rhodes and Jeffery (2006). Right panel: simulated data from Ross et al. (2014) for the exemplar-based (left), traditional norm-based (center), and two-pool norm-based (right) models. Copyright (2013) Psychonomic Society, reproduced from Ross et al. (2014) with permission from Springer Science & Business Media and the Psychonomic Society.

Rhodes and Jeffery (2006) extended this paradigm with the inclusion of a critical control condition. In addition to contrasting adaptation along opposite morph trajectories that passed through the norm (Leopold et al., 2001), they tested adaptation along non-opposite morph trajectories that did not (Figure 1). Adaptation affected face identification with respect to the norm, such that post-adaptation identification thresholds were significantly lower for faces along opposite trajectories, compared to those along non-opposite trajectories. Rhodes and Jeffery (2006) reasoned that since the magnitude of adaptation was dependent on whether the morph trajectory passed through the norm (opposite trajectories) or not (non-opposite trajectories), the psychological representation of faces must make some reference to a norm face.

Intuitions About Face Adaptation

Finding that adaptation biases face recognition with respect to the norm rather than simply biasing recognition away from the adaptor has been widely interpreted as evidence for norm-based coding (Rhodes et al., 2005; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006; Robbins et al., 2007; Susilo et al., 2010; Rhodes and Leopold, 2011; Rhodes and Calder, 2014; Short et al., 2014; Walsh et al., 2015, but see Zhao et al., 2011; Storrs and Arnold, 2012). At its most simplest, finding that face adaptation is sensitive to the norm intuitively suggests that face representations are constructed with respect to a norm.

Intuitions are also often supported by considering illustrations of one-dimensional two-pool (norm) and multichannel (exemplar) models (Figure 2). In a two-pool model, face representation are assumed to be broadly tuned, responding maximally to a particular extreme within face space. For example, in Figure 2, the two pools encode variations in eye height, with one pool preferring faces with extremely high eyes and the other pool preferring faces with extremely low eyes. The location of a face along a given dimension is encoded by the proportional activity of the two opposing pools. Because the pools are explicitly specified to intersect at the location of the norm face, two-pool models are generally considered instantiations of norm models. By contrast, in a multichannel model, face representations are assumed to be narrowly tuned, preferring faces at a particular location along some dimension. These are generally considered analogous to exemplar models because faces are encoded only with respect to other faces, rather than making reference to any norm face.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Illustration of intuitive, hypothesized adaptation effects from a two-pool model (left) and exemplar-based model (right) reproduced from Susilo et al. (2010). The dimension used in this illustration is eye height. In the two-pool model (left), eye height is encoded by opposing pools of face representations, centered on the norm with the combined activity of the pools representing the location of a face on the eye height dimension. In the exemplar-based model (right), eye-height is encoded by multiple representations with bell-shaped tunings. In both cases, adaptation is assumed to result in a decrease in activation of each representation in proportion to its activation by the adapting stimulus. The effect of adaptation of the face representations is represented by the dashed line. Simple illustrations such as these often drive theoretical intuitions about face adaptation aftereffects. Copyright (2010) Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology (RVO).

For both two-pool and multichannel models, face representations are assumed to adapt in proportion to their level of activation to the adaptor. In the case of the two-pool norm model, adapting one of the pools more strongly than the other will cause a subsequent bias in the relative activation of the two pools, with the effect that after adaptation, perception will be biased toward the opposite extreme. By contrast, for the multichannel exemplar models, adaptation will only affect representations at the location of the adaptor, causing subsequent perception to be biased away from the adaptor, not specifically toward the opposite extreme.

A significant limitation of this past work is that nearly all of the assumptions about how norm and exemplar models might respond to face adaptation have been based on intuitions and pictorial illustrations of one- or two-dimensions. Rarely are these intuitions supported by explicit simulations, where predictions are formally generated, compared, and evaluated. The mechanisms involving face recognition undoubtedly involves combinations of high-dimensionality, non-linear activation, and parallelism – all properties of the human brain. Making predictions based on intuition or illustration alone must be the subject of scrutiny (e.g., see Hintzman, 1990; Burton and Vokey, 1998; Lewis, 2004; Palmeri and Cottrell, 2009), as we recently illustrated (Ross et al., 2014).

