- 1Institute of Psychology, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
- 2Courant Research Center Evolution of Social Behavior, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
- 3Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- 4Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, MI, USA
People judge attractiveness and make trait inferences from the physical appearance of others, and research reveals high agreement among observers making such judgments. Evolutionary psychologists have argued that interest in physical appearance and beauty reflects adaptations that motivate the search for desirable qualities in a potential partner. Although men more than women value the physical appearance of a partner, appearance universally affects social perception in both sexes. Most studies of attractiveness perceptions have focused on third party assessments of static representations of the face and body. Corroborating evidence suggests that body movement, such as dance, also conveys information about mate quality. Here we review evidence that dynamic cues (e.g., gait, dance) also influence perceptions of mate quality, including personality traits, strength, and overall attractiveness. We recommend that attractiveness research considers the informational value of body movement in addition to static cues, to present an integrated perspective on human social perception.
Perception of Physical Appearance
The morphology of the face and body affects people’s perceptions of others, and this has consequences for human social interaction (Fink and Penton-Voak, 2002; Grammer et al., 2003a; Gangestad and Scheyd, 2005; Rhodes, 2006). Physical appearance influences many social decisions, including consumer behavior, economic and recruitment decisions, and even legislation (Rhode, 2010; Hammermesh, 2011; Saad, 2011). Dion et al. (1972) first demonstrated that the assignation of desirable traits to attractive people (“what is beautiful is good”) serves as a cognitive heuristic deployed in social perception. Since this seminal study, researchers from several disciplines have investigated the causes of this assessment heuristic (for review, see Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2008). Evolutionary scientists argue that the human interest in facial and body morphology—and the social perceptions evoked by physical features—are neither arbitrary nor culture-bound, but instead reflect the operation of adaptations shaped by sexual selection (Grammer et al., 2003a,b; Little et al., 2011). These adaptations motivate the search for desirable qualities in a potential romantic partner (Buss, 1989, 1994; Etcoff, 1999). Although the evidence obtained from studies of faces and bodies supports this evolutionary perspective on attractiveness perceptions, studies of static cues capture a limited range of those present in social interaction.
Attractiveness research has relied heavily on the use of static representations (i.e., images) of the human face and body, thus neglecting the fact that in social situations we rarely observe others in this condition. In a meta-analysis, Langlois et al. (2000) concluded that there is variability in the results of attractiveness research with regard to the use of stimuli types (i.e., photographs, videos, and in situ encounters) and that this variability could produce different attractiveness judgments. With regard to face perception however, recent research does not confirm this assertion. For example, Koscinski (2013) reported a large positive correlation between attractiveness perceptions of static images and videos of the same faces, indicating that results of studies relying on attractiveness assessments of static images are ecologically valid. Rhodes et al. (2011) arrived at a similar conclusion, suggesting that the validity of static and dynamic images of faces in attractiveness research might result from the tendency to make rapid and robust attributions. There is evidence that people make trait inferences from physical appearance in milliseconds. In five experiments on assessments of unfamiliar faces, varying only in exposure time, Willis and Todorov (2006) demonstrated that judgments made after a 100 ms exposure correlated highly with judgments made in the absence of time constraints. However, it was also found that increased exposure time led to more differentiated person impressions. With increased exposure time, observers made a more fine-grained discrimination of trait attributes such that the effect of attractiveness on trait judgment was smaller.
Perception of Body Movement
Rapid trait attribution has also been documented for perceptions of others’ body movements. Although the time required to accurately assess another person based on movement cues is not yet known, one can speculate that it is longer than that reported for face perception. Thoresen et al. (2012) presented point-light walkers to participants and identified the movement components that accounted for personality judgments. Observers made reliable trait judgments using a small number of movement components. Moreover, systematic manipulations of these components affected people’s judgments, indicating that certain movement cues (or combinations of them) influence personality perception. Observers were not necessarily accurate in their assessments however, as movement components were associated with perceived, but not with self-reported personality. Further analyses indicated that accuracy of trait ratings was accounted for by observers’ perceptions of emotion, attractiveness, and masculinity of the walkers. These results corroborate that of previous studies, and provide evidence that observers can identify basic emotions from body movement even when information about body morphology is impoverished, through—for example—the use of point-light displays (e.g., Atkinson et al., 2004; Clarke et al., 2005).
