Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Physiol., 12 June 2013
Sec. Integrative Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic How nature shaped echolocation in animals View all 21 articles

Evolution of the heteroharmonic strategy for target-range computation in the echolocation of Mormoopidae

  • 1Research Group of Bioacoustics and Neuroethology, Department of Animal and Human Biology, Faculty of Biology, Havana University, Havana, Cuba
  • 2Institut für Zellbiologie und Neurowissenschaft, J.W. Goethe Universität Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
  • 3Institut Biochemie und Biologie, Allgemeine Zoologie, Universität Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Echolocating bats use the time elapsed from biosonar pulse emission to the arrival of echo (defined as echo-delay) to assess target-distance. Target-distance is represented in the brain by delay-tuned neurons that are classified as either “heteroharmonic” or “homoharmormic.” Heteroharmonic neurons respond more strongly to pulse-echo pairs in which the timing of the pulse is given by the fundamental biosonar harmonic while the timing of echoes is provided by one (or several) of the higher order harmonics. On the other hand, homoharmonic neurons are tuned to the echo delay between similar harmonics in the emitted pulse and echo. It is generally accepted that heteroharmonic computations are advantageous over homoharmonic computations; i.e., heteroharmonic neurons receive information from call and echo in different frequency-bands which helps to avoid jamming between pulse and echo signals. Heteroharmonic neurons have been found in two species of the family Mormoopidae (Pteronotus parnellii and Pteronotus quadridens) and in Rhinolophus rouxi. Recently, it was proposed that heteroharmonic target-range computations are a primitive feature of the genus Pteronotus that was preserved in the evolution of the genus. Here, we review recent findings on the evolution of echolocation in Mormoopidae, and try to link those findings to the evolution of the heteroharmonic computation strategy (HtHCS). We stress the hypothesis that the ability to perform heteroharmonic computations evolved separately from the ability of using long constant-frequency echolocation calls, high duty cycle echolocation, and Doppler Shift Compensation. Also, we present the idea that heteroharmonic computations might have been of advantage for categorizing prey size, hunting eared insects, and living in large conspecific colonies. We make five testable predictions that might help future investigations to clarify the evolution of the heteroharmonic echolocation in Mormoopidae and other families.

Introduction

Echolocation allows bats to create perceptual images of complex night environments (Griffin, 1959; Moss and Surlykke, 2010). A key piece of information obtained during echolocation is the space-depth of surrounding objects that constitute possible targets (Simmons, 1973, 2012; Wenstrup and Suthers, 1984). Target distance is assessed from the time delay between the outgoing call and the returning echo (Simmons, 1971; Simmons et al., 1979). The central auditory system of echolocating bats contains specialized neurons that respond to particular call-echo delays (Feng et al., 1978; O'Neill and Suga, 1979). The combined activity of populations of delay-tuned neurons presumably determines the bat's ability for target-range computation (Suga, 1990; Simmons, 2012).

The neural processing of target-distance has been studied in six bat species from four different families: Mormoopidae (Pteronotus parnellii; O'Neill and Suga, 1979; Suga et al., 1979 and Pteronotus quadridens, Hechavarría et al., 2013); Rhinolophidae (Rhinolophus rouxi; Schuller et al., 1991); Vespertilionidae (Myotis lucifugus; Sullivan, 1982; Wong and Shannon, 1988 and Eptesicus fuscus; Feng et al., 1978; Dear et al., 1993); and Phyllostomidae (Carollia perspicillata; Hagemann et al., 2010, 2011). Two different neuronal strategies for target-range computation have been identified. In bats that broadcast frequency-modulated (FM) calls, delay-tuned neurons respond to similar harmonics in the calls and echoes, thus employing a homoharmonic computation strategy (HmHCS) (Feng et al., 1978; Sullivan, 1982; Dear et al., 1993; Hagemann et al., 2010). In two bat species from the family Mormoopidae (P. parnellii and P. quadridens) and one species from the family Rhinolophidae (R. rouxi) delay-tuned neurons are activated by the combination of the FM component of the fundamental harmonic in the call and one of the higher harmonic FM components in the echo (O'Neill and Suga, 1979; Suga et al., 1979; Schuller et al., 1991).

The “heteroharmonic computation strategy” (HtHCS) was first described in P. parnellii (Suga et al., 1978) and R. rouxi (Schuller et al., 1991). These two bat species use echolocation calls that combine long constant-frequency (CF) and FM components. For this reason it was long believed that HtHCS was an exclusive feature of the so called long CF-bats (Schuller et al., 1991; Wenstrup and Portfors, 2011). Recently, Hechavarría et al. (2013) reported that HtHCS is also a feature of neurons in the auditory cortex of the mormoopid P. quadridens, a species that uses short CF (sCF)-FM echolocation (Macías and Mora, 2003; Macías et al., 2006). That P. quadridens is able to use HtHCS echolocation is interesting not only from a functional point of view but also from an evolutionary angle, since (to our knowledge) Mormoopidae is the only family of bats including both CF-FM and sCF-FM species.

