Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 January 2015
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Behavioral and physiological bases of attentional biases: Paradigms, participants, and stimuli View all 10 articles

Depression, not PTSD, is associated with attentional biases for emotional visual cues in early traumatized individuals with PTSD

  • 1Clinical Neuropsychology, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
  • 3Schön Klinik Hamburg-Eilbek, Hamburg, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany

Using variants of the emotional Stroop task (EST), a large number of studies demonstrated attentional biases in individuals with PTSD across different types of trauma. However, the specificity and robustness of the emotional Stroop effect in PTSD have been questioned recently. In particular, the paradigm cannot disentangle underlying cognitive mechanisms. Transgenerational studies provide evidence that consequences of trauma are not limited to the traumatized people, but extend to close relatives, especially the children. To further investigate attentional biases in PTSD and to shed light on the underlying cognitive mechanism(s), a spatial-cueing paradigm with pictures of different emotional valence (neutral, anxiety, depression, trauma) was administered to individuals displaced as children during World War II (WWII) with (n = 22) and without PTSD (n = 26) as well as to non-traumatized controls (n = 22). To assess whether parental PTSD is associated with biased information processing in children, each one adult offspring was also included in the study. PTSD was not associated with attentional biases for trauma-related stimuli. There was no evidence for a transgenerational transmission of biased information processing. However, when samples were regrouped based on current depression, a reduced inhibition of return (IOR) effect emerged for depression-related cues. IOR refers to the phenomenon that with longer intervals between cue and target the validity effect is reversed: uncued locations are associated with shorter and cued locations with longer RTs. The results diverge from EST studies and demonstrate that findings on attentional biases yield equivocal results across different paradigms. Attentional biases for trauma-related material may only appear for verbal but not for visual stimuli in an elderly population with childhood trauma with PTSD. Future studies should more closely investigate whether findings from younger trauma populations also manifest in older trauma survivors.

Introduction

In 2014, the United Nations Refugee Agency (UNHCR; United Nations Refugee Agency, 2014) reported that at the end of 2013, more than 50 million people were forcibly displaced due to war, conflicts, or human right violations. The negative consequences of forced displacement for the psychological and physical well-being are documented in many studies (e.g., Fazel et al., 2005; Porter and Haslam, 2005). Beyond that, forced displacement is associated with a highly increased risk for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD, e.g., Fazel et al., 2005; Steel et al., 2009; Freitag et al., 2013). Even decades later, a substantial proportion of those affected suffer from negative consequences (e.g., Muhtz et al., 2011; Freitag et al., 2013). In addition, the negative consequences of traumatic events are not restricted to those directly exposed, but seem to impact close relatives such as the children (Danieli, 1998; Leen-Feldner et al., 2013). For example, an increased risk for the development of PTSD was reported in offspring of individuals with PTSD (Baider et al., 2000, 2006; Yehuda et al., 2008). Nonetheless, the literature is not fully consistent and adequately designed population-based studies did not find clear-cut evidence for a transgenerational transmission (Van IJzendoorn et al., 2003; Levav et al., 2007; Fridman et al., 2011).

In order to study the long-term consequences of forced displacement, individuals displaced at the end of World War II (WWII) were increasingly investigated in recent years. Studies revealed a high rate of PTSD more than 60 years later (e.g., Teegen and Meister, 2000; Muhtz et al., 2011). According to DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000), PTSD is characterized by symptoms of re-experiencing, avoidance of trauma-related stimuli, and hyperarousal. Beyond that, PTSD is associated with different biases in information processing, for example, attentional biases for trauma-related stimuli (Buckley et al., 2000; Constans, 2005).

In experimental psychopathology, different paradigms have been adopted from cognitive psychology to investigate attentional biases in PTSD, most commonly the emotional Stroop task (EST, Williams et al., 1996) and the dot-probe paradigm (MacLeod et al., 1986). EST studies largely contributed to our knowledge of attentional biases in PTSD and the emotional Stroop effect (ESE) was considered a robust finding for many years as it was replicated across different trauma populations (for reviews see Buckley et al., 2000; Constans, 2005). However, the specificity and stability of the effect were critically discussed (Kimble et al., 2009; Cisler et al., 2011) and closer scrutiny reveals that interpretability of many studies is constrained by methodological limitations. For example, while some studies lack a trauma control group (Paunovic et al., 2002; El Khoury-Malhame et al., 2011a; Fleurkens et al., 2011), the interpretability of other studies is restricted because no negatively-valenced control stimuli were included (Harvey et al., 1996; Paunovic et al., 2002; Bremner et al., 2004). Thus, the question whether the effect is specific to PTSD and/or to trauma-related stimuli remains unresolved. Furthermore, the EST has several methodological problems (e.g., Algom et al., 2004) and does not allow to disentangle the different attentional bias components, that is, whether these are comprised of attentional facilitation (i.e., preferred processing of trauma-related compared to neutral stimuli), attentional interference (i.e., difficulties disengaging from trauma-related to other stimuli) or attentional avoidance (i.e., attention allocation toward the opposite location of trauma-related stimuli, cf. Cisler and Koster, 2010). The differentiation of these components can provide a better theoretical understanding and allows the development or improvement, respectively, of novel interventional techniques (Shipherd and Salters-Pedneault, 2008).

In dot-probe tasks, two stimuli of different valence are presented simultaneously for a set time (e.g., 500 ms). Subsequently, one of the two stimuli is replaced by a probe and participants are asked to respond to either its location or to classify the probe (e.g., * or **, Cisler et al., 2009). A faster reaction to probes that replace negatively-valenced (congruent trials) compared to neutral stimuli (incongruent trials) are interpreted as evidence for attentional facilitation as attention is already drawn to the spatial location of the threatening stimulus (Yiend, 2010). The opposite response pattern, that is, slower reactions to probes that replace negatively-valenced compared to neutral stimuli are indicative of attentional avoidance (Cisler and Koster, 2010; Yiend, 2010). The majority of studies administering variants of the dot-probe paradigm in PTSD do not provide evidence for attentional biases for trauma-related/negative material (Dalgleish et al., 2003; Elsesser et al., 2004, 2005; Fani et al., 2011); however, some studies demonstrated attentional biases (Bryant and Harvey, 1997; El Khoury-Malhame et al., 2011a,b). More recent studies suggest that acute stress (e.g., missile attacks) is associated with attentional avoidance of threat-related information (Bar-Haim et al., 2010; Wald et al., 2011a,b), which, in turn, predicted psychopathological symptoms in the short- (Bar-Haim et al., 2010; Wald et al., 2011a) and long-term (Wald et al., 2011b).