Most studies on the perception of human body movement have used visual representations of human walk (predominantly point-light displays) in their experimental paradigm. A rare exception is Dittrich et al. (1996), which showed that the conclusions about social perception derived from studies of human walk also apply to dance. These researchers instructed professional dancers to convey specific emotions (i.e., anger, disgust, fear, grief, joy, surprise) via body movements, which were captured and transferred into point-light displays. Participants’ recognition rate of the displayed emotions from point-lights was high (63% correct) and greater when fully lit videos of the dancers were used as stimuli (88% correct). Similar recognition rates have been found for human gait. Troje et al. (2005) presented point-light stimuli of human walkers from different viewpoint angles to male and female observers. The stimuli were altered such that observers viewed the normal (unaltered) walk, walks in which all stimuli had been normalized with respect to body size or body shape, and two conditions in which walking frequency was altered. After an initial presentation in which observers viewed the stimuli and were told the names of the walkers, they then received a series of training sessions. Recognition performance reached 90% after five training sessions. Observers accurately identified an individual when the stimuli had been normalized for body shape and walking frequency, leading the researchers to conclude that structural information (i.e., body morphology) plays a secondary role to gait kinematics in the identification of a person. This finding provides insight into the relative significance of certain cues (static and/or dynamic cues) in social perception by emphasizing the role of dynamic cues.
The ability to recognize emotions of conspecifics from their body movements, however, does not necessarily inform us about people’s preferences for certain types of body movement, and how such preferences may affect the way individuals form impression about others, including decisions about potential romantic partners. Mate selection forms an integral part of the sociocultural world, and as such we might expect that the sensitivity to movement cues extends to other features derived from movement, such as gender identity, sexual orientation, health, and personality. With regard to sexual orientation, for example, Johnson et al. (2007) presented computer-generated animations of humanoid characters (avatars) that varied morphologically (five levels of waist-to-hip ratio, WHR; Singh, 2002, 2006) and dynamically (five levels of motion ranging from extreme shoulder swagger to extreme hip sway). Judgments of sexual orientation of walkers perceived to be male were strongly affected by motion, but not morphology, whereas judgments of sexual orientation of walkers perceived to be female were affected by motion and morphology.
Dance and Interpersonal Attraction
In the context of interpersonal attraction, studies investigating the role of body movement have focused on dance movements, perhaps because dance shows greater inter-individual variation than gait or running, and because dance is typically observed in courtship situations. Dance is a series of body movements composed of purposeful, intentional, rhythmical, and culturally patterned sequences that are part of the courtship practices of many cultures (Kurath, 1960; Kaeppler, 1978; Hanna, 1987). Hanna (1987, 2010) argued that dance is produced by sexually selected mechanisms designed to display beauty, health, strength, and sexual attractiveness. Recent research supports this hypothesis, as there is evidence that dance movements, particularly those of men, correlate with characteristics relevant to mate selection.
For example, Hugill et al. (2009) and McCarty et al. (2013) reported a positive relationship between women’s perceptions of the attractiveness and dance quality of men’s dances with the male’s physical strength (as measured via hand-grip strength). Recent research has confirmed the attractiveness-strength relationship in men, and reported its absence in women (Weege et al., in press b). Moreover, women’s assessments of men’s dance attractiveness were found to correlate with particular personality traits, including sensation-seeking (Hugill et al., 2011) and self-reported global personality descriptors (Fink et al., 2012b). Thus, there is evidence that, similar to the findings on associations of physical strength and male facial appearance (Fink et al., 2007), partner attractiveness and risk-taking behavior (Henderson et al., 2005), women make attributions about such qualities using cues found in men’s dance movements. Female sensitivity to these cues affects their mate preferences, as women judge dances of physically stronger men and those who score higher on sensation seeking to be more attractive. Hence men may convey information about their mate quality via dance movements1.
In the attempt to identify body movements that characterize a “good” male dancer, Neave et al. (2011) motion-captured dance movements of men dancing to a basic drumbeat. Women rated videos of shape-standardized humanoid avatars for perceived dance quality. “Good” dancers displayed larger and more variable bending and twisting movements of their head/neck and torso, and faster bending and twisting movements of their right knee. Given that comparative biological studies have suggested that females prefer vigorous and skilled males, Neave et al. (2011) concluded that women derive information about physical condition, including health, fitness, and genetic quality from male dancing ability. Weege et al. (2012) presented videos of pairs of “good” and “bad” dancers in an eye-tracking experiment and recorded dwell time and fixations of female participants, in addition to securing attractiveness and masculinity ratings for each dancer. “Good” male dancers received greater visual attention and were judged to be more attractive and masculine than “bad” male dancers. These results indicate a cognitive bias that facilitates mating-related motives [see Maner et al.’s (2003) “beauty captures the mind of the beholder” hypothesis].
