Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 02 June 2015
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Prefrontal cortical areas linked to cognitive function - do we have it all tied down? View all 5 articles

Cognitive control, emotional value, and the lateral prefrontal cortex

  • Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuroscience of Thought Laboratory, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Introduction

Cognitive control refers to the intentional selection of thoughts, emotions, and behaviors based on current task demands and social context, and the concomitant suppression of inappropriate habitual actions (Miller and Cohen, 2001). Common situations that require cognitive control include: studying for an exam while resisting the impulse to check Facebook; having fruit instead of dessert when on a diet; and being patient with one's kids instead of yelling at them for spilling juice on the carpet. Historically, there has been preferential interest in how cognitive control operates. An influential model suggests that the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) represents rules or instructions in working memory and that this information adaptively guides perceptual and motor processing in posterior brain regions, thus resulting in the selection of appropriate behaviors, and the suppression of maladaptive habitual actions (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Bunge, 2004). Surprisingly, there has been less focus on why individuals choose to engage cognitive control in the first place (Dixon and Christoff, 2012; Botvinick and Braver, 2015). In this article, I outline a value-based framework of cognitive control which suggests that: (1) individuals choose to engage cognitive control when they expect that it will produce an emotionally valued outcome (i.e., a reward or the avoidance of punishment); (2) the LPFC is a critical neural substrate involved in representing the value of engaging cognitive control; and (3) the LPFC is organized along a rostro-caudal axis, with different sub-regions contributing to different elements of the decision to employ cognitive control.

Cognitive Control and Emotional Value

A number of studies have shown that individuals are naturally disinclined to engage cognitive control (McGuire and Botvinick, 2010; Dixon and Christoff, 2012; Botvinick and Braver, 2015). For example, when given the choice between two tasks, individuals will reliably choose the easier task (McGuire and Botvinick, 2010). It requires considerable effort to intentionally direct action, and this may often be experienced as aversive. This has led to the idea that cognitive control carries an intrinsic effort cost (Botvinick and Braver, 2015). Accordingly, individuals may only engage cognitive control if they think that it will produce an emotionally valuable outcome that outweighs this effort cost.

We examined this idea by offering participants the choice between engaging cognitive control or selecting a habitual action, and varying the amount of money they earned based on their choices (Dixon and Christoff, 2012). The results were striking: participants invariably selected the habitual action when it was expected to yield an equal or larger monetary reward. In contrast, participants frequently chose to engage cognitive control when it was expected to result in a larger monetary reward than the habitual action. This suggests that the anticipated emotional value of the monetary outcome was a critical factor influencing the decision of whether or not to engage cognitive control. This finding complements other work demonstrating that reward incentives often lead to faster and more accurate performance (Locke and Braver, 2008; Jimura et al., 2010; Padmala and Pessoa, 2011; Chiew and Braver, 2013; Etzel et al., 2015).

While it is clear that cognitive control and emotional value systems do not operate independent of one another (Watanabe and Sakagami, 2007; Pessoa, 2008; Dixon and Christoff, 2014), the precise mechanism underlying their interaction remains to be determined. I suggest that the brain flexibly creates temporary bindings or associations between rules for action that support cognitive control and the emotional outcomes that are expected to be obtained from rule-use (Figure 1). When a rule becomes associated with a high-value outcome, this may sharpen and stabilize the rule representation in working memory (Etzel et al., 2015), thereby enabling individuals to hold this information in mind until it has successfully guided behavior. Thus, if a student is deliberating between going to the movies or studying for an exam, the student will be more likely to resist the immediate pleasure of going to the movies if they focus on the causal relationship between studying (which requires cognitive control) and the desired outcome it will produce (a good grade). If the student's attention is merely drawn to the effort of exerting cognitive control, they will be more likely to go to the movies. Similarly, when parents use the phrase “because I said so” after requesting their child to exert self-control, they should not be surprised when the child does not comply, because they have not provided the child with an incentive to invest the effort.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Rule-outcome associations in the LPFC. Information about rules may be first registered in regions including the posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC), while information about rewards and punishment may be first registered in regions including the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), insula, and pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pgACC). This information may then be passed to the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) where it is temporarily bound into a rule-outcome association that specifies whether engaging cognitive control is expected to result in an emotionally valued outcome. Other value regions including the ventral striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex may also interact with the LPFC in a context dependent manner.

