Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 March 2016
Sec. Cultural Psychology

Contextual Effect of Wealth on Independence: An Examination through Regional Differences in China

  • 1Faculty of Economics, Shiga University, Hikone, Japan
  • 2School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 3Business School, Durham University, Durham, UK
  • 4Graduate School of Management, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan

The current study disentangled two different effects of wealth on psychological tendency toward independence: one is an effect exerted at the individual level (i.e., being rich) and the other one is a contextual effect (i.e., being surrounded by rich individuals). Past research has found a stronger tendency toward independence among people in economically developed societies. This association has often been explained as a result of a greater amount of choices, and thus more opportunities to express individuality that wealth affords individuals. In addition to this individual-level process, theories in cultural psychology imply that the wealth-independence link also reflects social processes—living in a rich society, regardless of one’s own wealth, promotes independence (contextual effect of wealth on independence). Through a large-scale survey in China, using multilevel analyses, we found that wealth had both the individual-level effect and contextual effect on independence as well as related psychological tendencies (influence orientation and generalized trust), suggesting that individuals are more likely to be independent with greater personal wealth and when surrounded by wealthy others. Possible processes through which independence is promoted by liing in a wealthy area are discussed.

Introduction

Across human societies, individuals differ in their relative independence from, and interdependence within, social relationships (Hofstede, 1980; Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995; see also Henrich et al., 2010b). Independent (individualistic) tendency is characterized by individuals seeing themselves as independent from others, which also relates to motivation to influence (rather than adjusting oneself to) others (Morling et al., 2002) and willingness to interact with others outside of one’s immediate social circles, and thus generalized trust of others (Allik and Realo, 2004; Hamamura, 2012). These psychological tendencies give rise to behavioral patterns that, in turn, collectively maintain the social environment and reinforce psychological processes associated with independence (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Shweder, 1991; Leung and Cohen, 2011; Yamagishi, 2011).

One of the best-established correlates of independent tendency at the societal level is wealth. Previous cross-cultural research has found that people residing in rich societies are more likely to show independent tendencies (Hofstede, 1980; Inglehart and Baker, 2000; Greenfield et al., 2003; Georgas et al., 2004; Grossmann and Varnum, 2015). For example, Hofstede (1980) reported a strong correlation (r = 0.82) between Gross National Product per capita and individualism through a nation-level correlation analysis. Georgas et al. (2004) found that differences in individualism across countries was best explained by economic differences among a range of eco-cultural variables, such as religion and education.

However, the societal-level correlation between wealth and independence found in previous studies (e.g., Hofstede, 1980) may either reflect an effect of affluent societies promoting independent tendencies or an effect of affluent individuals tending to be more independent, or both of them. Stated differently, the individual-level effect of wealth may give rise to the societal-level pattern that Hofstede (1980) identified.

In fact, a number of theories have interpreted the wealth-independence link as reflecting the effects associated with personal wealth. For example, it has been suggested that wealth promotes independence because “(a)ffluence implies the ability to ‘do one’s own thing”’ (Triandis et al., 1988, p. 324) or “affluence creates choices and allows people to express their individuality by making different choices” (Schimmack et al., 2005, p. 21). This type of explanation postulates that personal wealth facilitates greater independence through its effect in affording more choices to individuals. Consistent with this theory, research has demonstrated an association between individual differences in socioeconomic status and independence (e.g., Kohn et al., 1990; Stephens et al., 2007; Kraus and Keltner, 2009; Grossmann and Varnum, 2010; Na et al., 2010; Hamamura et al., 2013).