Two studies investigated whether women’s body movements influence their attractiveness to men and, in particular, whether this relationship is mediated by female fertility. Using point-light displays, Provost et al. (2008) found that women at high-fertility (versus low-fertility) displayed gaits that were less attractive to men. In contrast, Fink et al. (2012a) used silhouette displays and found that women at high-fertility displayed gaits and dances that men rated as more attractive. Although it is unclear what might account for these conflicting results, there is independent evidence corroborating the findings from Fink et al. (2012a) For example, women at high-fertility (versus low-fertility) report increased sexual desire and wear more attractive clothing, and female lap dancers at high-fertility (versus low-fertility) report higher earnings (see Haselton and Gildersleeve, 2011). Thus, at high-fertility, women may alter their behavior—including the manner in which they walk and dance—to appear more attractive to men.
People derive information from body movements, particularly dance movements, and this affects attractiveness judgments and social perceptions. However, because most of this previous work included only European participants (with some bias toward the study of male dance), it is unknown whether the results generalize to other countries/societies. There is mixed evidence for claims regarding the universality of certain human features and displays. A recent example is facial expressions of emotions, which has been a matter of debate since Darwin’s (1872) seminal publication. For more than 100 years, it has been asserted that all humans communicate six basic emotional states using the same facial expressions/movements (Ekman and Friesen, 1971; Ekman et al., 1987). A recent study, however, refutes this hypothesis (Jack et al., 2012), showing that people from Western and Eastern cultures differ in their mental representations of the six emotions in terms of associated facial movements. There has been controversy about the Jack et al. (2012) study, with some scholars arguing that the findings do not refute the universality of facial expressions of emotions, but instead contribute to the investigation of perceptual consistencies and differences of specific emotional facial cues (Sauter and Eisner, 2013). A likely scenario is that evolved cognitive mechanisms and socio-cultural variables affect the perception of emotions from facial expressions, as has been reported for gestures and postures (Argyle, 1988; Moore et al., 2010). We suspect a similar interaction between evolved mechanisms and socio-cultural variables that also affects assessments of body movement.
Cross-Cultural Perception of Body Movement
Although cross-cultural similarities and differences in facial expressions of emotions have received attention from researchers in many disciplines (for review, see Russell et al., 2003), little research has focused on cross-cultural similarities and differences in perceptions of body movements. The fact that the ability to decode information from point-light animations of movement is done rapidly and accurately—and from an early age (Simion et al., 2008), and that this processing is served by dedicated neurological components (e.g., Grossman et al., 2000, 2005)—suggests that the detection and interpretation of biological motion may be produced by evolved cognitive mechanisms (Pica et al., 2011). There may be a “shared taste” in body movement perception across cultures, which is attributable to adaptations and which is independent of socio-cultural effects. If so, one might expect to find cross-cultural agreement in body movement perceptions, but empirical evidence on this hypothesis is scarce. Montepare and Zebrowitz (1993) investigated similarities and differences in American and Korean participants’ perceptions of age-related gait qualities in men and women. The researchers videotaped American men and women with reflective tape attached to major limb joints, highlighting movements against a black background. American and Korean observers judged the videos on several traits, including happiness, sexiness, and dominance. There was substantial cross-cultural agreement, even when controlling for perceived age and sex of the walkers. However, although American ratings of perceived dominance decreased with the walkers’ age, this relationship was not observed with Korean ratings. The researchers concluded that some judgments derived from gait might be universal, whereas others may be determined by culturally specific values that alter age-related gait cues.
More recently, Fink et al. (2014) recruited a sample of Brazilian and German women to view animated virtual characters of male dancers (for methodology, see Neave et al., 2011; Fink et al., 2012b). Women rated the attractiveness of 10 s videos of each dancer on a 5-point Likert-type scale. There was a significant positive correlation of Brazilian with German women’s assessments of men’s dance attractiveness, suggesting cross-cultural similarity in dance attractiveness perceptions. However, additional analyses considering the dancers’ personality (as assessed by the NEO-FFI; Costa and McCrae, 1992) as covariates revealed a significant difference between Brazilian and German women’s ratings of men’s dance movements, as well as significant interaction effects of country with two of the personality factors (neuroticism and conscientiousness).