A paradigm that allows the differentiated assessment of attentional bias components represents the visual search task (VST, Öhmann et al., 2001). In VSTs, participants are asked to detect a discrepant target stimulus embedded in an array of identical stimuli. The VST was applied in two studies to differentially assess facilitation and interference in PTSD (Pineles et al., 2007, 2009). In the interference condition, a target (e.g., non-word) was presented in an array of experimental stimuli (e.g., trauma-related words) whereas in the facilitation condition, the arrangement was reversed (i.e., target experimental word embedded in an array of identical non-words). Facilitation to trauma-related words was inferred from faster reaction times to trauma-related compared to neutral targets in an array of non-word distracters. Interference to trauma-related words was inferred from slower reaction times to target stimuli embedded in arrays with trauma-related compared to neutral distracters (Pineles et al., 2009). In both studies, PTSD was associated with attentional interference to trauma-related stimuli (Pineles et al., 2007, 2009) and this effect was specific to trauma-related stimuli (Pineles et al., 2009). However, there was no evidence for attentional facilitation. This finding conflicts with the theoretical assumption of hypervigilance in PTSD (Pineles et al., 2009). However, the latter two paradigms are also plagued by interpretational problems (see Hauschildt et al., 2013, for a further discussion).

Spatial-Cueing Task

One paradigm that enables the assessment of the precise underlying mechanism represents a modification of the spatial-cueing paradigm (Posner, 1980). A great advantage of cueing paradigms is the fact that the behavioral reaction is made in response to a neutral target, thus, response bias explanations can be ruled out (Yiend, 2010). Furthermore, by varying the stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA), cueing paradigms allow the assessment of the temporal attention allocation. This is important when investigating attentional biases in PTSD as this disorder seems to be associated with delayed disengagement from trauma-related cues (e.g., Pineles et al., 2007) and disengagement is based upon controlled processes that need more time to take effect (cf. Yiend, 2010).

Attention is focused on a fixation point located between two rectangles. Subsequently, a cue is presented in one of the two rectangles, followed by a target that either appears in the same rectangle (valid trial) or in the opposite rectangle (invalid trial). In some trials, no cue appears (catch trials). The participants' task is to indicate (e.g., key press) in which rectangle the target was presented. For the assessment of attentional biases in psychopathology, the cue is varied as to its emotional valence (e.g., threatening, neutral). While facilitation is operationalized as faster responses to validly cued trials when the cue is threatening/disorder-specific compared to neutral, avoidance is characterized by slower RTs to threatening compared to neutral cues in valid trials. Slower RTs to threatening/disorder-specific compared to neutral stimuli in invalidly cued trials are interpreted as interference, faster RTs as avoidance (Koster et al., 2006). However, this pattern is only true for short SOAs (<300 ms, Posner and Cohen, 1984). With longer time intervals between cue and target (SOA), inhibition of return (IOR) occurs, that is, cued locations lose their attentional preference as attention is directed to uncued locations after a certain time (for a review see Klein, 2000). It is assumed that this effect is adaptive as redirecting attention to an already attended location does not provide additional information. As PTSD seems to be associated with problems in disengaging and patients “stick” to trauma-related material, the IOR effect should be reduced or even absent as attention is not re-directed to new locations. Although some studies provide evidence for this assumption in anxiety (Nelson et al., 1993; Fox et al., 2002), other results speak for the stability of the IOR effect (Stoyanova et al., 2007; Lange et al., 2008). To the best of our knowledge, one study applied the spatial-cueing paradigm in PTSD (Hauschildt et al., 2013). A spatial-cueing paradigm with pictures of different emotional valence (trauma-related, negative control, general threat, neutral) and varying SOA (450, 1200 ms) was administered to 25 participants with PTSD, 22 non-PTSD and 24 healthy control participants. Although neither PTSD nor trauma exposition were associated with attentional biases, depressive symptomatology was linked with attentional avoidance of trauma-related and negative control stimuli.

Transgenerational Information Processing Studies

First evidence that information processing biases can be transferred came from Motta and colleagues (Motta et al., 1994, 1997) who administered an EST to children of Vietnam veterans and non-veterans. In the first study (Motta et al., 1994) the mean difference between children of veterans and non-veterans for the PTSD-related card was 1.97 s, whereas the mean reaction time differences for all other cards varied between 0.21 and 0.81 s. In a replication study with a larger sample, children of veterans were significantly slower to color-name the war-related card compared to children of non-veterans, whereas RTs to all other cards (OCD-related, positive, neutral) did not differ between groups (Motta et al., 1997). Evidence for a transmission was also found when the children's group allocation was based on parental trauma exposure (Suozzi and Motta, 2004). These findings were replicated in a sample of children and adolescents (Moradi et al., 1999): children whose parents suffered from PTSD exhibited an ESE for threat-related compared to neutral words and compared to the children of healthy control participants. However, conflicting evidence stems from one study in which children of displaced individuals (with and without PTSD) were compared to children of non-traumatized healthy control participants regarding their color naming latencies in an EST (Wittekind et al., 2010). There was no evidence for attentional biases for trauma-related words in children of displaced individuals with PTSD. However, the sample differed from previous studies in several important aspects (e.g., time since parental traumatization, children's age, trauma type, parental trauma vs. PTSD) limiting comparability between studies.

To conclude, a substantial body of studies assert that PTSD is related to attentional biases for trauma-related material which seem to results from difficulties to disengage (Pineles et al., 2007, 2009). However, interpretability of many studies is constrained by methodological limitations and results need to be replicated across different paradigms and stimulus modalities (i.e., verbal vs. visual stimuli). Furthermore, prior research almost exclusively recruited younger trauma samples (average age in emotional Stroop studies: 36 years, Cisler et al., 2011), thus, it remains unclear whether attentional biases persist over the course of the disorder. Beyond that, essential influencing factors (e.g., SOA, stimulus type, comorbid depression) have been neglected in prior research on attentional biases in PTSD (also see Cisler et al., 2009). Transgenerational studies applied the EST to assess whether parental trauma or PTSD, respectively, is related to attentional biases in the second generation. Beside the fact that evidence is ambiguous, it is yet unclear whether findings translate to different paradigms and which attentional bias component drives the effect.