On the other hand, influential theories in cultural psychology seem to support the view that wealth also has a unique effect on independence at the societal level. Theories of cultural psychology have emphasized roles of social processes, such as shared meaning systems (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Snibbe and Markus, 2005; Kitayama and Uskul, 2011), social norms (Gelfand et al., 2011), and shared social contexts (Kraus et al., 2012) in shaping psychological processes. These social processes may help explain the effect of wealth on independence beyond its effect operating at the level of individuals. In other words, these theories predict the presence of a contextual effect, that wealth exerts an effect on independence at the societal level above and beyond its effect at the individual level. Depending on the relative strength of individual-level and societal-level effect, the contextual effect can take two forms (e.g., Tay et al., 2014). One is the case in which the societal-level effect is larger than the individual-level effect—a society’s wealth has a stronger effect on people’s independence than predicted on the basis of their level of affluence. The other is the case in which the societal-level effect is smaller than the individual-level effect—a society’s wealth has a weaker effect on people’s independence than predicted on the basis of their affluence. Theories in cultural psychology seem to suggest the presence of the former type of contextual effect. Analytically, a contextual effect can be computed in a multilevel model that simultaneously examines the effect of wealth on independence at the level of individual (within-group effect) and society (between-group effect) and by taking their difference (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002).

In some areas of research, contextual effects have clearly been established (e.g., Henrich et al., 2005, 2010a; Christ et al., 2014; Colleran et al., 2014; Tay et al., 2014). For example, findings in Henrich et al. (2005, 2010a) suggested that anonymous interpersonal interactions that are normative in modern market exchange tend to promote community members’ prosocial behavior toward an anonymous other, above and beyond the effects associated with individuals’ personal experiences of market exchange. This research demonstrated the role of a socially shared factor (the norms of anonymous interactions at markets) in influencing individual behavior (prosocial behavior toward an anonymous other) beyond the individual-level process (personal experience of market exchange).

In the current study, we predicted a significant individual-level association between wealth and independence, replicating past research (e.g., Kohn et al., 1990; Stephens et al., 2007; Kraus and Keltner, 2009; Grossmann and Varnum, 2010; Na et al., 2010; Hamamura et al., 2013). Additionally, we examined wealth’s contextual effect on independent tendency—an effect suggested by cultural psychological theories articulating the role of social processes in shaping psychological processes (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Gelfand et al., 2011; Kitayama and Uskul, 2011).

We examined the effect of wealth on independent tendency across provinces in China. China was an excellent context for the current research because there are large regional variations within China with regard to the distribution of wealth (Yang, 2002; Fleisher et al., 2010), as well as in the cultural practices maintained through legal restrictions on inter-provincial migrations. Yet, because the provinces are parts of a single nation, major variables that usually confound cross-country comparisons are naturally ruled out (Varnum et al., 2010).

The units of analysis in the current study were individual participants (Level 1) that are nested within provinces (Level 2). Participants were recruited from all the provinces in mainland China, who provided information on their annual household income and responded to scales designed to measure their tendency toward independence. One measure of independence was the Independent Self-Construal Scale (Singelis, 1994). In addition to this direct measure of independence, we assessed two psychological tendencies conceptually associated with independence. One measure assessed orientation toward influencing surrounding social circumstances (as opposed to adjusting oneself). As the independent self is perceived as relatively immutable and a consistent entity working within the context of a mutable world (Heine, 2008), individuals with independent self-construal tend to engage in influence rather than adjustment (Morling et al., 2002). The other measure assessed generalized trust or trust toward others who are not closely connected to oneself (Yamagishi and Yamagishi, 1994). Previous studies (Allik and Realo, 2004; Hamamura, 2012) found a positive association between independence and generalized trust, suggesting that taking the risk of trusting others outside of a close-knit community may be sustained by viewing oneself as independent from others [but see also the findings of Yuki et al. (2005) showing that individualists sometimes show a strong in-group bias; see Brewer and Chen (2007), and Brewer and Yuki (2007), for reviews]. Thus, we explored if generalized trust would show similar correlational patterns as independent self-construal.

