Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 September 2013
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Affective Disorders in Focus. Cognitive Functioning and Emotional Information Processing. View all 8 articles

Symptoms of depression as reported by Norwegian adolescents on the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire

  • 1Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare, Uni Health, Uni Research, Bergen, Norway
  • 3K. G. Jebsen Center for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 4Department of Clinical Medicine, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway

The present study investigated sex-differences in reports of depressive symptoms on a Norwegian translation of the short version of the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ). The sample comprised 9702 Norwegian adolescents (born 1993–1995, 54.9% girls), mainly attending highschool. A set of statistical analyses were run to investigate the dimensionality of the SMFQ. Girls scored significantly higher than boys on the SMFQ and used the most severe response-category far more frequently. Overall, the statistical analyses supported the essential unidimensionality of SMFQ. However, the items with the highest loadings according to the bifactor analysis, reflecting problems related to tiredness, restlessness and concentration difficulties, indicated that some of the symptoms may both be independent of and part of the symptomatology of depression. Measurement invariance analysis showed that girls scored slightly higher on some items when taking the latent variable into account; girls had a lower threshold for reporting mood problems and problems related to tiredness than boys, who showed a marginally lower threshold for reporting that no-one loved them. However, the effect on the total SMFQ score was marginal, supporting the use of the Norwegian translation of SMFQ as a continuous variable in further studies of adolescents.

Introduction

Psychiatric disorders are highly frequent in adolescence (Costello et al., 2011). Depression is probably the most frequent of them all (Kessler et al., 2005), with female gender as one of the main risk factors (Klein et al., 2013). The greatest increase in sex-difference occurs during adolescence (Hankin et al., 1998). At that age, population-based studies have shown that girls have a more than two-fold higher risk of depression than boys (Merikangas et al., 2010). This female:male ratio has been explained by sex-difference in pubertal development (Joinson et al., 2011, 2012; Black and Klein, 2012), but psychosocial (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Copeland et al., 2009; Conley et al., 2012) and cognitive factors (Labelle et al., 2013) are probably as important.

Depressive symptomatology is associated with high rates of comorbid mental health problems (Angold et al., 1999a), social and academic dysfunction, suicide and substance abuse (Birmaher et al., 2007). Early identification and treatment may thus be crucial for present and future functioning. A main obstacle to early identification has been the lack of screening-instruments adapted to the child and adolescent language and daily life situations. The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) was developed as a response to that need (Angold et al., 1987). The questionnaire captures symptoms included in the DSM-IV criteria for major depressive disorders (Costello and Angold, 1988), and is an appropriate assessment tool in the age range 8–18 years. MFQ is validated in a large sample of both clinical and non-clinical subjects (Daviss et al., 2006) and has been translated and been part of population-based studies in Norway (Sund et al., 2003, 2011). A short version of MFQ (SMFQ) has also been developed as a promising option to validly assess core depressive symptoms in epidemiological studies (Angold et al., 1995). A strong unidimensionality and high internal consistency of SMFQ have been documented in a population-based study (Sharp et al., 2006).

The items included in SMFQ assess mood and anhedonia, tiredness, restlessness, concentration difficulties, and several aspects of negative self-evaluation. In their Item Response Theory (IRT) analysis, Sharp et al. (2006) showed that SMFQ items reflecting negative self-evaluation (referred to as cognitive by Sharp et al.) were characterized by high loadings on a unidimensional latent factor of depression. According to Nolen-Hoeksema (1991), different responses on such items may explain the sex differences seen in adolescents. Girls tend to ruminate more than boys, leading to a passive and repeated focus on ones depressive symptoms, causes and consequences (Watkins and Moulds, 2005). Rumination is shown to increase the vulnerability to develop depressive symptoms, and tends to increase the duration and severity of existing symptoms (Abela and Hankin, 2011). On the other hand, such thoughts may be easier to handle for girls than for boys, in that their social skills may help them to get support. In a population-based study of 13–17 years olds, Derdikman-Eiron et al. (2012) showed that symptoms of anxiety and depressions actually had more negative consequences for boys than for girls; when boys reported depressive symptoms, these symptoms had a stronger impact on their function. Finally, it is also a question as to whether a more affective response-style in girls than boys contributes to reports of more severe depressive symptoms in girls. This was indicated by a recent study of 8–17 years olds (van Beek et al., 2012), which showed that girls tend to report more severe problems on a questionnaire than expected from an evaluation based on a formal clinical assessment.