The Present Study

The aim of the present study was to replicate and extend the results by Hauschildt et al. (2013) in a sample of older individuals with chronic PTSD due to childhood trauma as well as their offspring. To meet this aim, we also administered a modified version of the spatial-cueing paradigm using visual instead of verbal stimuli that differed as to their emotional valence (trauma-related, depression-related, anxiety-related, neutral). As PTSD seems to be associated with delayed disengagement from trauma-related stimuli (Pineles et al., 2007, 2009), one would expect a diminished IOR effect for targets following trauma-related compared to other emotional or neutral pictures in individuals with PTSD. However, as the majority of previous studies do not provide evidence for a reduced IOR effect in PTSD (Hauschildt et al., 2013) and other anxiety disorders (Stoyanova et al., 2007; Lange et al., 2008), we assume that the IOR effect is not affected by cue valence. Regarding a transgenerational transmission, we hypothesized that offspring of PTSD participants demonstrate an attentional bias for trauma-related material; however, we did not have a directed hypothesis whether attentional biases result from facilitation, interference, or avoidance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Individuals displaced as children during or after WWII (n = 50) and one of their adult children were recruited by (a) a database built up in a previous study (for a detailed description of recruitment strategies see Muhtz et al., 2011), (b) contact to displacement networks and self-help groups, (c) word of mouth, and (d) personal contacts. Participants were born between 1932 and 1941 and experienced at least one traumatic event according to DSM-IV trauma criteria during their flight. Group allocation was based on the PTSD module of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I, First et al., 1997). To assure that diagnoses of PTSD were indeed due to forced displacement and not to a later trauma, we inquired whether participants experienced a traumatic event other than flight/displacement. If this was the case, participants had to indicate which of the traumatic events was worse. Subsequently, PTSD criteria for each traumatic event were assessed via the SCID and items were rephrased such that the relation to the respective event was stressed, e.g., instead of “traumatic event” we explicitly used “after displacement.” Exclusion criteria for all groups were a lifetime history of psychotic, manic or bipolar symptoms, substance dependence within the last year or suicidal tendencies as assessed with the MINI Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI, Sheehan et al., 1998). Of all traumatized participants who were assessed, two participants had to be excluded (manic disorder, trauma criteria A2 not fulfilled). Three adult children were excluded due to alcohol dependence, withdrawal of informed consent, and psychotic symptoms. Thus, the PTSD group comprised 22 traumatized participants of whom 12 fulfilled all PTSD criteria and 10 participants were diagnosed with subsyndromal PTSD as suggested by Blanchard et al. (1996, DSM-IV criteria A, B, E, F were fulfilled and either criterion C or D) and 21 of their adult children. The remaining 26 participants (and 24 of their children) were allocated to the non-PTSD group. Twenty-two non-traumatized (DSM-IV trauma criteria A1 and A2) participants who were not displaced during WWII, not married to an individual displaced during WWII, not meeting any current axis I disorder (based on the MINI) and one adult offspring formed the healthy control group. The latter group was recruited by means of advertisement in local media, notices in public places, and word of mouth. Written informed consent was obtained prior to the study from all participants. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Measures

Psychopathology

All participants were interviewed with the MINI interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) in order to determine (a) exclusion criteria for all participants, (b) (comorbid) psychiatric disorders in traumatized participants, and (c) absence of any current axis I disorder in non-traumatized controls. In order to quantify PTSD severity, the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS, Foa et al., 1997) was administered to all traumatized participants. The PDS is a self-report questionnaire showing high reliability and validity (Foa et al., 1997). All 17 items of the PDS were paraphrased such that “traumatic event” was replaced by “flight/displacement.” Depression severity was quantified with the 17-item version of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS, Hamilton, 1960). Finally, verbal intelligence was estimated using a vocabulary test [Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B (MWT-B), Lehrl, 2005].

Stimulus selection

Pictorial stimuli of the present study captured five different conditions (Trauma, Depression, Anxiety, Neutral, Neutral old). Pictures were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS, Lang et al., 2008), the internet or from books and media reports about displacement after WWII (trauma-related stimuli). Besides the emotional conditions and the neutral condition (IAPS pictures), we included a fifth condition (neutral old) containing pictures that came from the same time as the trauma- (i.e., displacement) related pictures. This was done to control for “age effects” as it is conceivable that trauma-related pictures are processed differentially due to their deviation from pictures taken from the internet or the IAPS. All stimuli were rated by 15 displaced individuals in a pilot study that was conducted via an online survey regarding (a) their relevance for (aa) flight/displacement after WWII, (ab) depression and (ac) anxiety (1 = very relevant, 2 = slightly relevant, 3 = not relevant), (b) neutrality (yes/no), and (c) personal relevance (yes/no). For the final picture set, trauma-related pictures had to be rated as highly displacement-relevant (rating = very relevant) by at least 80% and as personally relevant by at least 60% of displaced individuals. Furthermore, trauma-related pictures were rated as significantly more displacement-relevant than pictures from all other categories, all ps < 0.001. The final set of pictures comprised 10 trauma-related (e.g., refugee trek), 10 depression-related (e.g., sad person), 10 anxiety-related (e.g., snake), 10 neutral (IAPS, e.g., towel), and 10 neutral-old pictures (e.g., landscape). Pictures were presented in black-and-white.

Procedure and experimental task

Before the experimental paradigm started, demographic and psychopathological information (MINI, HDRS) were thoroughly inquired. Traumatized participants were also assessed with the PTSD module of the SCID.

The experimental paradigm was constructed using Superlab® software and was presented individually via a Macintosh computer in a dimly lit room to prevent reflections on the monitor. Participants were instructed in written and verbal form to classify via key press whether a target (black dot) was presented in the right or left rectangle (“m” and “y” [German keyboard], respectively, on the keyboard). They were told that each target would be preceded by a picture whose position was irrelevant for the task. To ensure that all participants understood the task, a practice trial with 10 items was administered to participants prior to the experimental task.

The procedure for each trial was as followed: to focus attention to a central point, a small fixation cross was presented between two rectangles (7 cm high by 9.4 cm wide) for 500 ms. The rectangles remained on the screen throughout a block of trials. Subsequently, a cue stimulus picture appeared with equal probability inside one of the two rectangles (400 ms). The cue varied as to its emotional valence (Trauma, Depression, Anxiety, Neutral-old, Neutral) and was followed by the fixation cross/rectangles for either 50 or 800 ms. Thus, SOA between cue and target varied between short (450 ms) and long (1200 ms) intervals (Moritz et al., 2009; Hauschildt et al., 2013). Then, the target was presented equally often in the center of one of the two rectangles and independent of the cue (i.e., the position of the cue had no predictive value for the position of the target). In valid trials, cue and target appeared in the same rectangle, whereas in invalid trials, cue and target appeared in opposite rectangles. The target remained on the screen until a response (i.e., key press) was made. In approximately 9% of trials, no target was presented (catch trials) and rectangles remained on the screen for 1500 ms before the next trial was automatically initiated. The inter-trial interval was 1000 ms. In total, the task comprised 450 trials with 10 practice, 40 catch and 400 experimental trials (5 conditions × 10 stimuli × 2 long/short × 2 valid/invalid × 2 right/left) presented in fully randomized order. The task was divided in two blocks (220 trials/block) with a short break in-between. Subsequently, participants rated all pictures as to their valence and personal relevance, respectively (1 = positive and personal relevant, 2 = positive, 3 = neutral, 4 = negative, 5 = negative and personal relevant). For the rating task, pictures were also presented in random order.

Strategy of Data Analyses

Dependent variables were reaction time (in ms) and accuracy (i.e., percentage of errors). According to a prior study (Hauschildt et al., 2013), only RTs between 150 and 2000 ms were considered for analyses. Furthermore, RTs of incorrect trials (i.e., wrong key) were omitted. For each participant, RTs for each combination of Cue Type, Validity, SOA, and Position were determined. However, as position was not considered crucial for subsequent analyses, RTs were collapsed across position yielding six RTs per participant. Catch trials were not analyzed.