We performed multilevel analyses using group-mean centering. Hence, all the predictors at the individual level were centered around respective provincial means. This method removes all inter-provincial differences and allows examination of within-province (individual-level) effects and between-province (province-level) effects separately from each other (Enders and Tofighi, 2007). A contextual effect is obtained by taking the difference between these two effects. The positive contextual effect (between-province > within-province effect) would indicate that wealth at the level of provinces has a stronger effect on people’s independence than predicted on the basis of people’s affluence.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from all the provinces (including directly controlled municipalities as well as autonomous regions) in mainland China by a marketing research firm, Cross Marketing, Inc. Fifty individuals from each province took part in the study, except for Shanghai and Gansu from which 100 people participated. (The larger sample size from these locations is for the purpose of another research project in which the current study was part.) The total number of participants was 1,650, which is comparable to a previous study examining regional differences in China (Talhelm et al., 2014, N = 1,162). In all provinces, the same recruiting methods were used (i.e., advertising on the Internet, mass media, and face-to-face surveys). The sample size at both levels were determined prior to data collection. To maximize the sample size at level 2 (province level), which is essential for accurate estimations of the level 2 effects (Maas and Hox, 2005), we collected data from all the provinces in mainland China (i.e., n = 31). The number of participants from each province (n = 50 for most provinces) was determined based on results of simulations by Maas and Hox (2005) to ensure unbiased estimations in multilevel analyses. Participants’ responses were collected through an online survey (Supplementary Table S1 for sample characteristics).

Individual-Level Measures

Participants completed scales designed to measure independence and related tendencies. The scales were the Self-Construal Scale (Singelis, 1994), Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Values (CSIV; Locke, 2000), and Generalized Trust Scale (Yamagishi and Yamagishi, 1994). All scales were administered in Chinese. The consensus method was taken to ensure the validity of translations, and at least two independent translators checked the translations (see Supplementary Table S1 for scale reliability and intra-class correlations of the measures).

The Self-Construal Scale consisted of 12 items for assessing independent self-construal (e.g., “Being able to take care of myself is a primary concern for me”) and 12 items for assessing interdependent self-construal (e.g., “I will sacrifice my self-interest for the benefit of my group”). Responses were indicated on a 7-point scale with options ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

The CSIV assessed influence–adjustment orientation. Participants were instructed to think about interpersonal situations (such as being with close friends, with strangers, at work, and at social gatherings) and indicate, for example, how important it was to them “that I have an impact on them” (influence) and “that I go along with what they want to do” (adjustment). Following the procedure used in previous studies (Tsai et al., 2007; Miyamoto and Wilken, 2010), four items were used to measure influence orientation and five items were used to measure adjustment orientation. Responses were indicated on a 5-point scale with options ranging from 0 (not important to me) to 4 (extremely important to me).

The third measure, the Generalized Trust Scale, included five items such as, “Most people are basically honest.” Responses were indicated on a 7-point scale with options ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

After completing these psychological measures, participants provided information on their annual household income (open-ended format), which was log-transformed prior to analyses in order to correct the skew. They also indicated their gender, age, and education level. For education level, participants were asked to indicate their latest academic background from a list of options ranging from elementary school to graduate school (Ph.D. level). We used a dummy-coded variable for education (reference category = no college degree) in our analyses.

The institutional review board at Kyoto University [the primary investigator’s (KT) home institution at the time of study] did not require review of a low-risk survey such as ours. As such, we did not seek institutional approval. All aspects of the study were conducted in accordance with the ethical rules of the American Psychological Association. Participation was voluntary and consent was indicated by completion of the survey.

Province-Level Measures

As an index of province-level wealth, we used provincial means of participants’ annual household income (after log-transformation). We also obtained several province-level variables and included them as covariates. These included population density (Vandello and Cohen, 1999) and inequality in the distribution of wealth (Fincher et al., 2008). We computed population density from population size (obtained from National Bureau of Statistics of China, 2011) divided by land area (obtained from the website of the State Council of the People’s Republic of China1 on February 5, 2014). For an index of inequality, urban-rural income disparity was used (i.e., ratio of urban disposable income to rural net income, obtained from All China Data Center2 on February 6, 2014). In addition, the amount of direct foreign investments into provinces (obtained from All China Data Center on February 6, 2014) was controlled for, because greater foreign investment may signal greater exposure to Western cultures (Yang, 2002) and may contribute to independent tendencies of residents. Percentage of in-migrants from other provinces (obtained from Duan et al., 2013) was also included in the models, as past research suggested that high residential mobility promotes independent tendencies (Oishi and Kisling, 2009) and wealthy provinces presumably attract more migrants. Additionally, because four directly controlled municipalities in China (i.e., Beijing, Chongqing, Tianjin, and Shanghai) are generally urbanized, we included a dummy-coded variable to represent a directly controlled municipality. Finally, as an index of education at the province level, we calculated the percentage of those who obtained a college degree or above from our participants’ responses. We included this variable as a covariate at the province level. Descriptive statistics of the province-level variables are shown in Supplementary Table S2.