Assessment using questionnaires is challenged by the fact that depressive symptoms are very common, and that there is not a clear-cut distinction between case and noncase. This is especially true for emotional problems in adolescence, when feelings of misery and unhappiness are part of normal development. These mood related problems might be hard to differentiate from symptoms that are predictive of persistent mental health problems into adulthood. The importance of symptoms below the threshold of a diagnosis of depression in adolescents was illustrated in a study by Fergusson et al. (2005). In a 25-year longitudinal study of a birth cohort including 1265 children, they showed that symptoms of depression at a subclinical level at age 17–18 represented a highly relevant risk factor of depressive disorder and suicidal behavior later in life. The importance of subclinical symptoms of depression was also illustrated by results in a study of Norwegian families where an adolescent had committed suicide (Freuchen et al., 2012). Retrospectively, parents frequently had noticed symptoms of depressed mood before the fatal behavior, but at a level that did not elicit enough worry to engage professional help. These studies emphasize the importance of evaluating reports on questionnaires as a continuous rather than categorical variable, and to reveal symptoms that are predictive of persistent psychiatric problems and self-harm.

The aim of the present study was to investigate sex-difference in SMFQ reports in a large population-based sample of adolescents. Although the unidimensionality of SMFQ is known from an earlier study (Sharp et al., 2006), we found it necessary to analyse the factor structure of the Norwegian translation as part of the present study, separately for boys and for girls, with follow-up of differences with measurement invariance analyses. Revealing unidimensionality would enable using the total SMFQ score as a continuous variable representing severity of depressive symptoms. From earlier studies we expected to find that girls would report depressive symptoms more frequently and at a more severe level than boys.

Methods

The present study included data from the ung@hordaland-survey of adolescents in the county of Hordaland, Western Norway, where all adolescents born between 1993 and 1995 living in the county and all students attending highschool during the spring 2012 were invited to participate. The adolescents received information via e-mail, and one school hour was allocated for them to complete the questionnaire. Adolescents not in school received information by postal mail to their home addresses. The questionnaire was web-based and covered a broad range of mental health- and demographic background variables. The Regional Center for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare, Uni Research, collaborated with Hordaland County Council to conduct the study. The study was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Western Norway. The current study is based on the first version of quality-assured data files released in May 2012, and included the adolescent's self-reports on the SMFQ.

Sample

Of the included adolescents (born 1993–1995, N = 19, 121), 10,220 completed or filled in parts of the questionnaire, yielding a participation rate of 53%. A total of 518 adolescents were excluded due to missing reports on some of the SMFQ items, resulting in a total sample of 9702 adolescents included in the present study. Information about sex and year of birth were based on the personal identity number in the Norwegian National Population Register.

Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire

In the original version of the MFQ, the respondents are asked to rate 33 items on a 3-point scale, indicating how much they have felt or acted that way during the past 2 weeks (Angold et al., 1987). In the present study we included the official Norwegian version of the short version (SMFQ) (Angold et al., 1995), with 13 items focusing on affective and cognitive symptoms. The wordings of the response-categories in the Norwegian translation equals the original categories of Not true, Sometimes true, and True.

Statistical Analyses

SPSS version 18 was applied for descriptive statistics (SPSS I, 1999). The remaining analyses were conducted using Mplus version 7.0 (Muthen and Muthen, 2012). Missing data were dealt with using pair-wise deletion, which is Mplus default when analysing categorical outcome variables with the robust-weighted least square estimator (WLSMV). Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFAs) were used on reports from girls and boys separately. The WLSMV was used in the CFAs due to highly skewed categorical data (ordinal data with three options). WLMSV uses polychoric correlations for estimation and seems relatively robust to violations of normality (Flora and Curran, 2004; Brown, 2006). Bentlers comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, 1990), TuckerÐLewis Index (TLI) (Tucker and Lewis, 1973) and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger and Lind, 1980) were used for evaluation of model fit. The cut-off values to determine acceptable fit followed the suggestions given by Yu (2002): CFI 0.96, TLI 0.95 and RMSEA 0.05. An adjusted chi-square test (DIFFTEST) (Muthen and Muthen, 2012) will be reported, as it is appropriate when comparing difference in fit between nested models. Explorative bifactor analyses were conducted when the hypothesized unidimensional model did not fit the data.