To test the main hypothesis, mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted for each generation separately as we were especially interested in intra-generational effects. To facilitate interpretation, only relevant interactions including group are reported. Furthermore, an IOR effect was calculated by substracting mean RTs of valid trials from mean RTs of invalid trials (Moritz and Von Mühlenen, 2005). Positive values are indicative of a facilitation effect of the cue on the target, negative values for an inhibitory (i.e., IOR) effect. The alpha level for all statistical tests was 0.05 (two-tailed). Effect sizes were calculated with η2p ≈ 0.01 indicating a small, η2p ≈ 0.06 a medium, and η2p ≈ 0.14 a large effect (Kinnear and Gray, 2008). To break down significant interactions, One-Way ANOVAs were calculated. Greenhouse-Geisser correction for degrees of freedom was applied if assumption of sphericity was violated. Correlational analyses (Pearson) were conducted between IOR effects (i.e., RTinvalid - RTvalid) for both SOA (450, 1200 ms) and depressive (parents: n = 70, offspring: n = 66) as well as posttraumatic symptomatology (n = 48).

Results

Sociodemographic Information and Psychopathology

As can be derived from Table 1, neither parental nor offspring groups differed regarding age, gender, or verbal intelligence (all ps > 0.09). As expected, traumatized participants with PTSD suffered from higher PTSD (PDS, p < 0.001) and depressive symptomatology (HDRS, p < 0.001). Offspring groups differed significantly on depressive symptom severity (p = 0.003); however, mean ratings were within the normal range in all offspring groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Background variables (sociodemography and psychopathology) for parental and offspring groups: means (standard deviation) or frequency.

Subjective Valence Ratings

To verify stimuli allocation, participants' mean ratings were submitted to two Two-Way ANOVAs with Cue Type (Trauma, Anxiety, Depression, Neutral-old, Neutral) as within- and Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, non-Trauma) as between-subjects factor. Mean valence ratings (1 = positive and personally relevant, 2 = positive, 3 = neutral, 4 = negative, 5 = negative and personally relevant) served as dependent variables.

Parents

As expected, the main effect Cue Type was significant, F(3.08, 206.63) = 201.51, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.75. All emotional pictures were rated as more negative than neutral pictures (ps < 0.001). Furthermore, trauma-related pictures were perceived significantly more negative than anxiety- and depression-related pictures (ps < 0.001), however, the latter two conditions were not rated differently (p > 0.99). The main effect of Group also achieved significance, F(2, 67) = 12.53, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.27: the most negative ratings were obtained for the PTSD group which differed significantly from the non-Trauma (p < 0.001) and at trend level from the non-PTSD group (p = 0.085). The main effects were modified by a significant Cue Type × Group interaction, F(6.17, 206.63) = 4.64, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.12 (for post-hoc One-Way ANOVAs [α = 0.05], see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Subjective valence ratings: means (standard deviation) and results of post-hoc ANOVAs.

Offspring

Cue Type exerted a significant influence on valence ratings, F(3.30, 210.88) = 179.42, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.74. As in the parental generation, emotional pictures were rated as more negative than all other pictures (ps < 0.001) and trauma-related pictures as more negative than anxiety- and depression-related pictures (ps < 0.001, see Table 2). Furthermore, depression-related pictures were considered more negative than anxiety-related pictures (p = 0.001). Groups also differed as to their overall ratings, F(2, 64) = 5.23, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.14. Children of the non-PTSD group rated pictures on average more negative than children of healthy controls (p = 0.007). However, the interaction Cue Type × Group did not reach significance, F(6.59, 210.88) = 1.29, p = 0.258, η2p = 0.04.

Accuracy

One participant of the offspring non-Trauma group pressed the wrong keys, thus, these data could not be considered in all subsequent analyses. Accuracy was high (PTSD: 97.92%, non-PTSD: 97.88%, non-Trauma: 98.80%, offspring PTSD: 97.85%, offspring non-PTSD: 97.17%, offspring non-Trauma: 98.06%) and did not differ between groups, F(5, 130) < 1, p = 0.977.

Attentional Biases

Parents

To test whether participants with PTSD exhibit attentional inference for trauma-related stimuli, a repeated measures Four-Way ANOVA with Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, non-Trauma) as between-subject factor and Cue Type (Trauma, Anxiety, Depression, Neutral-old, Neutral), SOA (450, 1200 ms), and Validity (Valid, Invalid) as within-subject factors was conducted. Mean RT served as dependent variable (see Table 3). A main effect of Cue Type emerged, F(4, 268) = 3.49, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.05: RTs for trauma- (M = 491.57 ms, SE = 7.43 ms) and anxiety-related pictures (M = 491.67 ms, SE = 7.41 ms) were significantly slower than RTs for neutral-old pictures (M = 484.65 ms, SE = 7.07 ms, ps < 0.05). The main effect of SOA was also significant, F(1, 67) = 124.32, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.65, due to faster RTs to long (M = 468.33 ms, SE = 6.86 ms) vs. short (M = 509.48 ms, SE = 7.92 ms) SOAs. Furthermore, RTs to invalid cues (M = 469.22 ms, SE = 7.05 ms) were faster than to valid cues (M = 508.59 ms, SE = 7.77 ms), F(1, 67) = 112.70, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.63, reflecting an IOR effect. Finally, groups differed as to their overall RT, F(2, 67) = 5.07, p = 0.009, η2p = 0.13, with the PTSD group being slower (M = 521.41 ms, SE = 12.77 ms) than both the non-PTSD and the non-Trauma group (M = 470.53 ms, SE = 11.74 ms and M = 474.77 ms, SE = 12.77 ms, respectively, ps < 0.05). However, neither the Three-Way interaction of Cue Type × Validity × Group, F(8, 268) < 1, p = 0.544, η2p = 0.025, nor the Four-Way interaction of Cue Type × SOA × Validity × Group were significant, F(7.91, 264.92) = 1.18, p = 0.314, η2p = 0.03. Thus, groups did not react differently to trauma-related stimuli.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean RTs (in ms), standard deviations and IOR effects for each combination of picture type, SOA, validity, and group.