Results

Zero-Order Correlations

As preliminary analyses, we examined zero-order correlations at the individual level (Table 1) and at the province level (Table 2). As anticipated, log-transformed annual income (individual level) was positively associated with independent self-construal (r = 0.22), influence orientation (r = 0.26), and generalized trust (r = 0.12).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Zero-order correlations between individual-level variables (Pearson’s r).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Zero-order correlations at the province level (Pearson’s r).

Unexpectedly, interdependent self-construal (r = 0.12) and adjustment orientation (r = 0.16) were also positively correlated with annual income, but to a somewhat lesser degree when compared to the counterpart subscale (i.e., independent self-construal for interdependent self-construal, and influence orientation for adjustment orientation). The unexpected positive correlations between annual income and the interdependence measures might be caused by response style, such as acquiescence response style, which has been found to be strong in collectivistic societies (Schimmack et al., 2005). In fact, the counterpart subscales were positively correlated (r = 0.66 for independent and interdependent self-construal, r = 0.63 for influence and adjustment orientations), though past research suggests that independence and interdependence are two separate constructs (Oyserman et al., 2002). As such, in our main analyses using multilevel modeling the counterpart subscale was controlled for. For example, independent self-construal was analyzed using the residual term of the regression analysis in which independent self-construal was predicted from interdependent self-construal. Similarly, when we predicted influence we controlled for adjustment by using the residual from the regression of influence on adjustment. After controlling for the counterpart subscales, correlational patterns between the scales were in line with theoretical predictions. Independent self-construal was positively correlated with influence, r = 0.28, p < 0.001, while it was very weakly (and negatively) correlated with adjustment, r = –0.04, p = 0.069. Also, interdependent self-construal was not correlated with influence, r = –0.02, p = 0.356, while it was positively correlated with adjustment, r = 0.17, p < 0.001.

At the province level (Table 2), provincial mean of logged annual household income was positively associated with independent self-construal (r = 0.60), influence (r = 0.62), and generalized trust (r = 0.41). Unexpectedly, income was also positively associated with adjustment, although relatively weakly (r = 0.49). Provincial income correlated with several province-level variables (e.g., population density, percentage of in-migrants). As these correlations among the province-level variables might be confounding the correlations between provincial income and the independence-related measures, in the following analyses we controlled for the effects of these province-level factors as well as the individual-level factors (i.e., age, gender, and education level). Controlling for these variables was important in order to examine the unique effect of wealth on the independent tendencies at the individual level as well as the province level using multilevel modeling. Among the province-level covariates, directly controlled municipality, population density, and percentage of in-migrants—three variables presumably related to degree of urbanization—were highly correlated with each other (rs = 0.43 to 0.67). To avoid multicollinearity in the multilevel modeling, we created a single composite measure of urbanity by averaging them after standardization, and used the composite measure as a province-level covariate.

Multilevel Analyses of Independence

Multilevel analyses were conducted predicting independence and controlling for the individual- and province-level covariates (Table 3). We found that the within-province effect of annual income on independence was significant and positive. This finding confirms the wealth-independence association at the individual level. Similarly, within-province effects of annual income were also found for influence orientation and generalized trust. The between-province effects of income were also found to be significant and positive on all the independence-related measures.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Multilevel analyses of independent self-construal, influence, and generalized trust.

Importantly, the contextual effect, which was obtained by taking the difference of the between-province and within-province effect, was positive in all cases. As illustrated in Figure 1, the regression line for the between-province effect of income on independent self-construal was steeper than the regression lines for the within-province effect of income, and this contextual effect (i.e., the difference between within-province and between-province effects) was positive and significantly different from zero. Similar patterns were found for influence and generalized trust, though the contextual effect on generalized trust was relatively weak and marginally significant (Table 3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Results of the multilevel analysis: Independent self-construal as a function of logged annual household income. In the graph, the three solid lines are regression lines for the effect of individual-level income on independent self-construal at three levels of provincial income (average household income at the province level). The dotted line is the regression line for the relationship between province-level income and independent self-construal.