The evolution of echolocation has received much attention in the last decade. Recent molecular phylogenies (Eick et al., 2005; Teeling et al., 2005) have shaped new perspectives on the evolution of bat echolocation behavior (Jones and Teeling, 2006). Signal design (Jones and Holderied, 2007), duty cycle (Fenton et al., 2012), call frequency (Stoffberg et al., 2011), and Doppler shift compensation (Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011) have been reviewed in the light of new phylogenetic insights.

In this review, we explore the evolution of the HtHCS in bat species from the family Mormoopidae. There are several recent findings that motivated this work. (1) HtHCS was found in P. quadridens (Hechavarría et al., 2013). (2) The CF-bat P. parnellii holds a basal position in the lineage of the genus Pteronotus (Van den Bussche and Weyandt, 2003; Dávalos, 2006). (3) The auditory cortex of newborn bats that do not yet echolocate is equipped with a set of fully functional delay-tuned neurons (Kössl et al., 2012) which suggests that target-range computation strategies could be genetically pre-determined. (4) A scheme for the evolution of “Doppler shift compensation” by bats of the family Mormoopidae was proposed (Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008). (5) New call designs, activity patterns and diets were described in Caribbean mormoopids (Mora et al., 2011; Goerlitz et al., 2012; Mancina et al., 2012; Rolfe and Kurta, 2012).

We discuss how brain adaptations, distinctive characteristics of calls- and echoes- and phylogenetic relationships in mormoopids could have led to the acquisition of the heteroharmonic target-range computation strategy in this family. We argue that the HtHCS provides mormoopids with behavioral and ecological advantages for categorizing prey-size, hunting eared insects, and living in large colonies. By conducting the analysis in the light of recent molecular phylogenies, we are able to explore the evolutionary relationships between HtHCS and CF-specializations. We present the hypothesis that in Mormoopidae, HtHCS echolocation evolved independently from long-CF echolocation, high duty cycle (HDC) echolocation and Doppler Shift Compensation. We make five specific, testable predictions that might help future investigations to decipher the evolution of the heteroharmonic echolocation in Mormoopidae and other families.

Delay Tuning in Auditory Neurons of Different Bat Species

The most commonly used approach to determine whether a neuron is tuned to echo-delay or not consists in presenting the animal with artificial (or natural) pulse-echo pairs with different delays. The response of the neurons is measured as the number of spikes fired by the neuron in response to each echo-delay. If the echo-level is also changed during the recording, then the neuronal response is represented in the two dimensional space of echo-delay and echo-level in the form of a delay response area (DRA). Delay tuned neurons respond only (or maximally) to a few combinations of echo-delay and echo-level (see examples DRAs in Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Example delay response areas (DRAs) of two units from P. quadridens. The units were tuned to different harmonic combinations. Responses of the same unit were aligned horizontally. (B) The range of theoretical insect sizes generated from echoes of each biosonar harmonic. Insect size was calculated according to the maximum and minimum frequencies in each harmonic after Macías et al. (2006). It is suggested that each neuron could play a role in prey categorization according to size.

Different methods have been used to study the harmonic sensitivity of delay-tuned neurons in different bat species. By deleting components of the echolocation call and echo, Suga and co-workers (Suga et al., 1983) demonstrated that in the cortex of P. parnellii, the maximum response of delay-tuned neurons occurs when the fundamental FM-harmonic in the biosonar pulse (FM1) is followed by one of the upper FM-harmonics in the echo (i.e., FM2, FM3, or FM4) with a certain delay. Delay-tuned neurons are classified according to their best harmonic combination, i.e., the combination of pulse and echo harmonic that elicits the largest response. In P. parnellii, neurons tuned to combinations of FM1 and FM2, FM1-FM3, and FM1-FM4 have been found (Suga et al., 1983; Hagemann et al., 2011). Heteroharmonic neurons can be found in newborn P. parnellii long before they start to echolocate (Kössl et al., 2012). The latter could indicate that this neuronal ability is imprinted in the genome of the species and therefore it could have been subjected to evolutionary pressures.

In P. quadridens the frequency profile of cortical delay-tuned neurons was studied by presenting the bat with combinations of different harmonic components that included FM1/FM2, FM1/FM3, FM1/FM1, and FM2/FM2 (Hechavarría et al., 2013). The frequency profile of delay-tuned neurons in P. quadridens is quite similar to the frequency profile of delay-tuned neurons of P. parnellii. Example heteroharmonic neurons of P. quadridens are shown in Figure 1A. The delay-tuned neurons of P. quadridens fire only (or more strongly) in response to heteroharmonic pulse echo-pair combinations, i.e., FM1/FM2 and FM1/FM3. It has been suggested that neurons tuned to different harmonic combinations could provide information about targets with different acoustic properties i.e., preys of different sizes (Figure 1B).