Following Hauschildt et al. (2013), groups were dichotomized according to the presence of current depression (yes/no) as this disorder constitutes a common psychiatric comorbidity (Pietrzak et al., 2011) and is hardly considered as a confound in attentional bias research (Bar-Haim et al., 2007). We used a categorical MINI diagnosis to compose groups as we were interested in the impact of current depressive symptomatology. Sociodemographic and psychopatholgical characteristics are presented in Table 5. The mixed Four-Way ANOVA was repeated, this time with depressed (n = 14) vs. non-depressed (n = 56) as between-subject factor. As groups differed significantly regarding gender, the ANOVA was repeated with gender as an additional between-subject factor. Neither the main effect gender nor any interaction including gender was significant, ps > 0.1. Only relevant effects for group are reported. The depressed group (M = 521.29 ms, SE = 16.43 ms) was slowed compared to the non-depressed group (M = 479.50 ms, SE = 8.22 ms), F(1, 68) = 5.18, p = 0.026, η2p = 0.07. Whereas the Three-Way interaction of Cue Type × Validity × Group was not significant, F(4, 272) < 1, p = 0.715, η2p = 0.01, the Four-Way interaction of Cue Type × SOA × Validity × Group was, F(3.94, 268.02) = 4.02, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.06. To break down this interaction, Two-Way ANOVAs within SOAs (450 vs. 1200 ms) were calculated with Group (Depressed, non-Depressed) as between-subject and Cue Type (Trauma, Anxiety, Depression, Neutral-old, Neutral) as within-subject factors. IOR effects served as dependent variables. For short SOA, the relevant interaction of Group × Cue Type was significant, F(4, 272) = 2.52, p = 0.041, η2p = 0.04, whereas for the long SOA, significance was bordered, F(4, 272) = 2.38, p = 0.052, η2p = 0.03. As can be seen in Figure 1, the significant interaction (short SOA) reflected facilitated RTs (i.e., reduced IOR effect) for the depressed group in the depression- and trauma-related condition in comparison to the other conditions. For the non-depressed group, the reverse pattern emerged, that is, IOR effects for the neutral conditions were reduced compared to emotional conditions. For post-hoc conducted t-tests (short SOA), difference scores were calculated (i.e., IOR effect emotional condition - IOR effect neutral condition). For trauma-related cues, groups did not differ significantly, t(68) = 1.58, p = 0.118, d = 0.48. However, for depression-related cues, the IOR effect was significantly reduced in depressed individuals, t(68) = 2.62, p = 0.011, d = 0.8. Within group comparisons (depressed group) did not reveal a significant main effect of Cue Type, F(4, 52) < 1, p = 0.586, η2p = 0.05; however, numerically IOR effects were considerably smaller for depression-related compared to neutral cues (M = −46.70, SE = 10.91 vs. M = −66.88, SE = 14.36), d = 0.42.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. IOR effects for short SOA (in ms, standard error) for each picture type. Negative values indicate an inhibitory of the cue (picture) on the target.

Offspring

To examine whether children of individuals with PTSD show attentional biases for trauma-related stimuli, the mixed Four-Way ANOVA was repeated within the offspring generation (see Table 4). Cue Type did not influence RT, F(4, 252) = 1.07, p = 0.372, η2p = 0.02. However, RTs to long SOA (M = 413.12 ms, SE = 8.23 ms) were significantly faster than RTs to short SOA (M = 455.89 ms, SE = 8.67 ms), F(1, 63) = 192.45, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.75. Furthermore, the IOR effect occurred, F(1, 63) = 107.24, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.63, with shorter RTs to invalid (M = 420.16 ms, SE = 8.54 ms) than valid (M = 448.85 ms, SE = 8.31 ms) trials. Groups did not differ in their overall RT, F(2, 63) = 1.81, p = 0.171, η2p = 0.05. More critically, neither the Three-Way interaction of Cue Type × Validity × Group, F(6.93, 219.35) < 1, p = 0.520, η2p = 0.03, nor the Four-Way interaction of Cue Type × SOA × Validity × Group were significant, F(7.67, 241.55) < 1, p = 0.537, η2p = 0.03.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean RTs (in ms), standard deviations and IOR effects for each combination of picture type, SOA, validity, and group (offspring).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Background variables (sociodemography and psychopathology) for the depressed and non-depressed group: means (standard deviations) or frequency.

Relationship to psychopathology

For traumatized groups, there was a significant association between intrusions (assessed with the PDS) and IOR effects for anxiety-related (r = 0.347, p = 0.016) and depression-related cues (r = 0.289, p = 0.046) for short SOA. Furthermore, the IOR effect for anxiety-related cues and long SOA correlated with avoidance in the PDS (r = −0.323, p = 0.025). For the PTSD group, the association between intrusions and the IOR effect for anxiety-related cues and short SOA was even more pronounced (r = 0.595, p = 0.004). Interestingly, in the PTSD group avoidance in the PDS was correlated with an increased IOR effect for trauma-related cues (long SOA, r = −0.522, p = 0.013). Depression severity was not related to IOR effects for emotional cues, ps > 0.1. For offspring groups, depression did not correlate with any IOR effect, ps > 0.2.

Discussion

Summary of the Main Results

The first aim of the present study was to differentially assess attentional bias components (i.e., facilitation, interference, avoidance) in older trauma survivors (with and without chronic PTSD) using a spatial-cueing paradigm with pictorial stimuli of varying emotional valence. Secondly, we wanted to investigate whether children of traumatized participants would exhibit attentional biases for trauma-related material and whether this effect was attributable to parental trauma vs. PTSD.

Parents

Traumatized participants with PTSD did not show attentional interference for trauma-related stimuli (i.e., no reduction of the IOR effect), nor did they react with attentional facilitation or avoidance. However, correlational analyses revealed that the magnitude of the IOR effect for anxiety- and depression-related cues and short SOA was influenced by symptom severity. Specifically, more intrusions were related to smaller IOR effects. In contrast, for long SOA, avoidance negatively correlated with IOR effects for anxiety-related cues in the traumatized groups, that is, more self-reported avoidance was associated with larger inhibitory effects. For the PTSD group, self-reported avoidance was related to larger IOR effects for trauma-related cues which is indicative of attentional avoidance. Interestingly, when groups were dichotomized based on current depression status, a reduced IOR effect emerged for depression-related cues in depressed compared to non-depressed individuals for short SOA (i.e., 450 ms). This finding speaks to impaired disengagement in depression and was specific to depression-related material.

Our results diverge from previous studies claiming impaired disengagement in PTSD (Pineles et al., 2007, 2009) and from studies administering an EST in which—despite all methodological limitations—attentional biases for trauma-related or threatening stimuli were rather consistently found. In the present study, we also administered an EST with different emotional word conditions (Trauma, Anxiety, Depression, Neutral) and found evidence for an attentional bias for trauma-related words (Wittekind et al., in preparation). Thus, although traumatized participants with PTSD showed some kind of attentional bias, it did not become apparent using a different paradigm and stimulus modality. Due to the discrepant findings, it seems unlikely that our null findings result from a lack of power or the overall low symptom severity. Rather, different paradigms and stimulus modalities might explain the equivocal evidence and the inconsistent findings highlight the necessity to replicate results across different paradigms and modalities, respectively, before firm conclusions can be drawn. In general, attentional biases in PTSD were more consistently found under conditions in which disorder-related stimuli are present during the task (interference tasks, e.g., EST and VST) and when verbal material is applied. Divergence of findings pinpoint that different paradigms cannot self-evidently be used interchangeable as they might capture different aspects of attention (e.g., cueing: spatial attention vs. EST: selective attention, e.g., Shalev and Algom, 2000). Results suggest that PTSD is related to deficits in selective (but not in spatial) attention, possibly due to deficits in attentional control and consequently an inability to inhibit the impact of emotional distracters (Derryberry and Reed, 2002; Bardeen and Orcutt, 2011).

Another interpretation of the null findings is that the IOR effect is unaffected by emotional cues in PTSD. This interpretation is in line with previous studies that investigated IOR effects using emotional cues and found no evidence for any effects of these cues on the magnitude of the IOR effect (Stoyanova et al., 2007; Lange et al., 2008). Whereas in the study of Lange et al. IOR effects were not related to symptom severity, we found a significant relation between severity of intrusive symptomatology and the magnitude of the IOR effect indicating that IOR effects are not totally unaffected by emotionality in traumatized participants.