In summary, we found consistent patterns across all the independence-related tendencies. Annual income had positive associations with independence and related tendencies above and beyond the individual level. Among participants with the same level of personal wealth, those in rich provinces were more likely to be independent, influence-oriented, and trustful of others, than those in less rich provinces.

Supplementary Analysis: Effect of Wealth on Interdependence

We also examined the effects of wealth on the interdependence-related tendencies by similar multilevel analyses (Table 4). As expected, after controlling for various possible confounding factors the associations between wealth and the interdependent measures, which were partially observed in the zero-order correlation analyses, were no longer found. Annual income was not associated with interdependent self-construal or adjustment orientation either at the individual or province level. The diverging patterns of findings between independence and interdependence-related tendencies are consistent with an observation in the literature that independent and interdependent tendencies are separate dimensions with different correlates (Oyserman et al., 2002).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Multilevel analyses of interdependent self-construal and adjustment.

Discussion

Previous studies have found that individuals living in rich societies are inclined toward independence. The purpose of the current study was to separate the individual-level effect and contextual effect of wealth on independence, and to examine whether the wealth-independence link emerges through social processes above and beyond individual processes. We investigated regional differences in independence-related tendencies (independent self-construal, influence orientation, and generalized trust) across 31 provinces in China. The multilevel analyses confirmed the individual-level associations between wealth and independence-related tendencies. The results also revealed that income had a contextual effect on independence-related tendencies, as indicated by larger effects at the province level than the individual level. Thus, even if one is not rich, his or her independent tendency is likely promoted through the affluence of surrounding others.

Below, we discuss a question that should be addressed in future studies. Specifically, why does wealth of surrounding others promote one’s independence? The first possibility concerns the social transmission of psychological/behavioral tendencies from rich to non-rich individuals. It has been suggested that people tend to learn from prestigious others (e.g., Colleran et al., 2014). Rich (i.e., economically successful) individuals may be regarded as a model of social learning. Consequently, individuals living in rich provinces might have acquired independent tendency through social learning from rich people, who have acquired independent tendency based on their own richness through individual-level processes.

The second possibility is based on the social norms and institutions, particularly norms of modern market exchange, that people create as a group. These norms may promote independent tendency by facilitating interactions among strangers (Greenfield et al., 2003; Henrich et al., 2010a). At the individual level, the extent to which an individual is integrated into market exchange (e.g., reliant on markets in obtaining resources) may be correlated with the amount of money he or she has, as money affords means to obtain resources through market exchange. Consider a case of labor outsourcing, such as baby-sitting. One could either arrange childcare through their own social network (e.g., asking a friend or a relative) or through market exchange, and rich individuals are more able to rely on the latter. At the societal level, presence of a large number of market-integrated individuals likely indicates the salience of social norms relevant to exchange with strangers, which in turn may promote independent tendency (Yuki and Takemura, 2013). Research supports this rationale. For example, members of herding communities, in comparison to farming communities, interact with strangers more (Uskul and Over, 2014) and have more independent tendency (Witkin and Berry, 1975). Similarly, Leung and Cohen (2011) argued that the presence of modern markets is connected to a culturally shared notion that individual worth is inalienable (vs. socially conferred), which is an important aspect of independent culture (Heine, 2005). These findings imply that in provinces where many individuals are engaged in market exchange and use money to obtain resources, norms that facilitate independent tendency are collectively created.

The third explanation is the role of choice that wealth affords at the individual level (e.g., Triandis et al., 1988). While originally this theory might have suggested that it is the amount of choices that an individual has that matters, our findings suggest that what matters more is the amount of choices that an individual sees. Presumably, there are more shops, goods, and services in the wealthier provinces, and individuals would perceive a greater range of options and possibilities for goods and services to select from in richer provinces regardless of one’s own richness. In turn, perceiving a wider range of options and possibilities may motivate individuals to pursue personal success and achievement over interpersonal harmony with others.