Although P. parnellii and P. quadridens use comparable heteroharmonic computations, they differ in the cortical organization of neurons according to their best harmonic combination (Figure 2). In P. parnellii, delay-tuned neurons are clustered together forming three distinct cortical areas defined as the FM–FM, dorsal fringe and ventral fringe areas. Within the FM–FM and dorsal fringe areas, there is a “harmonic organization” of neurons, i.e., neurons with different best harmonic combinations occur in distinct cortical subdivisions (Suga and O'Neill, 1979). The most ventral subdivision is dominated by neurons tuned to FM1/FM2, the middle subdivision is dominated by neurons tuned to FM1/FM4 and the most dorsal subdivision is dominated by neurons tuned to FM1/FM3 (O'Neill and Suga, 1982) (Figure 2A). In the boundaries between subdivisions, there are “multiple-combination sensitive neurons” that respond maximally when the echo contains combinations of 2nd, 3rd, and 4th biosonar harmonics (Misawa and Suga, 2001). The cortex of P. quadridens is different from the cortex of P. parnellii in the sense that it is not “harmonically” organized (Hechavarría et al., 2013). In P. quadridens only the FM–FM area has been studied and within this area neurons tuned to FM1/FM3 are interspersed with neurons tuned to FM1/FM2 (Figure 2B). One organizational principle shared by the cortices of P. parnellii and P. quadridens is the “chronotopic” organization of neurons. In these two species, neurons tuned to short echo-delays are located rostrally, while neurons tuned to longer echo-delays are located more caudally (Suga and O'Neill, 1979; O'Neill and Suga, 1982; Schuller et al., 1991; Hagemann et al., 2011; Hechavarría et al., 2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Topographic organization of the FM/FM area of (A) P. parnellii and (B) P. quadridens. In each species, schematic representations of the brain are given. In the schematic brain representations, prominent landmarks and blood vessels are indicated. The yellow areas indicate cortical regions dominated by delay-tuned neurons. Note that in the dorsal auditory cortex of both species, close to the pseudocentral sulcus, there are large areas dedicated to the processing of call-echo delay. In each species, a detailed map of the FM/FM area is given. The data from P. parnellii is from one specimen [modified from Hagemann et al. (2011)]. The data from P. quadridens was pooled from 3 specimens [see Hechavarría et al. (2013) for methods for reconstruction of cortical maps]. In FM/FM area maps, numbers positioned at the coordinates of each neuron indicate characteristic delays. Numbers were color-coded to indicate the best harmonic combination of each neuron. Note that in P. parnellii neurons processing different harmonic combinations form different clusters in the cortical surface. However, in P. quadridens, neurons processing FM1/FM2 and FM1/FM3 are intermixed. In both species neurons processing shorter delays are located rostrally and those processing longer delays are located more caudally.

Phylogeny studies have shown that P. parnellii and P. quadridens stem from the most basal and most recent branches in the Pteronotus lineage, respectively (Van den Bussche and Weyandt, 2003; Dávalos, 2006). Because of the latter, and the fact both P. parnellii and P. quadridens possess comparable heteroharmonic neurons, Hechavarría et al. (2013) suggested that the HTCS could be a generalized feature of the genus Pteronotus that was preserved during the evolution. The same was suggested for the chronotopic organization of the cortex that is found in both species. On the other hand, harmonically organized chronotopic axes either evolved only in P. parnellii or were lost during the evolution of P. quadridens (Hechavarría et al., 2013).

Besides P. parnellii and P. quadridens, heteroharmonic neurons have been found in R. rouxi (Schuller et al., 1991). Only neurons tuned to FM1-FM2 were found in this species. Like in P. parnellii and P. quadridens, in R. rouxi there is a clear chronotopic organization of delay tuned neurons. The genus Rhinolophus is not closely phylogenetically related to the genus Pteronotus (Jones and Teeling, 2006). In fact rhinolophid bats seem to be more phylogenetically related to the megabats than to the remaining microbats (Teeling et al., 2005). The latter suggests that any specialization shared by Pteronotus and Rhinolophus could be the product of parallel evolution.

Delay-tuning has been studied in other three bat species besides the two Pteronotus and R. rouxi. In M. lucifugus, E. fuscus, and C. perspicillata delay tuning seems to be “homoharmonic,” i.e., delay-tuned neurons of these three species respond strongly to pulse-echo combinations of the same harmonic (Sullivan, 1982; Dear et al., 1993; Hagemann et al., 2010). M. lucifugus uses a simple FM-pulse for echolocation without prominent harmonics (Griffin, 1962) and therefore it is not surprising that this species uses homoharmonic computations. E. fuscus and C. perspicillata use biosonar calls that contain at least two harmonics (Thies et al., 1998; Monroy et al., 2011) although call structure can change drastically depending on the behavioral task and the reflective properties of the environment. Yet the delay-tuned of these two species respond strongly to homoharmonic pulse-echo pairs (Dear et al., 1993; Hagemann et al., 2010). Among the homoharmonic species studied so far, only C. perspicillata is reported to have a chronotopically organized representation of delay-tuned neurons (Hagemann et al., 2010).