An important implication is that in some cases, individual and group differences in cognitive control may be erroneously attributed to differences in “cognitive” mechanisms (e.g., the ability to hold rules in mind, reasoning, response inhibition), when in fact it reflects differences in the perceived emotional value of engaging those cognitive mechanisms. Furthermore, there may be substantial individual differences in the types of incentives that effectively motivate cognitive control—some individuals may be more responsive to monetary rewards, others to food rewards, and others to social praise, intrinsic motivation, or the avoidance of punishment.

The Lateral Prefrontal Cortex and Rule-Outcome Associations

Is there evidence that the brain flexibly creates temporary associations between rules that support cognitive control and the emotional value of expected outcomes? A number of studies have provided indirect evidence for this idea, showing that cognitive control related activation in the LPFC is amplified by reward incentives (Kouneiher et al., 2009; Beck et al., 2010; Jimura et al., 2010; Padmala and Pessoa, 2011). To directly examine this hypothesis, we had participants perform a cognitive control task in which they used one of two rules to respond to stimuli on each trial, and earned one of two monetary outcomes based on their performance (Dixon and Christoff, 2012). Using fMRI-adaptation, we looked for any regions that were sensitive to specific rule-outcome pairings. We found that the LPFC exhibited precisely this effect. Notably, the relevant rules and monetary outcome changed from trial to trial suggesting that the LPFC was continuously forming and then dissolving such associations in a flexible manner. In contrast to the LPFC, other value-related regions including the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and ventral striatum were sensitive to the expected reward outcome, but did not encode rule-outcome associations. Recent theoretical work has suggested that the mid-cingulate cortex may play an important role in determining the value of engaging cognitive control (Shenhav et al., 2013), however, the findings from this study favor the interpretation that the LPFC is the critical region in determining when it is worth it to intentionally direct action. This idea is consistent with recent evidence demonstrating involvement of the LPFC in comparing the value of different actions (Morris et al., 2014). In particular, the LPFC has been implicated in “model-based” decision making—the construction of an internal model of the world, including the relationships between context, task-rules, and anticipated outcomes, in order to decide between choice options (Smittenaar et al., 2013; Buckholtz, 2015). Buckholtz (2015) has convincingly argued that the LPFC's capacity to construct rule-outcome associations in a model-based manner is a critical computation underlying adaptive social behavior.

How does the LPFC create rule-outcome associations? One possibility is that information about rules and information about reward/punishment is first registered in separate specialized posterior brain regions, and then is passed to the LPFC which creates a temporary association between this information within working memory (Figure 1). Consistent with this idea, we found that the LPFC exhibited synchronized activation with reward processing regions (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex, ventral striatum, insula, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex) and with rule processing regions (e.g., posterior parietal cortex, posterior middle temporal gyrus) when participants engaged cognitive control in order to obtain a monetary outcome (Dixon and Christoff, 2012). Extending these findings, recent work has shown that a frontoparietal network (including the LPFC) flexibly shifts connectivity patterns with other parts of the brain from trial to trial based on task demands (Cole et al., 2013). In line with these findings, LPFC neurons do not exhibit an intrinsic tuning toward a specific type of information, but rather, flexibly code information that is currently relevant on a given trial (Stokes et al., 2013). Thus, the adaptive coding properties and widespread anatomical connections of the LPFC may allow for the temporary construction of associations between diverse inputs, including rule and value information.