In summary, the current study contributes to the literature by identifying the role of social processes in the wealth-independence link. The current findings, in turn, shed light on a new question: What are the specific social mechanisms underlying the wealth-independence link? Further research on this issue would be instrumental in advancing understanding of the association between wealth and independent psychological processes.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study design. Data collection and the data analysis were performed by KT. KT and TH drafted the manuscript, and YG and SS provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article: Graduate School of Management, Kyoto University.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

The authors thank Dr. Thomas Talhelm and Dr. Yukiko Uchida for their comments.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00384

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://english.gov.cn/
  2. ^ http://chinadataonline.org

References

Allik, J., and Realo, A. (2004). Individualism-collectivism and social capital. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 35, 29–49. doi: 10.1177/0022022103260381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, M. B., and Chen, Y.-R. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychol. Rev. 114, 133–151. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, M. B., and Yuki, M. (2007). “Culture and social identity,” in Handbook of Cultural Psychology, eds S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (New York, NY: Guilford), 307–322.

Google Scholar

Christ, O., Schmid, K., Lolliot, S., Swart, H., Stolle, D., Tausch, N., et al. (2014). Contextual effect of positive intergroup contact on outgroup prejudice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 3996–4000. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320901111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colleran, H., Jasienska, G., Nenko, I., Galbarczyk, A., and Mace, R. (2014). Community-level education accelerates the cultural evolution of fertility decline. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281:20132732. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, C.-R., Yuan, Y., and Guo, J. (2013). The latest features of China’s floating population. Northwest Popul. J. 34, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Enders, C. K., and Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol. Methods 12, 121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D., and Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proc. Biol. Sci. 275, 1279–1285. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleisher, B., Li, H., and Zhao, M. Q. (2010). Human capital, economic growth, and regional inequality in China. J. Dev. Econ. 92, 215–231. doi: 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2009.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science 332, 1100–1104. doi: 10.1126/science.1197754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgas, J., van de Vijver, F. J. R., and Berry, J. W. (2004). The ecocultural framework, ecosocial indices, and psychological variables in cross-cultural research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 35, 74–96. doi: 10.1177/0022022103260459

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., and Childs, C. P. (2003). Historical change, cultural learning, and cognitive representation in Zinacantec Maya children. Cogn. Dev. 18, 455–487. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., and Varnum, M. E. W. (2010). Social class, culture, and cognition. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 181–189.

Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., and Varnum, M. E. W. (2015). Social structure, infectious diseases, disasters, secularism, and cultural change in America. Psychol. Sci. 26, 311–324. doi: 10.1177/0956797614563765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism-collectivism in the U.S. and Japan. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 3–24. doi: 10.1177/1088868311411587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamamura, T., Xu, Q., and Du, Y. (2013). Culture, social class, and independence-interdependence: the case of Chinese adolescents. Int. J. Psychol. 48, 344–351. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2011.647030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heine, S. J. (2005). “Constructing good selves in Japan and North America,” in Proceedings of the Culture and Social Behavior: The Tenth Ontario Symposium, eds R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J. M. Olson, and M. P. Zanna (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 115–143.

Google Scholar

Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural Psychology. New York, NY: Norton.

Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Gintis, H., Fehr, E., Camerer, C., et al. (2005). ‘Economic man’ in cross-cultural perspective: ethnography and experiments from 15 small-scale societies. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 795–855. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., et al. (2010a). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327, 1480–1484. doi: 10.1126/science.1182238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010b). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Inglehart, R., and Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. Am. Sociol. Rev. 165, 19–51. doi: 10.2307/2657288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitayama, S., and Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: current evidence and future directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 419–449. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohn, M. L., Naoi, A., Schoenbach, C., Schooler, C., and Slomczynski, K. M. (1990). Position in the class structure and psychological functioning in the United States, Japan, and Poland. Am. J. Sociol. 95, 964–1008. doi: 10.1086/229382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W., and Keltner, D. (2009). Signs of socioeconomic status: a thin-slicing approach. Psychol. Sci. 20, 99–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02251.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., Rheinschmidt, M. L., and Keltner, D. (2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol. Rev. 119, 546–572. doi: 10.1037/a0028756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A. K.-Y., and Cohen, D. (2011). Within- and between-culture variation: individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 507–526. doi: 10.1037/a0022151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Locke, K. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: reliability, validity, and applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. J. Pers. Assess. 75, 249–267. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7502_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maas, C. J. M., and Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology 1, 86–92. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.1.3.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, Y., and Wilken, B. (2010). Culturally contingent situated cognition: influencing other people fosters analytic perception in the United States but not in Japan. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1616–1622. doi: 10.1177/0956797610385954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morling, B., Kitayama, S., and Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 311–323. doi: 10.1177/0146167202286003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., and Nisbett, R. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible to individual differences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 6192–6197. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001911107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, S., and Kisling, J. (2009). “The mutual constitution of residential mobility and individualism,” in Understanding Culture: Theory, Research, and Application, eds R. S. Wyer, C.-Y. Chiu, and Y.-Y. Hong (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 223–238.