Regarding stimulus modality, in the spatial-cueing paradigm we used pictorial instead of verbal stimuli. Our results converge with the findings of other studies (Elsesser et al., 2005; Hauschildt et al., 2013) in which trauma or PTSD, respectively, was not associated with attentional biases for pictorial trauma-related stimuli. Furthermore, in a meta-analysis on attentional biases in anxiety disorders, attentional biases in clinical participants were only found for words, but not for pictures (Bar-Haim et al., 2007). Although it has been assumed that visual stimuli might be more attention-grabbing (Moritz et al., 2008), an advantage of verbal stimuli is that they might capture a wider range of traumatic experiences (Pineles et al., 2009). For example, in the present sample participants experienced a wide range of traumatic experiences that can more easily be grasped by broader expressions such as flight, hunger or loss than by pictures of single events.

Our finding that depressed participants exhibited interference for depression-related stimuli is noteworthy as it replicates and extends previous studies that investigated attentional biases in depression. That depression is associated with difficulties to disengage from depression-related pictorial cues corroborates prior studies that showed attentional biases for sad facial expressions in acutely (Gotlib et al., 2004; Fritzsche et al., 2010; for a review see Bistricky et al., 2011) and formerly depressed individuals (e.g., Fritzsche et al., 2010). Whereas most of the forerunner studies administered variants of the dot-probe task, in the present study results could be replicated with a different paradigm. Interestingly, although the IOR effect was immune to emotionality in anxiety, it was reduced in depression suggesting that it doesn't represent a stable phenomenon per se. We can only speculate why this discrepancy occurred. One possibility is that whereas emotional facial expressions are more salient stimuli (e.g., Bradley et al., 1997; Bistricky et al., 2011), anxiety-related stimuli in this study did not contain biologically relevant information. Rather, trauma-related cues become associated with threat during the traumatic event but are not inherently dangerous (Ehlers and Clark, 2000). Difficulties to disengage from depression-related stimuli might constitute a risk and maintaining factor for depression as attention remains on mood-congruent stimuli and this in turn might potentiate processes such as rumination, negative thinking or a negative emotional state (e.g., Beck, 1967; Ingram, 1984).

The finding that reduced IOR effects were only found for short (i.e., 450 ms) but not long (i.e., 1250 ms) SOA is unexpected as attentional biases in depression are assumed to occur at later stages of information processing that need more time to take effect (i.e., strategic processing, see for example, Yiend, 2010). However, neuroscience studies provide evidence that biases might also affect automatic (early) processes (Suslow et al., 2010).

Offspring

Regarding our second question, there was no evidence for a transgenerational transmission of attentional biases, that is, offspring of traumatized participants (with PTSD) did not react differently to trauma-related cues (i.e., no reduced IOR effect). Our findings diverge from studies with Vietnam veterans in which children of veterans exhibited attentional biases for trauma-related material compared to children of non-veterans (Motta et al., 1994, 1997). However, our results are in line with a forerunner study in which children of displaced individuals (with and without PTSD) did not show attentional biases for trauma-related material in an EST (Wittekind et al., 2010). Furthermore, in an EST, which was also administered in the present study (Wittekind et al., in preparation), there was no evidence for attentional biases in children of traumatized participants. Comparability between studies is constrained by methodological differences, for example, age of children at assessment or attentional paradigm (EST vs. spatial-cueing). Beyond that, group differences in the studies of Motta et al. might be attributable to differences in personal relevance as PTSD (i.e., Vietnam)-related words might be more personally relevant for children of veterans compared to non-veterans and personal relevance is associated with longer color naming latencies (e.g., Williams et al., 1996). However, personal relevance was not controlled for, neither by obtaining ratings of the stimuli nor by the inclusion of traumatized parents with and without PTSD. Taken together, results of the present and previous studies argue for the conclusion that parental trauma or PTSD due to forced displacement is not related to attentional biases for trauma-related material in their children. These findings are in line with the broader literature on secondary traumatization which provides evidence that children of traumatized individuals are well adjusted (e.g., Van IJzendoorn et al., 2003; Fridman et al., 2011).

Limitations

Results of the present study need to be interpreted against the background of several limitations. First, as more than 65 years passed between initial traumatization and assessment, we cannot answer the question whether attentional biases had occurred earlier in time. Second, sample size was rather small making it more difficult to detect subtle differences. However, as traumatization dates back more than 65 years, many of the individuals affected might not be available for assessment as traumatization and PTSD in particular are associated with higher morbidity and mortality (Boscarino, 2006; Glaesmer et al., 2011). In consequence, only the more resilient individuals might been reached for assessment. Thus, the sample under investigation represents a specific population and it remains to be tested whether findings can be transferred to other trauma populations. Second, Mogg et al. (2008) argue that findings from spatial-cueing paradigms might represent a generic response slowing for threat-related stimuli rather than a “pure index of disengagement processes” (p. 665). However, this problem also applies to other attentional paradigms (e.g., Algom et al., 2004). Furthermore, as we applied a localization instead of a categorization task, it is conceivable that attentional effects were confounded by motor preparation effects. To circumvent these latter confounds, future studies should combine attentional paradigms with paradigms that allow the assessment of visuospatial attention allocation, for example eye-tracking. Third, the applied cut off for RTs represent a limitation as we did not apply standard cut off values, for example, two standard deviations. However, as our aim was to extend and replicate findings of Hauschildt et al. (2013), we decided to keep the same strategy of data analysis to provide better comparability. Fourth, although trauma-related pictures were on average rated as negative and personally relevant by participants with PTSD, it is still conceivable that depicted trauma-related events (e.g., refugee treks) were not experienced by all individuals as flight histories differed substantially among traumatized participants. Finally, within both the PTSD and the depressed group comorbidity with depression or PTDS, respectively, was the norm rather than exception. In consequence, it remains unresolved whether attentional biases in the depressed group were indeed attributable to depression or related to comorbidity. Studies are needed that compare pure depression- and anxiety samples with a mixed depression-anxiety sample.

Conclusions

Attentional biases for emotional visual cues were related to depression, not PTSD, in an older trauma sample (with and without PTSD). Specifically, depression was associated with attentional interference for depression-related stimuli. Results of the present study do not support the assumption that PTSD in older adults is associated with difficulties to disengage from pictorial trauma-related stimuli. Rather, it seems that attentional biases in PTSD are specific to verbal stimuli and that selective, but not spatial, attention is affected. Future studies should directly compare visual and verbal stimuli within one paradigm and assess both selective and spatial attention. Beyond that, future studies should assess whether information processing biases in older trauma survivors resemble the ones found for younger trauma samples, for example, by directly comparing acute and chronic PTSD samples. Furthermore, there was no evidence for a transgenerational transmission of biased information processing. However, as offspring of the current study was rather old, it would be interesting to investigate information processing biases in younger children of parents with PTSD. One promising means to treat attentional biases represent attentional bias modification paradigms (ABM-trainings, Browning et al., 2010; Hakamata et al., 2010).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This study was supported by a PhD scholarship of the University Hamburg granted to Charlotte E. Wittekind. We would like to thank Katja Vent and Friederike Behmer for their support with the data collection.