These preliminary findings suggest that although there is some cross-cultural consensus in women’s perceptions of men’s dance movements, part of the variation in dance movement perception is attributable to culture-dependent personality cues (Schmitt et al., 2007) derived from dance movements. It is unclear, however, whether the attribution of certain personality characteristics to a dancer also corresponds with his/her “true” (i.e., self-reported) personality, or if personality attribution merely reflects a “halo” of attractiveness perception. The answer to this question is probably not solely in support of one of the two possibilities but rather a combination of the two. An interesting result in this context—though on face perception—is that of Little et al. (2006), who suggested that the attractiveness stereotype (sensu Dion et al., 1972) should be reconsidered in that attractiveness is not an unspecific attribution, but reflects the attribution of desired personality traits. In short, people provided higher attractiveness ratings when they thought that—based on facial appearance—the target has desired personality characteristics. We consider it likely that personality perception from dance movements may also be affected by personality characteristics desired in a romantic partner. Hence the inference to personality of a dancer via perceptions of dance attractiveness may be based on displays expressed in dance that do not match with (and are not caused by) self-reported personality.
Weege et al. (in press a) showed that men’s self-reported (NEO-FFI) personality scores did not correlate with women’s ratings of men’s personality standings, i.e., women were not able to accurately assess men’s personality from dance movements—a result that corroborates research investigating relationships of self-reported personality with observer-reports of personality based on gait (Thoresen et al., 2012). However, women’s ratings of a man’s dance attractiveness correlated negatively with their ratings of his neuroticism, and positively with their ratings of his conscientiousness. Weege et al. (in press a) suggested that there might be fundamental movement characteristics, which affect perceptions of human dance such that people who associate elements of a dance with desirable personality characteristics and—as a consequence—rate that dance as more attractive. In a classic study by Heider and Simmel (1944) participants viewed a brief movie showing the interaction of a rectangle, triangles and a circle and attributed intentional movement and goal-directed interactions to these shapes although no social cues were present. Although many interpretations have been offered over the years, it seems that basic features of these objects, together with their amplitude, speed, and velocity of movement are sufficient to cause personality attributions (see also Brownlow et al., 1997; Koppensteiner and Grammer, 2010, 2011). We suggest that future studies on relationships of objective (biomechanical) characteristics and perception of human body movement will arrive at similar conclusions.
In summary, humans attend to diverse stimuli to secure information about others, particularly when assessing attractiveness. There is robust evidence from the study of static representations of human faces and bodies that certain features provide information about an individual’s quality and this has consequences for human social perception, including people’s mating psychology and behavior. Recent research shows that quality information can also be obtained from body movement, such as dance and gait. Whether this information affords accurate assessment of personality characteristics is unclear. Preliminary evidence suggests that personality cannot be assessed accurately from dance movements, although people agree on trait attributions to dance movements. Thus, it may be that movement quality—as expressed by combination of amplitude, speed, and velocity—creates an impression about personality. Dance movements, however, seem to provide information about physical strength, especially in men, and women prefer the dances of men who are physically stronger. Comparative studies on motor performance in non-human animals indicate that elaborate and acrobatic courtship dances correlate with male reproductive success. For example, Barske et al. (2011) showed that female golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus) preferred males that performed displays at greater speed. Reproductively successful males also had higher heart rates, which was interpreted as greater metabolic investment in courtship displays. It is premature to speculate on analogous phenomena in humans, although it is plausible that women may judge energetically demanding dance movements more positively, because such displays are produced by high quality men. If women consider dance movements in their assessments of male quality, this raises questions about the relative strengths of the effects of static and dynamic cues on attractiveness assessments. Studies on facial and bodily attractiveness show that impression formation is made rapidly, and although rapid trait attribution has been reported for movement too, movement cues may contribute to a more detailed assessment of a person than impression formation based on static cues. We therefore recommend that attractiveness research includes assessments of body movement.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgment
Preparation of this article was supported by funds of the German Research Foundation (DFG), grant numbers FI1450/4-1 and FI 1450/7-1 awarded to BF. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Funds of the Göttingen University.
Footnotes
- ^ We omit reference to data on dance and symmetry, as reported by Brown et al. (2005). Careful investigation revealed that some of the study data were fabricated and a re-analysis could not confirm the reported results (Trivers et al., 2009). The Brown et al. (2005) paper was retracted on November 27, 2013. For a more detailed discussion of the case of dance and symmetry see Fink et al. (2014).