Measurement invariance between boys and girls was tested by using a top down strategy (Muthen and Muthen, 2012), where the fit of a model of which the loadings and thresholds were held equal between the sexes was compared to a model of which the same parameters (except for the identification item) were free to vary. The model was assumed non-invariant if the adjusted chi square was significant and the change in CFI was <= −0.002 (Meade et al., 2008). The loadings and threshold (in tandem) of potentially problematic items (based on having a modification index >10) were then sequently set to be free between the sexes.

Results

Demographics

The mean age of the sample was 17 years (SD = 0.87) and 53.5% of the respondents were girls (n = 5188). The majority (98%) were highschool students. The proportion of immigrants in the present sample, defined as having both parents born outside Norway, was 5.6% (n = 545).

Factor Models for Depressive Symptoms in Boys and Girls

The 1 factor (unidimensional) confirmatory model had only a modest fit to data for both boys and girls in the present sample, with an adequate fit for the CFI and TLI (girls: CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.96; boys: CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.97), but a somewhat high RMSEA (0.09 for girls and 0.07 for boys). Inspection of the modification indexes suggested that the misfit could partly be due to local dependencies between items 3, 4, 7, items 1, 2, and items 8, 9. Explorative factor analyses [estimator = geomin rotated solution (oblique)] on the polychorical correlation matrix were therefore conducted. A similar three factor solution with good fit and theoretical sense was found for both sexes (negative self-evaluations: item 5, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13; negative mood: item 1, 2, and 6; and a factor assessing restlessness, tiredness and concentration problems: item 3, 4, and 7). The results from the explorative factor analyses suggested, however, that the data were characterized by one dominant factor as the ratio between the first and second eigenvalue was large for both girls (7.87 vs. 1.15, 61% of the variance explained by the first factor) and boys (8.96 vs. 1.01, 67% of the variance explained by the first factor).

Explorative bifactor analyses (Bi-cf-quartimax (orthogonal) rotation) with a four factor solution were therefore conducted. The fit of this model was excellent for both sexes (RMSEA < 0.04 and CFI > 0.99). All items except item 4 (restlessness) had a rather strong loading (>0.60) on the general factor (Tables 1, 2). The loadings on the domain specific factors correspond to a certain extent with what was discovered in the EFA analyses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The factor structure of the Norwegian translation of SMFQ: girls.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The factor structure of the Norwegian translation of SMFQ: boys.

Although the results supported a dominant factor, the multi-group analyses Muthen and Muthen (2012) on the 1 factor solution suggested that some of the items function differently for the two sexes. The fully constrained model, where the loadings and thresholds were set to equal between the two sexes, had a somewhat poorer fit (Δx2 = 552.28, df = 24, p < 0.001, ΔCFI = −0.002) compared to the least restrictive model where the same (except for the identification item) parameters were set free. Gradually releasing the equality constraints (based on the modification indexes) suggested that five of the items functioned somewhat differently between the sexes. Due to similar item loadings between the two sexes, they were constrained to be equal, resulting in a final partial invariance model which still had a somewhat poorer fit than the least unrestricted model (Δx2 = 81.44, df = 19, p < 0.001).

The CFI index was in fact larger (ΔCFI = 0.009) for the partial invariance model, which supports its greater parsimony compared to the least restrictive model. Given the same level of depression, girls had a lower threshold for reporting that they felt miserable or unhappy (1), to feel so tired they just sat around doing nothing (3) and to cry a lot (6) than boys. Boys had a marginally lower threshold to report that they believed no one loved them (11) than girls with a similar depression trait value. Taking the different item functions (DIF) into account had a minimal impact on the estimated mean sex-difference, as girls had 1.05 Standard deviation units higher score based on latent mean depression score than boys in the final partial measurement model [Cohens d (Cohen, 1988)Δ 0.47 latent mean, variance girls 0.622, boys 0.708], compared to 0.98 Standard deviation units (latent mean Δ 0.68 variance girls = 0.653, boys = 0.943) when the fully constrained model was used.

Total SMFQ Score

The high factor loadings on the general factor allowed using the total SMFQ score as an indicator of severity. Girls obtained a statistically significant higher mean score (7.4 (6.1)) than boys (4.1 (4.9), t = 28.8, p < 0.001), with a medium to high effect-size (d = 0.60). Figure 1, showing the distribution of the scores within the two sex-groups, revealed that a high number of boys reported no problems on all items (i.e., a score = 0). Although the frequency was lower than for girls on most other scores, the boys were distributed throughout the most rightward length.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Distribution of scores on the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ), given in separate plots for females (to the left) and males (to the right).