References

Algom, D., Chajut, E., and Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 323–338. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Baider, L., Goldzweig, G., Ever-Hadani, P., and Peretz, T. (2006). Psychological distress and coping in breast cancer patients and healthy women whose parents survived the Holocaust. Psychooncology 15, 635–646. doi: 10.1002/pon.1010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baider, L., Peretz, T., Hadani, P. E., Perry, S., Avramov, R., and De-Nour, A. K. (2000). Transmission of response to trauma? Second-generation Holocaust survivors' reaction to cancer. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 904–910. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.6.904

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardeen, J. R., and Orcutt, H. K. (2011). Attentional control as a moderator of the relationship between posttraumatic stress symptoms and attentional threat bias. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 1008–1018. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Holoshitz, Y., Eldar, S., Frenkel, T. I., Muller, D., Charney, D. S., et al. (2010). Life-threatening danger and suppression of attention bias to threat. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 694–698. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070956

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Bistricky, S. L., Ingram, R. E., and Atchley, R. A. (2011). Facial affect processing and depression susceptibility: cognitive biases and cognitive neuroscience. Psychol. Bull. 137, 998–1028. doi: 10.1037/a0025348

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Buckley, T. C., Taylor, A. E., Vollmer, A., and Loos, W. R. (1996). Psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder related to motor vehicle accidents: replication and extension. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 64, 742–751. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.4.742

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boscarino, J. A. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder and mortality among U.S. Army veterans 30 years after military service. Ann. Epidemiol. 16, 248–256. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., Bonham-Carter, C., Fergusson, E., Jenkins, J., et al. (1997). Attentional biases for emotional faces. Cogn. Emot. 11, 25–42. doi: 10.1080/026999397380014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, J. D., Vermetten, E., Vythilingam, M., Afzal, N., Schmahl, B. E., and Charney, D. S. (2004). Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 612–620. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Holmes, E. A., and Harmer, C. J. (2010). The modification of attentional bias to emotional information: a review of the techniques, mechanisms, and relevance to emotional disorders. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 8–20. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., and Harvey, A. G. (1997). Attentional bias in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 10, 635–644. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490100409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, B. E., and Neill, W. T. (2000). Information processing and PTSD: a review of the empirical literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 1041–1065. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00030-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., Bacon, A. K., and Williams, N. L. (2009). Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: a critical review. Cogn. Ther. Res. 33, 221–234. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9161-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., and Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 203–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Adams, T. G. Jr., Babson, K. A., Badour, C. L., and Willems, J. L. (2011). The emotional Stroop task and posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 817–828. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constans, J. I. (2005). “Information-processing biases in PTSD,” in Neuropsychology of PTSD: Biological, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives, eds J. J. Vasterling and C. R. Brewin (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 105–130.

Google Scholar

Dalgleish, T., Taghavi, R., Neshat-Doost, H. T., Moradi, A. R., Canterbury, R., and Yule, W. (2003). Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: a comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 32, 10–21. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3201_02

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danieli, Y. (1998). International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Derryberry, D., and Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111, 225–236. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 319–345. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Khoury-Malhame, M., Lanteaume, L., Beetz, E. M., Roques, J., Reynaud, E., Samuelian, J.-C., et al. (2011a). Attentional bias in post-traumatic stress disorder diminishes after symptom amelioration. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 796–801. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Khoury-Malhame, M., Reynaud, E., Soriano, A., Michael, K., Salgado-Pineda, P., Zendjidjian, X., et al. (2011b). Amygdala activity correlates with attentional bias in PTSD. Neuropsychologia 49, 1969–1973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsesser, K., Sartory, G., and Tackenberg, A. (2004). Attention, heart rate, and startle response during exposure to trauma-relevant pictures: a comparison of recent trauma victims and patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 289–301. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.289

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsesser, K., Sartory, G., and Tackenberg, A. (2005). Initial symptoms and reactions to trauma-related stimuli and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress. Anxiety 21, 61–70. doi: 10.1002/da.20047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fani, N., Bradley-Davino, B., Ressler, K. J., and McClure-Tone, E. B. (2011). Attention bias in adult survivors of childhood maltreatment with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 57–67. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9294-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazel, M., Wheeler, J., and Danesh, J. (2005). Prevalence of serious mental disorder in 7000 refugees resettled in western countries: a systematic review. Lancet 365, 1309–1314. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)61027-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. B. W. (1997). User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders SCID-I. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Google Scholar

Fleurkens, P., Rinck, M., and van Minnen, A. (2011). Specificity and generalization of attentional bias in sexual trauma victims suffering from posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 783–787. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.03.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foa, E. B., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., and Perry, K. (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychol. Assess. 9, 445–451. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, E., Russo, R., and Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cogn. Emot. 16, 355–379. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000527

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitag, S., Braehler, E., Schmidt, S., and Glaesmer, H. (2013). The impact of forced displacement in World War II on mental health disorders and health-related quality of life in late life—a German population-based study. Int. Psychogeriat. 25, 310–319. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212001585

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridman, A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Sagi-Schwartz, A., and Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Coping in old age with extreme childhood trauma: aging Holocaust survivors and their offspring facing new challenges. Aging Ment. Health 2, 232–242. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2010.505232

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritzsche, A., Dahme, B., Gotlib, I. H., Joormann, J., Magnussen, H., Watz, H., et al. (2010). Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients. Psychol. Med. 40, 815–826. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990948

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaesmer, H., Braehler, E., Gündel, H., and Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2011). The association of traumatic experiences and posttraumatic stress disorder with physical morbidity in old age: a German population-based study. Psychosom. Med. 73, 401–406. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31821b47e8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Joormann, J., Arnow, B. A., and Johnson, S. L. (2004). Coherence and specificity of informationprocessing biases in depression and social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 386–398. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 982–990. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–61. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, A. G., Bryant, R. A., and Rapee, R. M. (1996). Preconscious processing of threat in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 20, 613–623. doi: 10.1007/BF02227964

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauschildt, M., Wittekind, C. E., Moritz, S., and Jelinek, L. (2013). Attentional bias to affective visual stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 207, 73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 8, 443–477. doi: 10.1007/BF01173284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimble, M. O., Frueh, B. C., and Marks, L. (2009). Does the modified Stroop effect exist in PTSD? Evidence from dissertation abstracts and the peer reviewed literature. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 650–655. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinnear, P. R., and Gray, C. D. (2008). SPSS 16 Made Simple. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 138–147. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01452-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., and De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to threat in anxiety-prone individuals: mechanisms underlying attentional bias. Cogn. Ther. Res. 30, 635–643. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9042-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures And Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Lange, W.-G., Heuer, K., Reinecke, A., Becker, E. S., and Rinck, M. (2008). Inhibition of return is unimpressed by emotional cues. Cogn. Emot. 22, 1433–1456. doi: 10.1080/02699930701809451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leen-Feldner, E. W., Feldner, M. T., Knapp, A., Bunaciu, L., Blumenthal, H., and Amstadter, A. B. (2013). Offspring psychological and biological correlates of parental posttraumatic stress: review of the literature and research agenda. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 1106–1133. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrl, S. (2005). Manual zum MWT-B, 5th Edn. Balingen: Spitta.