References
Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., and Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception 33, 717–746. doi: 10.1068/p5096
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Barske, J., Schlinger, B., Wikelski, M., and Fusani, L. (2011). Female choice for male motor skills. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3523–3528. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0382
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Brown, W. M., Cronk, L., Grochow, K., Jacobson, A., Liu, C. K., Popovic, Z., et al. (2005). Dance reveals symmetry especially in young men. Nature 438, 1148–1150. doi: 10.1038/nature04344
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Brownlow, S., Dixon, A. R., Egbert, C. A., and Radcliffe, R. D. (1997). Perception of movement and dancer characteristics from point-light displays. Psychol. Rec. 47, 411–421.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1–49. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00023992
Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., and Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception 34, 1171–1180. doi: 10.1068/p5203
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033731
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., and Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception 25, 727–738. doi: 10.1068/p250727
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 17, 124–129. doi: 10.1037/h0030377
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., Chan, A., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider, K., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgments of facial expressions of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 712–717. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.712
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fink, B., Hugill, N., and Lange, B. P. (2012a). Women’s body movements are a potential cue to ovulation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 53, 759–763. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.06.005
Fink, B., Weege, B., Flügge, J., Röder, S., Neave, N., and McCarty, K. (2012b). Men’s personality and women’s perception of their dance quality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 52, 232–235. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.008
Fink, B., Neave, N., and Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19, 82–87. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20583
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fink, B., and Penton-Voak, I. S. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 154–158. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00190
Fink, B., Weege, B., Neave, N., Ried, B., and do Lago, O. C. (2014). “Female perceptions of male body movement,” in Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Sexual Psychology and Behavior, eds V. Weekes-Shackelford and T. K. Shackelford (Berlin: Springer), 299–324.
Gangestad, S. W., and Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 34, 523–548. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.070203.143733
Grammer, K., Fink, B., Møller, A. P., and Thornhill, R. (2003a). Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 78, 385–407. doi: 10.1017/S1464793102006085
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Grammer, K., Keki, V., Striebel, B., Atzmueller, M., and Fink, B. (2003b). “Bodies in motion: a window to the soul?” in Evolutionary Aesthetics, eds E. Voland and K. Grammer (Berlin: Springer), 295–323.
Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbour, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 711–720. doi: 10.1162/089892900562417
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Grossman, E. D., Battelli, L., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over STSp disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Res. 45, 2847–2853. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.027
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hammermesh, D. S. (2011). Beauty Pays: Why Attractive People are More Successful. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Hanna, J. L. (1987). Dance, Sex and Gender: Signs of Identity, Dominance, Defiance, and Desire. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hanna, J. L. (2010). Dance and sexuality: many moves. J. Sex Res. 47, 212–241. doi: 10.1080/00224491003599744
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., and Gildersleeve, K. (2011). Can men detect ovulation? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 87–92. doi: 10.1177/0963721411402668
Heider, F., and Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 243–259. doi: 10.2307/1416950
Henderson, V. R., Hennessey, M., Barrett, D. W., Curtis, B., McCoy-Roth, M., Trentacoste, N., et al. (2005). When risky is attractive: sensation seeking and romantic partner selection. Pers. Individ. Dif. 38, 311–325. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.04.010
Hugill, N., Fink, B., Neave, N., Besson, A., and Bunse, L. (2011). Women’s perception of men’s sensation seeking propensity from their dance movements. Pers. Individ. Dif. 51, 483–487. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.002
Hugill, N., Fink, B., Neave, N., and Seydel, H. (2009). Men’s physical strength is associated with women’s perceptions of their dancing ability. Pers. Individ. Dif. 47, 527–530. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.009
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., and Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 7241–7244. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200155109
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Johnson, K. L., Gill, S., Reichman, V., and Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Swagger, sway, and sexuality: judging sexual orientation from body motion and morphology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 321–334. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.321
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaeppler, A. L. (1978). Dance in anthropological perspective. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 7, 31–49. doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.07.100178.000335
Koppensteiner, M., and Grammer, K. (2010). Motion patterns in political speech and their influence on personality ratings. J. Res. Pers. 44, 374–379. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.04.002
Koppensteiner, M., and Grammer, K. (2011). Body movements of male and female speakers and their influence on perceptions of personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 51, 743–747. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.014
Koscinski, K. (2013). Perception of facial attractiveness from static and dynamic stimuli. Perception 42, 163–175. doi: 10.1068/p7378