Brain Adaptations for Heteroharmonic Computations

The mechanisms for the central implementation of delay tuning have been intensively investigated in P. parnellii and excellent reviews are available (Wenstrup and Portfors, 2011; Wenstrup et al., 2012). Heteroharmonic delay-tuning is implemented in the auditory midbrain (Wenstrup et al., 2012). Heteroharmonic neurons integrate information from the fundamental biosonar harmonic that provides information about the timing of the pulse and one or several of the upper harmonics in the echo (Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999). Delay-tuned neurons perform as coincidence detectors, i.e., they respond only when there is a temporal coincidence of subthreshold excitations triggered by call and echo. It has been demonstrated that inhibition plays an instrumental role in delaying the response to the call so that it can be aligned in time with the response to the echo. If call-triggered inhibition similarly plays an instrumental role in the implementation of homoharmonic delay tuning is still unknown.

Integrating information from multiple biosonar harmonics is generally accepted as a building block for the implementation of heteroharmonic delay tuning. However, integrating multiple frequency bands (otherwise known as combination sensitivity) is not an exclusive feature of heteroharmonic neurons tuned to echo-delay. For example, combination sensitive responses have been found in mice, birds, monkeys and homoharmonic bat species, among others (Margoliash and Fortune, 1992; Dear et al., 1993; Rauschecker et al., 1995; Hernández et al., 2005; Portfors and Felix, 2005; Felix and Portfors, 2007; Hagemann et al., 2010). The currently available data suggests that combination sensitivity is a generalized principle of the mammalian auditory system that was further used by heteroharmonic bats for the implementation of a specialized strategy for target-distance computation.

Call Design and Target Range

The examination of call design could provide a better understanding of the evolution of the HtHCS for target-range computation in bats and specifically in the family Mormoopidae. Bats use a highly diverse repertoire of call designs. Biosonar call diversity is observed both across (e.g., Schnitzler et al., 2003) and within species (e.g., Mora et al., 2011). One approach for categorizing bat calls distinguishes short FM from long CF calls. Typically, bats that broadcast pure-FM calls listen for echoes before emitting the next call to avoid temporal overlapping of call and echo. This calling strategy maintains a low duty cycle (LDC) (i.e., the proportion of time occupied by biosonar calls during an echolocation sequence is <25%). On the other hand, bats that use long CF echolocation calls separate call and echo in the frequency domain (because of the Doppler shifted echo). CF-bats are able to broadcast calls and receive echoes at the same time and therefore they can use HDC echolocation, with duty cycle values above 25%. Call design is tightly linked to duty cycle. Most echolocating bats use LDC echolocation (Fenton et al., 2012). HDC echolocation is a feature of only a few bats species (i.e., species from the families Rhinolophidae and Hipposideridae, and P. parnellii from Mormoopidae). Only the family Mormoopidae includes both LDC and HDC species. Although useful as a first approach, classifying bats into FM-LDC and CF-HDC according to their calling strategy is not fine-grained enough to explore the evolution of target-range computation in mormoopids.

Call design is polymorphic within the family Mormoopidae. FM calls are emitted by the two species of the genus Mormoops, long CF-FM calls are emitted by P. parnellii and sCF-FM and FM-sCF calls are emitted by the other five species of the genus Pteronotus: P. personatus, P. davyi, P. gymnonotus, P. macleayi, and P. quadridens (Fenton, 1994; O'Farrell and Miller, 1997; Ibañez et al., 1999, 2000; Kössl et al., 1999; Macías and Mora, 2003; Macías et al., 2006; Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008; Mora and Macías, 2011) (Figure 3A). For ranging, the following parameters of signal design are expected to be of special importance: (1) the number of harmonics, (2) the frequency overlap of harmonics, (3) the bandwidth of the FM component, (4) the duration and curvature of the FM component, and (5) the frequency range and intensity of each FM-component.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Spectrograms of typical search calls of the eight bat species of the family Mormoopidae (Ppar: Pteronotus parnellii, Mm, Mormoops megalophylla; Pg, Pteronotus gymnonotus; Mb, Mormoops blainvillei; Pd, Pteronotus davyi; Pm, Pteronotus macleayii; Pper, Pteronotus personatus; Pq, Pteronotus quadridens). The light-gray area represents the frequency range of best audition in eared moths, after Fullard (1988). (B) Typical echolocation sequence (oscillogram and spectrogram) emitted by P. parnellii (up) and P. quadridens (down) during foraging. Note that call harmonics never overlap. (C) Relationships between peak frequency (up) and call period (down) and forearm length for the eight species of mormoopid bats. Lower frequency calls are emitted by larger bats. Call period is independent of body size and signal design. Data was taken from: Silva-Taboada (1979), Herd (1983), Adams (1989), Rodríguez-Durán and Kunz (1992), Rezsutek and Cameron (1993), Lancaster and Kalko (1996), O'Farrell and Miller (1997), Ibañez et al. (1999), Ibañez et al. (2000), Macías et al. (2006), Smotherman and Guillen-Servent (2008), MacSwiney et al. (2008), de la Torre and Medellin (2010), and Mancina et al. (2012).