Sex Differences in Scores on the SMFQ Items

The percentage of responses within the three response-categories are given in Table 3. A high percentage of female adolescents answered True on one or more items, ranging from 5.6% up to 25.9%, with the highest reports on items assessing mood problems (1), tiredness (3), poor concentration (7), loneliness (10) and feeling of being no good (5). In general, girls used the response-category True significantly more frequently than boys on all items (p < 0.001). The female:male ratios (OR) of using the most extreme response options were high, ranging from about two-fold up to a more than five-fold increase (cried a lot).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Percentage of girls and boys with a given response on the SMFQ items.

Discussion

The present study showed that the Norwegian translation of SMFQ is essentially unidimensional, with one dominant depression factor explaining much of the item responses. By this, the use of the sum score of SMFQ as a continuous measure of severity was supported. There was a high rate of depressive symptoms in this population-based sample of 16–19 years old adolescents. The frequency of girls using the most severe response category (i.e., True) to describe their problems was higher than for boys. Among those reporting symptoms, a high proportion of the boys reported Sometimes true on the items, with the highest frequency on items reflecting tiredness, restlessness and concentration problems.

The higher scores on items assessing depressive symptoms in girls than in boys confirmed results from several previous studies (Angold et al., 2002; Merikangas et al., 2010; Joinson et al., 2011), with the present study showing a difference in the magnitude of medium to high effect size. A more severe depression in girls than boys was also confirmed by the multi-group analysis. The importance of being aware of depressive symptoms in girls has been emphasized by studies showing that such symptoms place them at risk for future social and academic problems (Birmaher et al., 2007), as well as related problems such as suicidal thoughts/behavior (Fergusson et al., 2005) and drug problems (Fombonne et al., 2001). Findings in a recent study by Monshouwer et al. (2012) showed the importance of detecting depressive symptoms in girls at an early adolescent age: onset of depression when the child was 15 years or younger was predictive of more long-lasting and severe future episodes of depression than for adolescents with a later onset.

Although not as frequent as in girls, a high number of boys did report problems. Problems relating to mood, anhedonia, hypo-activity, poor concentration and feeling of loneliness were reported as Sometimes true or True by between 20 and 30% of the boys. The importance of being aware of and help boys to cope with depressive symptoms was emphasized by a longitudinal population-based study of children from 13 to 19 years (Derdikman-Eiron et al., 2012), showing that symptoms of depression during this age period were related to even larger functional impairment for boys than for girls; boys showed more academic, behavioral and social problems than girls after remission. If we assume that at least some of the boys are impaired even though they use a less severe response category than girls, it may be crucial to give such responses attention. However, further studies validating reports on SMFQ against conventional clinical diagnostic interviews are needed to investigate this assumption.

The factor analysis of the Norwegian translation of SMFQ is one of the main contributions of the present study. Overall, the results confirmed the essential unidimensionality of SMFQ described by Sharp et al. (2006), although they included a much younger sample (7–11 years) and showed a similar factor structure for girls and boys. These findings of a strong depression factor gave solid arguments for comparing adolescent boys and girls on a sum-scale of severity. At the same time, the sex difference was noteworthy higher if the degree of depression in girls and boys was compared by the use of the latent depression variable (Cohens d = 0.98) instead of the sumscore (Cohens d = 0.60). Future sex-comparisons on the SMFQ should therefore be conducted on the latent variable as this takes into account error variance and the fact that the items are at an ordinal level.

In their IRT analysis, Sharp et al. (2006) showed that some of the items that we have referred to as part of a negative self-evaluation factor (8: hated self, 9: bad person, 13: did everything wrong) showed a high discriminating power on all levels of the SMFQ. These items were allocated to one of the three bifactors in the present study. One should, however, be careful in treating these domain specific factors (related to negative mood, negative self-evaluation and psychomotor agitation/somatic) as subscales. The results from the bifactor analysis suggest that such subscales would be rather useless for measuring domain specific content due to low reliability. Items belonging to these domain specific factors most often had a much higher loading on the general factor. Such subscales would therefore to a large extent reflect the general factor instead of the domain specific area of which it would be labeled.