Modeling Face Adaptation Aftereffects

Part of our motivation for a close examination of formal predictions of norm and exemplar models comes from lessons learned in the categorization literature. Posner and Keele (1968, 1970) had people learn to classify novel dot pattern stimuli into different categories, each defined by a unique prototype. Even though they never saw the prototypes during learning, being trained only on distortions of prototypes, when tested after learning, classification of unseen prototypes was better than classification of distortions. The canonical interpretation of prototype enhancement (e.g., Reed, 1972; Rosch, 1975; Homa, 1984) was that category learning involves some form of prototype abstraction. How else could an unseen prototype be classified so well than it had been abstracted as a product of learning? These intuitions were challenged when prototype and exemplar models were formalized in mathematics and simulations and their predictions explicitly generated and compared to data. Models that assume memory for category exemplars, without any abstraction, account quite well for prototype enhancement effects as well as phenomena that pure prototype abstraction models cannot (e.g., see Busemeyer et al., 1984; Hintzman, 1986; Nosofsky, 1992; Palmeri and Nosofsky, 2001). Could a similar disconnect between intuition and formal prediction be the case for norm versus exemplar debate in face recognition?

Ross et al. (2014), we instantiated an exemplar model of face recognition, which bore similarities to exemplar models of categorization (e.g., Nosofsky, 1986; Kruschke, 1992) and descriptions of multichannel models (e.g., Robbins et al., 2007). We also instantiated two versions of a norm-based model. One version, which we referred to as a traditional norm-based model, had been formalized previously by Giese and Leopold (2005); the other, a two-pool model, was adapted from descriptions of norm-based coding (e.g., Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006). In the following descriptions, we omit many of the mathematical and computational details, focusing instead on what we did and why, and recommend the reader to refer to Ross et al. (2014).

Our core modeling framework is no different from dozens, if not hundreds, of other models of perception and cognition. There is an input layer that models the perceptual representation of the face, an intermediate layer that models alternative assumptions about face space (exemplar, traditional norm, and two-pool), and an output layer that generates a decision about the face (Adam, Jim, John, or Henry). Because we were interested in directly comparing three alternative theories of face space, we assumed the same input representation and output mechanism for every model. All that varied was the internal face-space representation.

To briefly outline, when a test face, say a particular face along the Anti-Adam/Adam morph continuum, is presented to the model for identification, a multidimensional perceptual representation is created by the visual system. Instead of attempting to model the complete processing hierarchy of the ventral stream visual system that creates this perceptual representation (e.g., Jiang et al., 2006; Serre et al., 2007), we considered two simpler possibilities. The first made no specific assumptions about how the perceptual representation of a face is created from its 2D retinal image, and simply assumed that a randomly sampled face is represented by a random sample from a multivariate Gaussian (normal) distribution (see also Lewis, 2004). The second used an actual 2D face image as input and created a multidimensional input representation of it via principal components analysis (PCA), much like many other models (e.g., O’Toole et al., 1993; Burton et al., 1999; Dailey and Cottrell, 1999; Giese and Leopold, 2005; Richler et al., 2007).

In both cases the perceptual representation is simply a vector specifying the location of a given test face along each dimension of face space. This face representation then activates exemplars, norms, or pools in the face-space layer according to the rules for that particular model of face space. In each case, we assumed that pre-experimental experience populated face space with a collection of exemplars, norms, or pools, depending on the model. Like other exemplar-based face-space model (e.g., Lewis, 2004; Giese and Leopold, 2005), activation of a given exemplar in face space is a non-linearly decreasing function of the distance of that exemplar to the test face. For the traditional norm-based model (Giese and Leopold, 2005), norm representations are activated by a test face as a function of both the difference in angular distance with respect to the norm as well as their relative distance from the norm. And for the two-pool model (e.g., Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006), competing pools of units on opposite side of the norm are activated as a function of the relative similarity of a test face to members of each pair in the pool. Simple assumptions were made to implement adaptation within the three models as a temporary rescaling of maximal activation according to the similarity of each exemplar, norm, or pool to the adaptor.

Finally, the distributed pattern of activity across these exemplars, norms, or pools is associated with output nodes for Adam, Jim, John, or Henry, with the relationship between a given pattern of face-space activations and a particular identity learned using a standard delta-rule learning algorithm. This step mirrors the initial learning of face identities by participants in the face adaptation experiments. Activation of the identity nodes is readily translated into identification probabilities (following, for example, Kruschke, 1992). While the actual identification of the learned face is a critical component of face adaptation experiments, most predictions described for different face-space models focus entirely on the face-space representation, largely ignoring how the activation of those representations in face space might be combined to generate the learned face identification response that is the key measure in the task.