Obviously, a heteroharmonic mechanism for target-range computation can only operate on call-echo pairs with at least two harmonics. The two mormoopids in which the HtHCS has been reported [i.e., (P. parnellii and P. quadridens)] broadcast calls with three or more harmonics, as do the remaining Pteronotus and Mormoops species (Figure 3A). Also, the echolocation calls of R. rouxi (the third species in which HtHCS has been described) contain two harmonics (Neuweiler et al., 1987). E. fuscus and M. lucifugus (two homoharmonic species) use echolocation calls with one or two harmonics (Moss et al., 1997; Surlykke and Moss, 2000). Besides Mormoopidae, other families that use multiharmonic echolocation calls (i.e., three or more harmonics) are Megadermatidae, Nycteridae, and Phyllostomidae (Jones and Teeling, 2006; Jones and Holderied, 2007). It is known that at least one phyllostomid species (Carollia perspicillata) uses the HmHCS (Hagemann et al., 2010). Therefore, broadcasting multiple harmonics does not seem to be sufficient for using the heteroharmonic target-distance computations.

In the three species known to use HtHCS, there is no overlapping between the harmonics of the FM-component of the calls (Figures 3A,B). Therefore the ability of calling (and hearing) in spectrally independent bands could be a prerequisite for using HtHCS. Supporting this idea is the fact that C. perspicillata (which uses HmHCS) uses multiharmonic calls with harmonic overlapping (Thies et al., 1998). The echolocation calls from all mormoopid species show non-overlapping harmonics. Non overlapping harmonics are also observed in the biosonar calls of rhinolophids and hipposiderids.

To be able to keep harmonics fully separated in the frequency domain, bats need to limit the bandwidth of their FM calls or components. Increasing bandwidth is appropriate to develop a detailed acoustic snapshot of the surrounding and to separate prey from background clutter (Simmons and Stein, 1980; Siemers and Schnitzler, 2004). It is known that most bat species are capable of adjusting call bandwidth according to echolocation task. In Rhinopomatidae (e.g., Habersetzer, 1981), Vespertilionidae (e.g., Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993), Molossidae (e.g., Mora et al., 2011), Emballonuridae (e.g., Kalko, 1995) and Phyllostomidae (e.g., Mora and Macías, 2007) for example, several species can adjust the bandwidth of their calls to broadcast from quasi-constant frequency calls (BW < 4 kHz) to wideband FM calls (BW > 15 kHz) by adjusting the frequency band of their FM components. Mormoopids are different; they keep the bandwidth of their calls remarkably constant (Figure 3B) (but see Mormoops: Macías et al., 2006; Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008), thus avoiding harmonic overlap (Macías and Mora, 2003; Macías et al., 2006; Mora and Macías, 2011).

Not only the bandwidth of the FM-sweep but also its duration and curvature may affect the estimation of target-range and influence the performance of the computation process. Both from the “distance of focus” theory (Boonman et al., 2003; Holderied et al., 2006) and from behavioral (Simmons, 1973) and neurophysiological data (Jen and Wu, 2008), there is evidence showing that short calls and echoes are more appropriate for an accurate estimation of short target-distances, which might decrease collision risks and increase the probability of a successful capture. For a bat flying faster than 3.4 m/s (1% of the speed of sound), Doppler effects will lead to a distortion of the perceived range due to compression of echo delay time and elevation of echo frequency. However, the accuracy of short target-distance estimation increases if short hyperbolic FM calls are used, and also if strong harmonics are added (Boonman et al., 2003; Simmons et al., 2004). To the best of our knowledge mormoopid calls have not yet been used to investigate how signal design could affect the measurement of echo-delay at different flight speeds as it has been done in other species (Simmons, 1973; Altes, 1980; Boonman et al., 2003). Nonetheless the visual inspection of mormoopid calls suggests a call structure suited to minimize errors in measuring distance caused by Doppler Effect, mainly if the FM-component is taken into account (Figure 3B). The CF-component of variable length in the mormoopid calls will effectively widen the envelope of the cross-correlation function, causing Doppler tolerance to decrease (Simmons, 1973). However, in a filter bank model Doppler tolerance will not decrease dramatically by adding a CF-component to the wideband FM-component, since this affects only a portion of the receiver channels (Boonman et al., 2003). In other words, the CF-component, thought to be used in the estimation of relative velocity and the recognition of fluttering insects (Schuller, 1984; Suga, 1990; review: Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011), and the FM-component, used to measure target-distance (Simmons, 1973; Saillant et al., 1993), must be analyzed independently.