Although a follow-up analysis showed that they loaded strongly on a general factor, an interesting sex-difference emerged. Given the same level of depression, girls had a lower threshold for reporting crying and feeling miserable and unhappy and being tired than boys. The sex difference related to crying has been shown in many previous studies on depression scales [e.g., (Teresi et al., 2009)] and probably reflects different gender norms of how acceptable it is to cry when feeling sad. It may also indicate that girls are somewhat more aware of negative feelings when depressed, as this could explain the tie between the reports of crying and feeling miserable and unhappy. Interestingly, boys were somewhat more likely to report that no one loved them than girls at a similar level of depression. We are hesitant to interpret this sex-difference until it is replicated as it was marginal. However, the results showed only a minor impact on the scale level. This supports that comparisons of depression can be made using the sum-score of SMFQ despite the fact that some items functioned differently for boys and girls.

Sharp et al. (2006) found the items related to tiredness (3), restlessness (4) and concentration (7) to have a very weak discriminative power in the severe end of the SMFQ. These three items were also found to have the most ambiguous loadings in the bifactor model of the present study, and it is thus tempting to suggest that this bifactor is related to sleep and/or concentration difficulties that adolescents may suffer from both independently and in conjunction with depressive symptomatology. First of all, restlessness and tiredness may be related to changes in the sleep-wake-pattern during adolescence, due to physiological and psychological factors (Gradisar et al., 2011). Problems related to concentration, with huge impact on adolescents attending highschool education, may be closely related to brain development (Buitelaar, 2012) as well as a range of contextual factors. Further studies are called for, both given the established link between depression and insomnia (Roane and Taylor, 2008; Sivertsen et al., 2012; Danielsson et al., 2013) and between depression and cognitive problems (Austin et al., 2001; Ardal et al., 2012; Hammar and Ardal, 2012).

Strengths and Limitations

The large sample-size and the statistical analysis of the Norwegian translation of the SMFQ are main strengths of the present study. It is, however, important to be aware that we used a short version of MFQ, which does not include important symptoms of depression like suicidality/self-harm, rumination and biological features like eating little/much, sleeping little/much. In addition to the danger of missing some important aspects of depression, our results did also indicate that several symptoms are probably shared by other disorders, indicating that SMFQ may also be overinclusive.

The lack of including information regarding co-occurring problems is probably a main limitation of the present study, in that co-existence of mental health problems tends to be the rule rather than the exception (Gillberg, 2010). Studies have shown that between 40 and 90 percent of adolescents with depressive disorders have other psychiatric disorders, of whom about half have two or more diagnoses (Birmaher et al., 2007). In adolescents, comorbidity with anxiety as well as ADHD and other neuro-developmental disorders are frequent and have a substantial influence on persistence and future function (Angold et al., 1999b). These co-existing problems or disorders could account for some of the gender differences, as well as some of the weaker factor loadings. Furthermore, it was beyond the scope of the present study to investigate if there are differences in functional outcomes for girls and boys on factors such as school functioning and leisure time activities. Finally, the cross-sectional design of the present study does not allow for investigation of developmental pathways; questions related to the development of gender differences in depressive symptoms from childhood to adolescence await longitudinal studies.

Conclusions

The present study confirmed the essential unidimensionality of the SMFQ in a Norwegian population-based sample of adolescents attending highschool. Adolescent girls reported depressive symptoms more frequently and at a more severe level than adolescent boys. Even though some of the SMFQ items seem to function somewhat different for boys and girls, these differences did not lead to biased results.

Reports on some of the items, e.g., those related to restlessness, tiredness and concentration problems, seemed to be both part of and independent of depressive symptomatology. The high number of adolescents reporting problems reflecting depressive symptoms is alarming from a public health perspective. Although only a fraction of the adolescents with a high score on the SMFQ will show a severity and a functional impairment confirming a diagnosis of depression, it is reasonable to assume that most of them have problems related to everyday functioning. The findings in the present study thus call for further studies, both to investigate co-existing problems, and risk and resilience factors associated with developmental pathways of depressive symptoms.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The present study was supported by the Centre for Child and Adolescent Mental Health and Welfare, Uni health, Uni Research, Bergen, Norway, and was also funded by the University of Bergen, the Norwegian Directorate for Health and Social Affairs, and the Western Norway Regional Health Authority. We are grateful to the children, parents and teachers participating in the Bergen Child Study (BCS) and members of the BCS project group for making the study possible.