We tested the three models on how well they could qualitatively predict the observed data from Leopold et al. (2001) and Rhodes and Jeffery (2006), outlined earlier, as well as a related data from Leopold and Bondar (2005). Each of the variants has a small collection of free parameters, including such things as the number of dimensions in the input perceptual representations, the width of the tuning of exemplars, norms, or pools, and scaling parameters that define things like the strength of adaptation and how output activity is mapped onto response probabilities. We explored a wide range of model parameterizations, and not only looked for parameter combinations that provided a good quantitative account of the observed data in each experiment, but also evaluated whether models made parameter-free qualitative predictions irrespective of particular parameterizations. Simply put, outside of subsets of parameters that produced no significant effect of adaptation at all – for example, having the scaling on adaptation too low or the width of the tuning function too small – in all cases, the quantitative predictions illustrated below map onto the qualitative predictions across parameters sets.

All three models are able to account for the data from Leopold et al. (2001) and only the exemplar model and the two-pool model are able to account for the data from Rhodes and Jeffery (2006), with the traditional norm-based model failing on that score (Figure 1). Despite prevalent intuitions that face adaptation aftereffects clearly support norm models over exemplar models, those intuitions were not borne out when those models were explicitly implemented and simulated and their predictions compared to observed data. In fact, our variant of the traditional norm-based model could not explain key aspects of the observed face adaptation data.

Why Intuitions Fail

Why do common intuitions about norm and exemplar face adaptation fail? To begin with, most illustrations depict one or two dimensions but face space likely has 10, 20, 100, or more dimensions. Human intuition beyond two or three dimensions often fails miserably (e.g., DiCarlo and Cox, 2007). Combine this high dimensionality with the non-linearities in activation functions and decision rules, and intuition is bound to fail.

Consider also the claim that representations in a multichannel model must be narrowly tuned (e.g., Robbins et al., 2007). On a single dimension perhaps this corresponds to a representation having at most significant activation across one quarter of the span of the dimension (Figure 2, right). But, consider instead a representation in two dimensions; we now have a circle that occupies significantly less than one quarter (one quadrant) of the total space. While in one dimensions it seems that each exemplar must be so broadly tuned that it would convey little useful information to discriminate different faces, in multiple dimensions this need not be the case.

Illustrations of exemplar models (Figure 2, right) also tend to impose a complete tiling across face space. To begin with, while a complete tiling might be feasible for one or even two dimensions, the sheer number of nodes necessary to fully tile a 100-dimension face space is far more than the number of neurons in the brain (and likely more than the number of atoms in the known universe). More importantly, exemplar models assume that face space will be populated based on specific experience with faces, and do not assume any a priori covering map across space. The combination of a finite number of face representations, together with non-uniform exemplar distribution, along with exemplar tuning widths required in high dimensions, creates a situation in which face adaptation is dependent on the adaptor faces location relative to the entire population of exemplars. Like other examples from the face recognition and category learning literatures (e.g., Lewis, 2004), the ability of the exemplar model to make predictions that appear to require a norm is driven by the fact that the average is implicitly represented in the statistical distribution of faces, not any explicit norm representation.

Discussion

Face adaptation aftereffects are used to draw mechanistic conclusions about how faces are represented. Our work makes clear that mechanistic conclusions really must be supported by formalized computational models that delineate specific testable assumptions about how faces are represented, how face knowledge is represented and used, how adaptation works, and how face identities are determined. Our assumptions about any of these components may be wrong, or there may be alternatives that we did not consider. But that is a strength of formal modeling, not a weakness, especially when compared to mechanistic predictions derived from intuitions or simplified illustrations, as has been the case for much theoretical work concerning face adaptation. In nearly every area of perception and cognition where a computational modeling has been deployed, there are examples of empirical phenomena that intuitively point to one particular mechanistic explanation but in fact can be explained as well or better by other explanations when those alternatives are formally evaluated (e.g., Farrell and Lewandowsky, 2010).

Author Contributions

Both DR and TP contributed equally to the conception, design and drafting of this manuscript and are accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported in part by NSF grants SBE-1257098 and SMA-1041755 (Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center) and by a Discovery Grant from Vanderbilt University.

References

Anstis, S., Verstraten, F. A., and Mather, G. (1998). The motion aftereffect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 111–117.