An additional ranging error is expected while flying since bats approach the target as the reflected echoes travel to the bat ears. Since this error causes an underestimation of target range while the Doppler-related error causes an overestimation of range, they cancel each other at a certain target distance (defined as the distance of focus; Boonman et al., 2003). By adjusting the design of the FM-sweep during flight in a range dependent way, bats can avoid these sources of error so that nearby objects are localized accurately, a behavior termed focusing (Boonman et al., 2003; Holderied et al., 2006). Future studies will show if mormoopids employ acoustic focusing and if they are able to adapt the duration, bandwidth, and curvature of FM biosonar elements to cancel out flight-speed-related ranging errors as a function of target-distance. If that is the case, delay-tuned neurons might show sharper delay tuning curves the shorter the “distance of focus” of the FM call-echo pairs used as acoustic stimuli in the neurophysiology experiment, improving the positive correlation between the best delay and the width of the delay tuning curves (see Figure 1) already observed in the auditory cortex (Suga and Horikawa, 1986; Hagemann et al., 2010; Hechavarría et al., 2013). Whether heteroharmonic bats correlate the returning echo with the actual outgoing call, or whether “hard-wired” replicas of the bat's characteristic signals are contained in the auditory system remains a very interesting question to be solved.

There is evidence that call design in mormoopids is linked to the HtHCS. Duty cycle, however, is not. In other words, call design may need to fulfill certain requirements for the bat to operate the HtHCS, but the HtHCS can operate both in LDC and HDC echolocation. For more than three decades (O'Neill and Suga, 1979) the heteroharmonic target-range computation was known only for two CF-HDC bats. However, the sCF-FM-LDC P. quadridens also computes call-echo delay heteroharmonically (Hechavarría et al., 2013). The relatively high proportion of the “on time” of the call is achieved in HDC rhinolophids and hipposiderids by increasing the duration of the call relative to the call period (i.e., the time between the onset of successive calls) (Fenton et al., 2012). However, call period in most insectivorous bat species studied to date, is determined by the species wingbeat period (Speakman and Racey, 1991). If the same applies to the CF-HDC mormoopid P. parnellii then call period would remain at values equivalent to those in the other species of the family, as it is shown in Figure 3C. What is of relevance for target-range computation is the time interval between the emissions of two consecutive FM-components. Therefore, it is not surprising that the general rules that govern the temporal parameters of the calling strategy in FM-HmHCS bats also govern those of CF-HtHCS bats if only the FM-components are taken into account, i.e., as bats get closer to targets, they shorten the call's duration (or that of the FM-component) and the interval between calls (or between FM-components) thus increasing ranging performance (Boonman et al., 2003) and the accuracy in the estimation of the target's angular position (Suga, 1990). In consequence, once CF-HDC bats detect, lock and start tracking fluttering insects, the CF-component will shorten principally to accommodate the temporal changes of the FM-component that will rule the distance-to-target dependent temporal adaptations of the bat calling behavior.

In conclusion, it seems likely that any bat making use of the HtHCS will broadcast FM calls (or calls with FM components) with two or more harmonics without frequency overlap. In addition, it is of advantage if the duration and curvature of the FM components are adjusted for acoustic focusing as a function of distance to target, in correlation with neuronal adaptations for the processing of call design as a complement of the target-range computation strategy. Also it could be predicted that bats that use HtHCS are capable of a precise control of call frequency and intensity. The latter will be explored in the following section.

Frequency and Intensity of Call and Echo

Echolocating bats dynamically change the acoustic parameters of calls (i.e., frequency, intensity, temporal parameters) to cope with their environment and perceptual task. A closer view at the common principles used by HtHCS bats to exploit frequency and intensity of calls- and echoes- may help to assess the evolution of their target-range computation strategy. This section focuses on the analysis of frequency and intensity because both parameters are closely related in the heteroharmonic target-range computation strategy. The frequency spectrum of each call is determined by the amount of energy or sound intensity distributed between harmonics, and frequency and intensity are the two main parameters used to characterize the receptive field of delay-tuned neurons (see section Delay Tuning in Auditory Neurons of Different Bat Species).

The most obvious difference between HtHCS and HmHCS bats is in the frequency content of interest for assessing the timing of calls and echoes. Species that compute target-range homoharmonically broadcast and listen in the same frequency band since relevant wavelengths in calls and echoes are the same (Simmons, 2012). In contrast, heteroharmonic bats always pay attention to the fundamental harmonic in the call but to the higher order harmonics in the echoes. All HtHCS bats focus energy in higher harmonics but assign very little (as little as 1% of the total energy) to the fundamental harmonic (Figure 3A). The bat will still hear the faint fundamental harmonic of its call due to the small distance between mouth and ear, and the relatively weak attenuation of low frequencies (Lawrence and Simmons, 1982). However, conspecifics will mainly hear the higher harmonics. Attenuating the fundamental harmonic in HtHCS bats could minimize call-echo interference in bat colonies with hundreds or thousands of individuals since FM-components of higher harmonics by themselves cannot excite FM–FM neurons (Suga, 1990). The high frequency FM-components of the echoes will only elicit auditory responses in delay-tuned neurons if the calling bat have previously emitted and listened to its own fundamental harmonic. In the Caribbean islands, mormoopid bats are dominant in cave ecosystems where they enjoy the advantages of living in large colonies (Silva-Taboada, 1979; Goerlitz et al., 2012; Lima and O'Keefe, 2013). However, it is worth mentioning that the largest bat colonies known to mankind are of presumed homoharmonic species i.e., Tadarida brasiliensis (Betke et al., 2008; Hristov et al., 2010). Future research is needed to unveil how HmHCS bats deal with target-range computation in environments with so much overlapping frequency interference.