References

Abela, J. R. Z., and Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition from early to middle adolescence: a multiwave longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 120, 259–271. doi: 10.1037/a0022796

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Angold, A., Castello, E. J., and Messer, S. C. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 5, 237–249.

Angold, A., Costello, E., Pickles, E., and Winder, F. (1987). The Development of a Questionnaire for Use in Epidemiological Studies of Depression in Children and Adolescents. London: Medical research Council.

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., and Erkanli, A. (1999a). Comorbidity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 40, 57–87. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00424

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Erkanli, A., and Worthman, C. M. (1999b). Pubertal changes in hormone levels and depression in girls. Psychol. Med. 29, 1043–1053. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008946

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Silberg, J., Eaves, L., and Costello, E. J. (2002). Depression scale scores in 8-17-year-olds: effects of age and gender. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 43, 1052–1063. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00232

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ardal, G., Lund, A., and Hammar, A. (2012). Health-related quality of life in recurrent major depressive disorder-a 10-year follow-up study. Nord. J. Psychiatry. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2012.746730. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P., and Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. Br. J. Psychiatry 178, 200–206. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.3.200

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Bernet, W., Bukstein, O., Walter, H., Benson, R. S., et al. (2007). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with depressive disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46, 1503–1526. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318145ae1c

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Black, S. R., and Klein, D. N. (2012). Early menarcheal age and risk for later depressive symptomatology: the role of childhood depressive symptoms. J. Youth Adolesc. 41, 1142–1150. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9758-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Buitelaar, J. K. (2012). Adolescence as a turning point: for better and worse. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 21, 357–359. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0296-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd Edn.) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Conley, C. S., Rudolph, K. D., and Bryant, F. B. (2012). Explaining the longitudinal association between puberty and depression: sex differences in the mediating effects of peer stress. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 691–701. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Copeland, W., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., and Angold, A. (2009). Configurations of common childhood psychosocial risk factors. J. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 50, 451–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02005.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Costello, E. J., and Angold, A. (1988). Scales to assess child and adolescent depression: checklists, screens, and nets. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27, 726–737. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198811000-00011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Costello, E. J., Copeland, W., and Angold, A. (2011). Trends in psychopathology across the adolescent years: what changes when children become adolescents, and when adolescents become adults? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 1015–1025. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02446.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Danielsson, N. S., Harvey, A. G., Macdonald, S., Jansson-Frjmark, M., and Linton, S. J. (2013). Sleep disturbance and depressive symptoms in adolescence: the role of catastrophic worry. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 1223–1233. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9811-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daviss, W. B., Birmaher, B., Melhem, N. A., Axelson, D. A., Michaels, S. M., and Brent, D. A. (2006). Criterion validity of the mood and feelings questionnaire for depressive episodes in clinic and non-clinic subjects. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 927–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01646.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Derdikman-Eiron, R., Indredavik, M. S., Bakken, I. J., Bratberg, G. H., Hjemdal, O., and Colton, M. (2012). Gender differences in psychosocial functioning of adolescents with symptoms of anxiety and depression: longitudinal findings from the nord-tr£ndelag health study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 47, 1855–1863. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0492-y

CrossRef Full Text

Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Ridder, E. M., and Beautrais, A. L. (2005). Subthreshold depression in adolescence and mental health outcomes in adulthood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 66–72. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.1.66

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Flora, D. B., and Curran, P. J. (2004). An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol. Methods 9, 466–491. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.466

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fombonne, E., Wostear, G., Cooper, V., Harrington, R., and Rutter, M. (2001). The maudsley long-term follow-up of child and adolescent depression: 1. psychiatric outcomes in adulthood. Br. J. Psychiatry 179, 210–217. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.3.210

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freuchen, A., Kjelsberg, E., Lundervold, A. J., and Grholt, B. (2012). Differences between children and adolescents who commit suicide and their peers: a psychological autopsy of suicide victims compared to accident victims and a community sample. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Mental Health 6:1. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-6-1

CrossRef Full Text

Gillberg, C. (2010). The essence in child psychiatry: early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Res. Dev. Disabili. 31, 1543–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gradisar, M., Gardner, G., and Dohnt, H. (2011). Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: a review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep. Sleep Med. 12, 110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hammar, A., and Ardal, G. (2012). Effortful information processing in patients with major depression - a 10-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Res. 198, 420–423. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., and Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 107, 128–140. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.1.128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joinson, C., Heron, J., Araya, R., Paus, T., Croudace, T., Rubin, C., et al. (2012). Association between pubertal development and depressive symptoms in girls from a uk cohort. Psychol. Med. 42, 2579–2589. doi: 10.1017/S003329171200061X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joinson, C., Heron, J., Lewis, G., Croudace, T., and Araya, R. (2011). Timing of menarche and depressive symptoms in adolescent girls from a uk cohort. Br. J. Psychiatry 198, 17–23, sup 1–2. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080861