Levav, I., Levinson, D., Radomislensky, I., Shemesh, A. A., and Kohn, R. (2007). Psychopathology and other health dimensions among the offspring of holocaust survivors: results from the Israel National Health Survey. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 44, 144–151.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., and Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., and Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 656–667. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moradi, A. R., Neshat-Doost, H., Taghavi, R., Yule, W., and Dalgleish, T. (1999). Performance of children of adults with PTSD on the Stroop color-naming task: a preliminary study. J. Trauma. Stress 12, 663–671. doi: 10.1023/A:1024721218869

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Fischer, B.-K., Hottenrott, B., Kellner, M., Fricke, S., Randjbar, S., et al. (2008). Words may not be enough! No increased emotional Stroop effect in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1101–1104. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Von Mühlenen, D. (2005). Inhibition of return in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 19, 117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Von Mühlenen, D., Randjbar, S., Fricke, S., and Jelinek, L. (2009). Evidence for an attentional bias for washing- and checking-relevant stimuli in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 365–371. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090511

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motta, R. W., Joseph, J. M., Rose, R. D., Suozzi, J. M., and Leiderman, L. J. (1997). Secondary trauma: assessing inter-generational transmission of war experiences with a modified Stroop procedure. J. Clin. Psychol. 53, 895–903. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199712)53:8<895::AID-JCLP14>3.0.CO;2-F

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motta, R. W., Suozzi, J. M., and Jospeh, J. M. (1994). Assessment of secondary traumatization with an emotional Stroop task. Percept. Mot. Skills 78:1274. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.78.3c.1274

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muhtz, C., von Alm, C., Godemann, K., Wittekind, C., Jelinek, L., Yassouridis, A., et al. (2011). Langzeitfolgen von in der Kindheit am Ende des II. Weltkrieges erlebter Flucht und Vertreibung. Psychother. Psychosom. Med. Psychol. 61, 233–238. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1275341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E., Early, T. S., and Haller, J. W. (1993). Visual attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 49, 183–196. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90104-O

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhmann, A., Flykt, A., and Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paunovic, N., Lundh, L.-G., and Öst, L.-G. (2002). Attentional and memory bias for emotional information in crime victims with acute posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J. Anxiety Disord. 16, 675–692. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00136-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietrzak, R. H., Goldstein, R. B., Southwick, S. M., and Grant, B. F. (2011). Prevalence and Axis I comorbidity of full and partial posttraumatic stress disorder in the United States: results from Wave 2 of the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 456–465. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.11.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pineles, S. L., Shipherd, J. C., Mostoufi, S. M., Abramovitz, S. M., and Yovel, I. (2009). Attentional biases in PTSD: more evidence for interference. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 1050–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pineles, S. L., Shipherd, J. C., Welch, L. P., and Yovel, I. (2007). The role of attentional biases in PTSD: is it interference or inhibition? Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1903–1913. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.08.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, M., and Haslam, N. (2005). Predisplacement and postdisplacement factors associated with mental health of refugees and internally displaced persons. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 294, 602–612. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.602

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orientation of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Cohen, Y. (1984). “Components of visual orienting,” in Attention and Performance, Vol. X, eds H. Bouma and D. Bouwhuis (London: Erlbaum), 531–556.

Google Scholar

Shalev, L., and Algom, D. (2000). Stroop and Garner effects in and out of Posner's Beam: reconciling two conceptions of selective attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 997–1017. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.3.997

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59, 22–33.

Shipherd, J. C., and Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Attention, memory, intrusive thoughts, and acceptance in PTSD: an update on the empirical literature for clinicians. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 15, 349–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, Z., Chey, T., Silove, D., Marnane, C., Bryant, R. A., and van Ommeren, M. (2009). Association of torture and other potentially traumatic events with mental health outcomes among populations exposed to mass conflict and displacement: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 302, 537–549. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1132

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoyanova, R. S., Pratt, J., and Anderson, A. K. (2007). Inhibition of return to social signals of fear. Emotion 7, 49–56. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.49

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suozzi, J. M., and Motta, R. W. (2004). The relationship between combat exposure and the transfer of trauma-like symptoms to offspring of veterans. Traumatology 10, 17–37. doi: 10.1177/153476560401000103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schöning, S., et al. (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 155–160. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teegen, F., and Meister, V. (2000). Traumatische Erfahrungen deutscher Flüchtlinge am Ende des II. Weltkrieges und heutige Belastungsstörungen. Z. Gerontopsychol. Psychiatrie 13, 112–124. doi: 10.1024/1011-6877.13.34.112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations Refugee Agency (UNHCR) (2014). Global Trends Report 2013. Available online at: http://www.unhcr.org/5399a14f9.html

Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Sagi-Schwartz, A. (2003). Are the children of Holocaust survivors less well-adapted? A meta-analytic investigation of secondary traumatization. J. Trauma. Stress 16, 459–469. doi: 10.1023/A:1025706427300

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wald, I., Lubin, G., Holoshitz, Y., Muller, D., Fruchter, E., Pine, D. S., et al. (2011a). Battlefield-like stress following simulated combat and suppression of attention bias to threat. Psychol. Med. 41, 699–707. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002308

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wald, I., Shechner, T., Bitton, S., Holoshitz, Y., Charney, D. S., Muller, D., et al. (2011b). Attention bias away from threat during life threatening danger predicts PTSD symptoms at one-year follow-up. Depress. Anxiety 28, 406–411. doi: 10.1002/da.20808

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., and MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol. Bull. 120, 3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittekind, C. E., Jelinek, L., Kellner, M., Moritz, S., and Muhtz, C. (2010). Intergenerational transmission of biased information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) following displacement after World War II. J. Anxiety Disord. 24, 953–957. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehuda, R., Bell, A., Bierer, L. M., and Schmeidler, J. (2008). Maternal, not parental, PTSD is related to increased risk for PTSD in offspring of Holocaust survivors. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 1104–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: a review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cogn. Emot. 24, 3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attentional bias, trauma, depression, bias components, transmission

Citation: Wittekind CE, Muhtz C, Jelinek L and Moritz S (2015) Depression, not PTSD, is associated with attentional biases for emotional visual cues in early traumatized individuals with PTSD. Front. Psychol. 5:1474. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01474

Received: 01 August 2014; Accepted: 01 December 2014;
Published online: 06 January 2015.

Edited by:

Ulrich S. Tran, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Nicolas Koranyi, University of Jena, Germany
Marijn Lijffijt, Baylor College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2015 Wittekind, Muhtz, Jelinek and Moritz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Charlotte E. Wittekind, Clinical Neuropsychology, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistr. 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany e-mail: c.wittekind@uke.uni-hamburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.