Rather fixed frequency-limits of FM-components also distinguish HtHCS from HmHCS bats. Mormoopids, but also rhinolophids and hipposiderids, keep the maximum frequency of their FM-components at the value of their CF-components (Figures 3A,B). In mormoopids flying in open spaces, even the minimal frequency of each FM-component seems to be restricted by the addition of a lower sCF-component to the call (O'Farrell and Miller, 1997; Mora and Macías, 2011). In contrast, frequency limits of individual harmonics are less fixed in HmHCS bats that vary either the maximal and/or the minimal frequencies of the emitted calls to adjust bandwidth (Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993; Surlykke and Moss, 2000; Mora et al., 2005).

The most widely used hypothesis to explain dominant call frequencies in bats is the allometry hypothesis (Jones, 1996, 1999). Due to the physics of sound, the structures associated with sound production generate lower-frequency sounds as size increases (Pye, 1979), and therefore it is predicted that larger bats emit at lower frequencies. In Mormoopidae, call frequency scales negatively with body size (i.e., forearm length) (Figure 3C). Since the allometry hypothesis explains call frequencies in several other bat families including the presumed HtHCS Rhinolophidae and Hipposideridae (Heller and Helversen, 1989; Jones, 1999), this hypothesis is not of much value to explore the evolution of the heteroharmonic strategy.

The allotonic frequency hypothesis suggests that relatively high or low echolocation frequencies are the result of selection to become less audible to eared insects, especially moths (Fullard, 1987). Tympanate moths have maximum hearing-sensitivity between 20 and 50 kHz (Fullard, 1988) which coincides with the frequency range echolocation calls of most bat species (Fenton et al., 1998). The fundamental biosonar harmonic of each mormoopid species contains frequencies syntonic (i.e., between 20 and 50 kHz) with moth hearing, but due to its relatively low intensity it may be barely detectable by the prey, thus offering a good example of harmonic-dependent stealth echolocation (Goerlitz et al., 2010). If multiharmonic echolocation evolved in mormoopids to allow these bats to exploit the soft nutritious moths as a food resource, then it would ideally combine faint first syntonic harmonics with loud high-frequency allotonic harmonics. Such a call would be optimally designed to overcome prey hearing (Figure 3A). In fact, several studies have shown that moth constitutes a major prey item in the diet of many Caribbean mormoopids (Silva-Taboada, 1979; Rolfe and Kurta, 2012). Therefore, hearing-mediated detection of bats by moths could have operated as an important evolutionary force for the acquisition of the heteroharmonic target-range computation strategy in Neotropical mormoopids.

The frequencies used by HtHCS mormoopids may be also explained by the prey detection hypothesis (Houston et al., 2004) which relates the strength of an echo with the wavelength of the call and the dimensions of the prey. The non-overlapping harmonics of the call theoretically allow mormoopids to exploit a broad range of prey sizes. For example, it is generally accepted that insects generate relatively strong echoes from biosonar wavelengths that match the dimensions of their prominent scattering points (i.e., head and wings). If the latter is true, P. quadridens could target a variety of insects with size differences of about 3 mm according to echoes from the minimum frequencies of the second harmonic (61.22 kHz, wavelength 5.5 mm) and from the maximum frequencies of the third harmonic (124.00 kHz, wavelength 2.7 mm). Distinction of insect size will be favored by individual auditory neurons responding to either the echoes from the second or the third harmonics (see Figures 1A,B). Smaller preys could be detected by adding a fourth harmonic to the call, which will significantly increase strength of echoes generated in smaller insects (Houston et al., 2004). Frequency-dependent atmospheric attenuation, however, would be a serious limitation in the use of high-order harmonics, but negligible at short range where it has been found that mormoopid bats incorporate a third and even fourth harmonic to their vocalizations (Macías and Mora, 2003; Mora and Macías, 2011). We argue that bats using HtHCS get a bonus in the categorization of insect size by focusing acoustic energy in discrete harmonic bands which in addition safes energy.