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., and Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2006). What is good is beautiful: face preference reflects desired personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 11, 1107–1118. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.015
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 66, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C. J., et al. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: beauty captures the mind of the beholder. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 1107–1120. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1107
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
McCarty, K., Hönekopp, J., Neave, N., and Fink, B. (2013). Male body movements as a possible cue to physical strength. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 25, 307–312. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22360
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Montepare, J. M., and Zebrowitz, L. A. (1993). A cross-cultural comparison of impressions created by age-related variations in gait. J. Nonverbal Behav. 17, 55–68. doi: 10.1007/BF00987008
Moore, N. J., Hickson, M. III, and Sacks, D. W. (2010). Nonverbal Communication: Studies and Applications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Neave, N., McCarty, K., Freynik, J., Caplan, N., Hönekopp, J., and Fink, B. (2011). Male dance moves that catch a woman’s eye. Biol. Lett. 7, 221–224. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0619
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pica, P., Jackson, S., Blake, R., and Troje, N. F. (2011). Comparing biological motion perception of two distinct human societies. PLoS ONE 6:e28391. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028391
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Provost, M. P., Quinsey, V. L., and Troje, N. F. (2008). Differences in Gait across the menstrual cycle and their attractiveness to men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 37, 598–604. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9219-7
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rhode, D. L. (2010). The Beauty Bias: The Injustice of Appearance in Life and Law. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rhodes, G., Lie, H. C., Thevaraja, N., Taylor, L., Iredell, N., Curran, C., et al. (2011). Facial attractiveness ratings from video-clips and static images tell the same story. PLoS ONE 6:e26653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026653
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J. A., and Fernandez-Dols, J. M. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 329–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145102
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Saad, G. (2011). The Consuming Instinct: What Juicy Burgers, Ferraris, Pornography, and Gift Giving Reveal About Human Nature. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
Sauter, D. A., and Eisner, F. (2013). Commonalities outweigh differences in the communication of emotions across human cultures. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, E180. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1209522110
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schmitt, D. P., Allik, J., McCrae, R. R., and Benet-Martinez, V. (2007). The geographic distribution of big five personality traits: patterns and profiles of human self-description across 56 nations. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 38, 173–212. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297299
Simion, F., Regolin, L., and Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 809–813. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707021105
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Singh, D. (2002). Female mate value at a glance: relationship of waist-to-hip ratio to health, fecundity and attractiveness. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 23, 81–91.
Singh, D. (2006). The universal allure of the hourglass figure: an evolutionary theory of female sexual attractiveness. Clin. Plast. Surg. 33, 359–370. doi: 10.1016/j.cps.2006.05.007
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thoresen, J. C., Vuong, Q. C., and Atkinson, A. P. (2012). First impressions: gait cues drive reliable trait judgements. Cognition 124, 261–271. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.018
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Trivers, R., Palestis, B., and Zataari, D. (2009). The Anatomy of a Fraud: Symmetry and Dance. Antioch, CA: TPZ Publishers.
Troje, N. F., Westhoff, C., and Lavrov, M. (2005). Person identification from biological motion: effects of structural and kinematic cues. Percept. Psychophys. 67, 667–675. doi: 10.3758/BF03193523
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Weege, B., Barges, L., Pham, M. N., Shackelford, T. K., and Fink, B. (in press a). Women’s attractiveness perception of men’s dance movements in relation to self-reported and perceived personality. Evol. Psychol. Sci.
Weege, B., Pham, M. N., Shackelford, T. K., and Fink, B. (in press b). Physical strength and dance attractiveness: further evidence for an association in men, but not in women. Am. J. Hum. Biol.
Weege, B., Lange, B. P., and Fink, B. (2012). Women’s visual attention to variation in men’s dance quality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 53, 236–240. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.011
Willis, J., and Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci. 17, 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., and Montepare, J. (2008). Social psychological face perception: why appearance matters. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2, 1497–1517. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: attractiveness, body movement, dance, perception, personality, strength
Citation: Fink B, Weege B, Neave N, Pham MN and Shackelford TK (2015) Integrating body movement into attractiveness research. Front. Psychol. 6:220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00220
Received: 14 November 2014; Accepted: 12 February 2015;
Published online: 03 March 2015.
Edited by:
Paul J. Watson, University of New Mexico, USAReviewed by:
Darine Zaatari, NORC at the University of Chicago, USAPaul J. Watson, University of New Mexico, USA
Melissa L. Franklin, University of New Mexico/Central New Mexico Community College, USA
Copyright © 2015 Fink, Weege, Neave, Pham and Shackelford. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Bernhard Fink, Institute of Psychology and Courant Research Center Evolution of Social Behavior, University of Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, D-37077 Göttingen, Germany e-mail: bernhard.fink@ieee.org