Emotion Regulation as Value-based Cognitive Control

This value-based framework of cognitive control may provide a new understanding of how the LPFC contributes to emotion regulation. Emotion regulation often involves the effortful use of rules or cognitive strategies in order to alter one's emotional state (Ochsner and Gross, 2005; Gross, 2015). Traditionally, the LPFC has been linked to the implementation of these strategies, such as attentional distraction, reasoning about emotions, and reinterpreting the meaning of events (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). However, the LPFC may also play an important role in the decision to initiate emotion regulation. Here are two examples of how it may contribute: (1) it may be involved in determining when emotion regulation is necessary by representing the value of a current emotional state in relation to social rules (e.g., determining that it is not appropriate to feel/express anger because one is currently at a work meeting); and (2) it may be involved in determining the value of employing effortful regulation strategies by representing the relationship between a given strategy (e.g., reappraisal of an event's meaning) and a desired outcome (e.g., less anger). Thus, the decision to instantiate emotion regulation may depend on the flexible synthesis of rule-outcome associations. This perspective is compatible with Gross (2015) extended process model of emotional regulation, which emphasizes the role of second-order valuation mechanisms in triggering the engagement of emotion regulation strategies. The two examples noted above broadly correspond to the identification and selection stages of Gross' model, respectively.

The Hierarchical Organization of the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

Accumulating evidence suggests that the LPFC is hierarchically organized along a rostral to caudal (anterior to posterior) axis (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Koechlin et al., 2003; Petrides, 2005; Bunge and Zelazo, 2006; Badre and D'Esposito, 2009; Christoff et al., 2009; Dixon et al., 2014). I consider how this hierarchical organization relates to the decision of whether or not to engage cognitive control. The rostral LPFC (area 10), mid-LPFC (areas 9/46/45), and caudal LPFC (areas 44/8/6) are discussed in turn.

Cognitive control often operates in service of desired long-term outcomes (e.g., studying over the course of a semester in order to get an “A”). The rostral LPFC plays a role in meta-cognitive awareness (Fleming et al., 2010; McCaig et al., 2011; De Martino et al., 2013) and relational processing (Christoff et al., 2001; Wendelken et al., 2008). It may contribute to the decision to engage cognitive control by enabling individuals to reflect on their thoughts and feelings, and to establish long-term priorities by comparing the value of potential future outcomes. Consistent with this, the rostral LPFC is recruited when individuals plan steps to attain a future goal (Gerlach et al., 2014), choose to avoid situations that may interfere with the attainment of future rewards (Crockett et al., 2013), monitor progress toward a desired future reward (Dixon et al., 2014), and select actions directed toward future rather than immediate rewards (McClure et al., 2004; Jimura et al., 2013). This region may work in concert with the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which is also involved in representing the affective value of future scenarios (D'Argembeau et al., 2008; Benoit et al., 2014; Gerlach et al., 2014). One possibility is that the VMPFC represents the value of potential future outcome, and the RLPFC contributes to the selection of one outcome to pursue as a goal. Interestingly, meditation practice is associated with increased gray matter volume in the rostral LPFC (Fox et al., 2014), suggesting that this meta-cognitive capacity to step back and reflect on thoughts (including desired future outcomes) can be trained and improved.