Google Scholar

Blakemore, C., and Sutton, P. (1969). Size adaptation: a new aftereffect. Science 166, 245–247. doi: 10.1126/science.166.3902.245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., and Hancock, P. J. B. (1999). From pixels to people: a model of familiar face recognition. Cogn. Sci. 23, 1–31. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2301_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, A. M., and Vokey, J. R. (1998). The face-space typicality paradox: understanding the faces-space metaphor. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 475–483. doi: 10.1080/713755768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busemeyer, J. R., Dewey, G. I., and Medin, D. L. (1984). Evaluation of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 10, 638–648. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byatt, G., and Rhodes, G. (1998). Recognition of own-race and other-race caricatures: implications for models of face recognition. Vis. Res. 38, 2455–2468. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00469-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dailey, M. N., and Cottrell, G. W. (1999). Organization of face and object recognition in modular neural network models. Neural Netw. 12, 1053–1073. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(99)00050-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiCarlo, J. J., and Cox, D. D. (2007). Untangling invariant object recognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 333–341. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrell, S., and Lewandowsky, S. (2010). Computational models as aids to better reasoning in psychology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 329–335. doi: 10.1177/0963721410386677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frisby, J. (1979). Seeing: Illusion, Brain, and Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J., and Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. quantitative studies. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 453–467. doi: 10.1037/h0059826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giese, M. A., and Leopold, D. A. (2005). Physiologically inspired neural model for the encoding of face spaces. Neurocomputing 65–66, 93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2004.10.060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hintzman, D. (1986). Schema abstraction in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychol. Rev. 93, 411–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411

CrossRef Full Text

Hintzman, D. L. (1990). Human learning and memory: connections and dissociations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 109–139. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.000545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Homa, D. (1984). On the nature of categories. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 18, 49–94. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60359-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, X., Rosen, E., Zeffiro, T., VanMeter, J., Blanz, V., and Riesenhuber, M. (2006). Evaluation of a shape-based model of human face discrimination using fMRI and behavioral techniques. Neuron 50, 159–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Javadi, A. H., and Wee, N. (2012). Cross-category adaptation: objects produce gender adaptation in the perception of faces. PLoS ONE 7:e46079 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohn, A. (2007). Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3155–3164. doi: 10.1152/jn.00086.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychol. Rev. 99, 22–44. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leopold, D. A., and Bondar, I. (2005). “Adaptation to complex visual patterns in humans and monkeys,” in Fitting the Mind to the World: Adaptation and After-Effects in High-Level Vision, eds C. W. G. Clifford and G. Rhodes (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 213–240.

Google Scholar

Leopold, D. A., Rhodes, G., Muller, K. M., and Jeffery, L. (2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 897–904. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leopold, D. A., O’Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., and Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 40–46. doi: 10.1038/82947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. B. (2004). Face-space-R: towards a unified account of face recognition. Vis. Cogn. 11, 29–69. doi: 10.1080/13506280344000194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. B., and Johnston, R. A. (1998). Understanding caricatures of faces. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 51, 321–346. doi: 10.1080/713755758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. B., and Johnston, R. A. (1999). A unified account of the effects of caricaturing faces. Vis. Cogn. 6, 1–41.

Google Scholar

Loffler, G., Yourganov, G., Wilkinson, F., and Wilson, H. R. (2005). fMRI evidence for the neural representation of faces. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1386–1391. doi: 10.1038/nn1538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 115, 39–57. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.39

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosofsky, R. M. (1992). “Exemplar-based approach to relating categorization, identification, and recognition,” in Multidimensional Models of Perception and Cognition, ed. F. G. Ashby (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 363–393.

Google Scholar

O’Toole, A. J., Abdi, H., Deffenbacher, K. A., and Valentin, D. (1993). Low-dimensional representation of faces in higher dimensions of the face space. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, 405–411.

Google Scholar

Palmeri, T. J., and Cottrell, G. W. (2009). “Modeling perceptual expertise,” in Perceptual Expertise: Bridging Brain And Behavior, eds D. N. Bub, M. J. Tarr, and I. Gauthier (New York, NY: Oxford University Press) 197–245.

Google Scholar

Palmeri, T. J., and Nosofsky, R. M. (2001). Central tendencies, extreme points, and prototype enhancement effects in ill-defined perceptual categorization. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 54, 197–235. doi: 10.1080/02724980042000084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. J. Exp. Psychol. 77, 353–363. doi: 10.1037/h0025953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Keele, S. W. (1970). Retention of abstract ideas. J. Exp. Psychol. 83, 304–308. doi: 10.1037/h0028558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richler, J. J., Mack, M. L., Gauthier, I., and Palmeri, T. J. (2007). “Distinguishing between perceptual and decisional sources of holism in face processing,” in Proceedings of the 29th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, eds D. S. McNamara and J. G. Trafton (Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society).