Google Scholar

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 128, 3–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raudenbush, S. W., and Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models, 2nd Edn. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., and Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: a valid and important dimension of cultural differences between nations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 17–31. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0901_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shweder, R. (1991). “Cultural psychology: what is it?,” in Thinking Through Cultures: Expeditions in Cultural Psychology, ed. R. A. Shweder (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press), 73–110.

Google Scholar

Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20, 580–591. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snibbe, A. C., and Markus, H. R. (2005). You can’t always get what you want: educational attainment, agency, and choice. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 703–720. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., and Townsend, S. S. M. (2007). Choice as an act of meaning: the case of social class. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 814–830. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., et al. (2014). Discovery of large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice vs. wheat agriculture. Science 344, 603–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tay, L., Morrison, M., and Diener, E. (2014). Living among the affluent: boon or bane? Psychol. Sci. 25, 1235–1241. doi: 10.1177/0956797614525786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & Collectivism. Boulder: Westview Press.

Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., and Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 323–338. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., Seppala, E., Fung, H. H., and Yeung, D. Y. (2007). Influence and adjustment goals: sources of cultural differences in ideal affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 1102–1117. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uskul, A., and Over, H. (2014). Responses to social exclusion in cultural context: evidence from farming and herding communities. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 106, 752–771. doi: 10.1037/a0035810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandello, J. A., and Cohen, D. (1999). Patterns of individualism and collectivism across the United States. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 279–292. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varnum, M. E. W., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., and Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: the social orientation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 9–13. doi: 10.1177/0963721409359301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Witkin, H. A., and Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1, 5–87.

Google Scholar

Yamagishi, T. (2011). “Micro-macro dynamics of the cultural construction of reality: a niche construction approach to culture,” in Advances in Culture and Psychology, Vol. 1, eds M. J. Gelfand, C.-Y. Chiu, and Y.-Y. Hong (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 251–308.

Google Scholar

Yamagishi, T., and Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motiv. Emot. 18, 129–166. doi: 10.1007/BF02249397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, D. T. (2002). What has caused regional inequality in China? China Econ. Rev. 13, 331–334. doi: 10.1016/S1043-951X(02)00088-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., and Takemura, K. (2005). Cross-cultural differences in relationship- and group-based trust. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 48–62. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuki, M., and Takemura, K. (2013). “Intergroup comparison and intragroup relationships: group processes in the cultures of individualism and collectivism,” in Frontiers of Culture and Psychology: Culture and Group Processes, eds M. Yuki and M. B. Brewer (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 38–65.

Google Scholar

Keywords: culture, independence, individualism, economic development, multilevel, contextual effect

Citation: Takemura K, Hamamura T, Guan Y and Suzuki S (2016) Contextual Effect of Wealth on Independence: An Examination through Regional Differences in China. Front. Psychol. 7:384. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00384

Received: 22 November 2015; Accepted: 03 March 2016;
Published: 17 March 2016.

Edited by:

Andrew Ryder, Concordia University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Yulia Chentsova Dutton, Georgetown University, USA
Igor Grossmann, University of Waterloo, Canada

Copyright © 2016 Takemura, Hamamura, Guan and Suzuki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kosuke Takemura, boz.takemura@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.