Whereas the RLPFC plays a role in decisions regarding overarching priorities, the mid-LPFC operates on a shorter time-scale, contributing to the decision of whether or not to employ rules to intentionally direct action at any given moment. This decision depends on discerning whether the outcomes expected from rule-use are sufficiently valuable to offset the effort cost. Thus, a critical component is constructing rule-outcome associations. Notably, our fMRI-adaptation study revealed robust encoding of rule-outcome associations in the mid-LPFC (Dixon and Christoff, 2012). Consistent with this, recent findings indicate that mid-LPFC activation reflects an interaction between the complexity of rules that are required to respond to stimuli and the size of an expected reward outcome, and this neural response correlates with individual differences in behavior (Bahlmann et al., 2015). Furthermore, mid-LPFC is reliably activated in studies of emotion regulation (Buhle et al., 2014), and may be involved in representing associations between rules/strategies (e.g., reappraisal) and desired outcomes (e.g., less sadness). The function of the mid-LPFC may depend on transient interactions with rule-processing and value-processing regions, and such network interactions may depend on context. The ventral striatum is activated during the anticipation of imminent rewards and the opportunity to exercise choice (Knutson et al., 2001; Leotti and Delgado, 2014), and may provide motivational signals to the mid-LPFC that promotes the engagement of cognitive control to obtain immediately available rewards. In contrast, the opportunity to make progress toward a desired future outcome may engage cognitive control via positive interactions between the rostral LPFC, mid-LPFC, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and inhibitory interactions between these regions and the ventral striatum (Diekhof and Gruber, 2010; Jimura et al., 2013; Van Den Bos et al., 2014).

Once the decision to engage cognitive control has been made, it requires that rules be translated into specific voluntary actions. The caudal part of the LPFC is involved in this process (Koechlin et al., 2003; Bunge, 2004; Petrides, 2005). The caudal LPFC is strongly activated during the execution of actions (Dixon et al., 2014), and facilitates the execution of context appropriate actions over competing actions by representing sensorimotor associations (Koechlin et al., 2003; Bunge, 2004; Petrides, 2005). Thus, caudal LPFC activity reflects the embodied output of the decision process—the behavioral instantiation of cognitive control.