Google Scholar

Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cogn. Psychol. 3, 382–407. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90014-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., and Calder, A. J. (2014). “Face perception,” in The New Visual Neurosciences,” eds J. S. Werner and L. M. Chalupa, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 711–723.

Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., and Jeffery, L. (2006). Adaptive norm-based coding of facial identity. Vision Res. 46, 2977–2987. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Boeing, A., and Calder, A. J. (2013). Visual coding of human bodies: perceptual aftereffects reveal norm-based, opponent coding of body identity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. 39, 313–317. doi: 10.1037/a0031568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Clifford, C. W. G., and Leopold, D. A. (2007). The timecourse of higher-level face aftereffects. Vision Res. 47, 2291–2296. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., and Leopold, D. A. (2011). “Adaptive norm-based coding of face identity,” in Oxford Handbook of Face Perception, eds A. Calder, G. Rhodes, M. Johnson, and J. Haxby (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 263–286.

Google Scholar

Robbins, R., McKone, E., and Edwards, M. (2007). Aftereffects for face attributes with different natural variability: adapter position effects and neural models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 570–592.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Robbins, R., Jaquet, E., McKone, E., Jeffery, L., and Clifford, C. W. G. (2005). “Adaptation and face perception – how aftereffects implicate norm based coding of faces,” in Fitting the Mind to the World: Adaptation and Aftereffects in High-Level Vision, eds C. W. G. Clifford, and G. Rhodes (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 213–240.

Google Scholar

Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 104, 192–233. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, D. A., Deroche, M., and Palmeri, T. J. (2014). Not just the norm: exemplar-based models also predict face aftereffects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 47–70. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0449-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serre, T., Oliva, A., and Poggio, T. A. (2007). A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 6424–6429.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Short, L. A., Lee, K., Fu, G., and Mondloch, C. J. (2014). Category-specific face prototypes are emerging, but not yet mature, in 5-year-old children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126, 161–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storrs, K. R., and Arnold, D. H. (2012). Not all face aftereffects are equal. Vision Res. 64, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susilo, T., McKone, E., and Edwards, M. (2010). Solving the upside-down puzzle: why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike?. J. Vis. 10, 1–16. doi: 10.1167/10.13.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangen, J. M., Murphy, S. C., and Thompson, M. B. (2011). Flashed face distortion effect: grotesque faces from relative spaces. Perception 40, 628–630. doi: 10.1068/p6968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, P., and Burr, D. (2009). Visual aftereffects. Curr. Biol. 19, R11–R14. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 161–204. doi: 10.1080/14640749108400966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, T. (2001). Face-Space Models of Face Recognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Valentine, T., and Bruce, V. (1986). The effects of distinctiveness in recognizing and classifying faces. Perception 15, 525–535. doi: 10.1068/p150525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, T., Lewis, M. B., and Hills, P. (2015). Face-space: a unifying concept in face recognition research. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.990392 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, N. J. (1994). A selective history of the study of motion aftereffects. Perception 23, 1111–1134. doi: 10.1068/p231111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, J. A., Maurer, D., Vida, M. D., Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., and Rutherford, M. D. (2015). Norm-based coding of facial identity in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Vision Res. 108, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, M. A. (2015). Visual adaptation. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1, 547–567. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, H. R., Loffler, G., and Wilkinson, F. (2002). Synthetic faces, face cubes, and the geometry of face space. Vision Res. 42, 2909–2923. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00362-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, C., Seriès, P., Hancock, P. J. B., and Bednar, J. A. (2011). Similar neural adaptation mechanisms underlying face gender and tilt aftereffects. Vision Res. 51, 2021–2030. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: face recognition, adaptation, exemplar, norm, computational modeling

Citation: Ross DA and Palmeri TJ (2016) The Importance of Formalizing Computational Models of Face Adaptation Aftereffects. Front. Psychol. 7:815. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00815

Received: 23 January 2016; Accepted: 17 May 2016;
Published: 13 June 2016.

Edited by:

Stefania D’Ascenzo, University of Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

K. Suzanne Scherf, Pennsylvania State University, USA
Aaron Michael Clarke, Bilkent University, Turkey

Copyright © 2016 Ross and Palmeri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David A. Ross, ZGF2aWQuYS5yb3NzMjQwOUBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.