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of dsm-iv disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klein, D. N., Glenn, C. R., Kosty, D. B., Seeley, J. R., Rohde, P., and Lewinsohn, P. M. (2013). Predictors of first lifetime onset of major depressive disorder in young adulthood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 1–6. doi: 10.1037/a0029567

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Labelle, R., Breton, J.-J., Pouliot, L., Dufresne, M.-J., and Berthiaume, C. (2013). Cognitive correlates of serious suicidal ideation in a community sample of adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 145, 370–377. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.08.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meade, A. W., Johnson, E. C., and Braddy, P. W. (2008). Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 568–592. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.568

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merikangas, K. R., He, J.-P., Brody, D., Fisher, P. W., Bourdon, K., and Koretz, D. S. (2010). Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders among us children in the 2001-2004 nhanes. Pediatrics 125, 75–81. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2598

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Monshouwer, K., Smit, F., Ruiter, M., Ormel, H., Verhulst, F., Vollebergh, W., et al. (2012). Identifying target groups for the prevention of depression in early adolescence: the trails study. J. Affect. Disord. 138, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthen, L., and Muthen, B. (1998-2012). Mplus Users Guide (7th Edn). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roane, B. M., and Taylor, D. J. (2008). Adolescent insomnia as a risk factor for early adult depression and substance abuse. Sleep 31, 1351–1356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sharp, C., Goodyer, I. M., and Croudace, T. J. (2006). The short mood and feelings questionnaire (smfq): a unidimensional item response theory and categorical data factor analysis of self-report ratings from a community sample of 7-through 11-year-old children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 34, 379–391. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9027-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sivertsen, B., Salo, P., Mykletun, A., Hysing, M., Pallesen, S., Krokstad, S., et al. (2012). The bidirectional association between depression and insomnia: the HUNT study. Psychosom. Med. 74, 758–765. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182648619

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steiger, J., and Lind, J. (1980). “Statistically based tests for the number of common factors,” in Annual Meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.

Sund, A. M., Larsson, B., and Wichstrm, L. (2003). Psychosocial correlates of depressive symptoms among 12-14-year-old norwegian adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 44, 588–597. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00147

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sund, A. M., Larsson, B., and Wichstrm, L. (2011). Prevalence and characteristics of depressive disorders in early adolescents in central norway. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Mental Health 5:28. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-5-28

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teresi, J. A., Ocepek-Welikson, K., Kleinman, M., Eimicke, J. P., Crane, P. K., Jones, R. N., et al. (2009). Analysis of differential item functioning in the depression item bank from the patient reported outcome measurement information system (promis): an item response theory approach. Psychol. Sci. Q. 51, 148–180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tucker, L., and Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/BF02291170

CrossRef Full Text

van Beek, Y., Hessen, D. J., Hutteman, R., Verhulp, E. E., and van Leuven, M. (2012). Age and gender differences in depression across adolescence: real or 'bias'? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 973–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02553.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watkins, E., and Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion 5, 319–328. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yu, C. Y. (2002). Evaluating cutoff criteria of model fit indices for latent variable models with binary and continuous outcomes. Master's thesis, University of California, Los Angeles, CA.

Keywords: depression, adolescent psychology, sex-differences, short mood and feelings questionnaire, factor analysis, statistical

Citation: Lundervold AJ, Breivik K, Posserud M-B, Stormark KM and Hysing M (2013) Symptoms of depression as reported by Norwegian adolescents on the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 4:613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00613

Received: 19 March 2013; Accepted: 21 August 2013;
Published online: 11 September 2013.

Edited by:

Marit T. Schmid, University of Bergen, Norway

Reviewed by:

Sven Mueller, University of Ghent, Belgium
Anne M. Sund, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Norway

Copyright © 2013 Lundervold, Breivik, Posserud, Stormark and Hysing. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Astri J. Lundervold, Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Jonas Lies vei 91, 5009 Bergen, Norway e-mail: astri.lundervold@psych.uib.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.