Conclusion

Considerable evidence suggests individuals will engage cognitive control when they expect that it will produce a valued outcome that outweighs the intrinsic effort cost—this may be an immediate or future reward, or a desired change in one's emotional state in the absence of external incentives. The LPFC contributes to this process, with the rostral LPFC representing desired long-term outcomes, the mid-LPFC supporting the flexible construction of rule-outcome associations, and the caudal LPFC translating rules into specific voluntary actions. The LPFC operates in the context of interactions with widely distributed rule and value-related regions (Dixon and Christoff, 2012, 2014). Much remains to be learned about the mechanisms underlying the decision to engage cognitive control, making this an important topic for future research.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Badre, D., and D'Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 659–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahlmann, J., Aarts, E., and D'Esposito, M. (2015). Influence of motivation on control hierarchy in the human frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 3207–3217. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2389-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, S. M., Locke, H. S., Savine, A. C., Jimura, K., and Braver, T. S. (2010). Primary and secondary rewards differentially modulate neural activity dynamics during working memory. PLoS ONE 5:e9251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benoit, R. G., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex supports affective future simulation by integrating distributed knowledge. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 16550–16555. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419274111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 83–113. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckholtz, J. W. (2015). Social norms, self-control, and the value of antisocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., et al. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex. 24, 2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunge, S. A., and Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 118–121. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00419.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunge, S. A. (2004). How we use rules to select actions: a review of evidence from cognitive neuroscience. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 564–579. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.4.564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiew, K. S., and Braver, T. S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Front. Psychol. 4:15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28, 168–186. doi: 10.3758/BF03331976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., Keramatian, K., Gordon, A. M., Smith, R., and Madler, B. (2009). Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction. Brain Res. 1286, 94–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.05.096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., Prabhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Zhao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., et al. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14, 1136–1149. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., and Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crockett, M. J., Braams, B. R., Clark, L., Tobler, P. N., Robbins, T. W., and Kalenscher, T. (2013). Restricting temptations: neural mechanisms of precommitment. Neuron 79, 391–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Argembeau, A., Xue, G., Lu, Z. L., Van Der Linden, M., and Bechara, A. (2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. Neuroimage 40, 398–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Martino, B., Fleming, S. M., Garrett, N., and Dolan, R. J. (2013). Confidence in value-based choice. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 105–110. doi: 10.1038/nn.3279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekhof, E. K., and Gruber, O. (2010). When desire collides with reason: functional interactions between anteroventral prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens underlie the human ability to resist impulsive desires. J. Neurosci. 30, 1488–1493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4690-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., and Christoff, K. (2012). The decision to engage cognitive control is driven by expected reward-value: neural and behavioral evidence. PLoS ONE 7:e51637. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., and Christoff, K. (2014). The lateral prefrontal cortex and complex value-based learning and decision making. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., Fox, K. C. R., and Christoff, K. (2014). Evidence for rostro-caudal functional organization in multiple brain areas related to goal-directed behavior. Brain Res. 1572, 26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etzel, J. A., Cole, M. W., Zacks, J. M., Kay, K. N., and Braver, T. S. (2015). Reward motivation enhances task coding in frontoparietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu327. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J., and Rees, G. (2010). Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 329, 1541–1543. doi: 10.1126/science.1191883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., et al. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 48–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlach, K. D., Spreng, R. N., Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Future planning: Default network activity couples with frontoparietal control network and reward-processing regions during process and outcome simulations. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1942–1951. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol.Inq. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimura, K., Chushak, M. S., and Braver, T. S. (2013). Impulsivity and self-control during intertemporal decision making linked to the neural dynamics of reward value representation. J. Neurosci. 33, 344–357. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002007107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., and Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 8871–8876. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002007107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., and Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21:RC159.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Koechlin, E., Ody, C., and Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 302, 1181–1185. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., and Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 939–945. doi: 10.1038/nn.2321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leotti, L. A., and Delgado, M. R. (2014). The value of exercising control over monetary gains and losses. Psychol. Sci. 25, 596–604. doi: 10.1177/0956797613514589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Locke, H. S., and Braver, T. S. (2008). Motivational influences on cognitive control: behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 99–112. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCaig, R. G., Dixon, M., Keramatian, K., Liu, I., and Christoff, K. (2011). Improved modulation of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex using real-time fMRI training and meta-cognitive awareness. Neuroimage 55, 1298–1305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, J. T., and Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 7922–7926. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910662107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R. W., Dezfouli, A., Griffiths, K. R., and Balleine, B. W. (2014). Action-value comparisons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex control choice between goal-directed actions. Nat. Commun. 5:4390. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmala, S., and Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3419–3432. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 148–158. doi: 10.1038/nrn2317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79, 217–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smittenaar, P., Fitzgerald, Thomash, B., Romei, V., Wright, N. D., and Dolan, R J. (2013). Disruption of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases model-based in favor of model-free control in humans. Neuron. 80, 914–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokes, M. G., Kusunoki, M., Sigala, N., Nili, H., Gaffan, D., and Duncan, J. (2013). Dynamic coding for cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 78, 364–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Bos, W., Rodriguez, C. A., Schweitzer, J. B., and Mcclure, S. M. (2014). Connectivity strength of dissociable striatal tracts predict individual differences in temporal discounting. J. Neurosci. 34, 10298–10310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4105-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, M., and Sakagami, M. (2007). Integration of cognitive and motivational context information in the primate prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17(Suppl. 1), i101–i109. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendelken, C., Nakhabenko, D., Donohue, S. E., Carter, C. S., and Bunge, S. A. (2008). “Brain is to thought as stomach is to ??”: investigating the role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in relational reasoning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 682–693. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive control, value, prefrontal cortex, motivation, self-control, emotion

Citation: Dixon ML (2015) Cognitive control, emotional value, and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Psychol. 6:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00758

Received: 07 April 2015; Accepted: 21 May 2015;
Published: 02 June 2015.

Edited by:

Anita Must, University of Szeged, Hungary

Reviewed by:

Kimberly Sarah Chiew, Duke University, USA
Mauricio R. Delgado, Rutgers-Newark: The State University of New Jersey, USA

Copyright © 2015 Dixon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matthew L. Dixon, mattdixon@psych.ubc.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.