Two other hypotheses have been used to explain the frequency composition of biosonar calls: the foraging habitat hypothesis (Jones and Barlow, 2004) and the acoustic communication hypothesis or acoustic resource partitioning hypothesis (Duellman and Pyles, 1983; Heller and Helversen, 1989). According to the foraging habitat hypothesis, bats species that forage in more-cluttered habitats should use calls of higher frequencies than species foraging in less cluttered/more-open habitats (Stoffberg et al., 2011). Due to the multiharmonic structure of mormoopid calls this hypothesis is of limited value for explaining the emission of high frequencies in relation to clutter; i.e., high frequency demands are solved in HtHCS species by adding more harmonics. However, it is important to note that P. parnellii (a species that forages in highly-cluttered environments), uses one of the lowest frequencies within the genus Pteronotus (Figure 3A). The acoustic communication hypothesis predicts that different frequencies could evolve under selection pressures imposed during social interactions (Heller and Helversen, 1989; Thabah et al., 2006). In our opinion, this hypothesis does not add new insights to the evolution of the target-range mechanism in Mormoopidae.

Doppler shift compensation is not linked to the HtHCS. DSC involves lowering the frequency of the next echolocation call to compensate for the flight-induced increase in the frequency of echoes from a previous emission (Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011). By compensating for Doppler effects, bats ensure that the CF-component of the echoes remains within the range of frequencies to which their auditory system is most sensitive, i.e., the “auditory fovea” (Neuweiler, 1990, 2003). The frequency value for which the auditory fovea shows its highest sensitivity is defined as the resting frequency (Suga and Jen, 1976; Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008). The CF-FM P. parnellii compensates for Doppler shifted echoes (Henson et al., 1980) but the sCF-FM P. quadridens does not (Mora and Macías, 2011). However, these two species measure target-distance using the HtHCS. It is tempting to predict that DSC will have an influence on the target-range computation of mormoopids. For example, in neurophysiological experiments with species showing DSC such as P. parnellii and P. personatus, the best responses of delay-tuned neurons might occur when the bat is presented with call-echo combinations in which the call's fundamental harmonic (FM1) is lowered in frequency and the echo's higher harmonics (FMx) is set at the species resting frequency. In contrast, the non-compensating smaller mormoopids should show best responses of delay-tuned neurons for combinations of the call's resting FM1 and the echoes' shifted FMx.

Changes in the amplitude of call and echo are also relevant for the target-range computation mechanism. During flight, both HmHCS and HtHCS echolocating bats decrease the intensity of their emitted pulses when approaching a prey item or an obstacle (Kobler et al., 1985; Boonman and Jones, 2002; Hiryu et al., 2007, 2008). Call intensity is adjusted in relation to the distance to target while maintaining echo intensity within an optimal sensitivity range. This intensity compensation will surely affect the shape of the response areas of delay-tuned neurons (DRAs, see section Delay Tuning in Auditory Neurons of Different Bat Species), that so far have been obtained by keeping constant the level of the call while changing the level of the echo (Suga, 1990; Hagemann et al., 2010; Kössl et al., 2012; Hechavarría et al., 2013). It is expected that in both HtHCS and HmHCS bats, lower call intensities will shift best call-echo delays to shorter values. Previous results from neurophysiology experiments in the HmHCS bat E. fuscus are in agreement with this prediction (Jen and Wu, 2008).

Intensity compensation has been mainly analyzed for whole calls and echoes, regardless of the species that is studied (Hiryu et al., 2007, 2008; Surlykke and Kalko, 2008). However, in bats using the HtHCS, the intensity compensation and its effect on target-range computation, need to be analyzed on a harmonic level. The changes of call/echo intensity in the fundamental harmonic may not be the same in the second or higher harmonics. Combining the acoustic/neuronal rules that seem to describe the HtHCS with those of intensity compensation, two main predictions arise: (1) that the intensity of the fundamental harmonic of the calls will remain stable while the intensity of the echo will increase as bats approach targets; and (2) that the intensity of the call's second or higher harmonics will decrease while the amplitude of the correspondent echoes will remain stable as bats approach targets.

Heteroharmonic Echolocation in the Phylogeny of Mormoopidae

To summarize the ideas discussed in the present review four important echolocation traits for mormoopid bats were mapped on a phylogenetic tree (Figure 4) (adapted from molecular data from Van den Bussche and Weyandt, 2003 and Dávalos, 2006). Mormoopid echolocation is characterized by quite diverse call designs and biosonar strategies that outside Mormoopidae distinguish different bat families. This diversity offers the opportunity to revise each species echolocation with the intention of tracking the ancestral condition and the evolutionary paths of each sonar trait. In some cases where there is not sufficient supporting data available, the two categories, “probably present” or “probably absent,” are used to be able to speculate on phylogenetic trends. Future research is needed to fill the gaps in knowledge and to evaluate the present speculations.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Echolocation traits of mormoopid bats mapped onto the molecular phylogenetic tree of the family, after Van den Bussche and Weyandt (2003) and Dávalos (2006). Schematics of echolocation calls from extant species (in red) and suggested common ancestors (in gray) have been represented. The heteroharmonic computation strategy is expected to be found in each species of the family, in contrast to the other three traits that characterize only some of the extant species. The evolutionary position of P. parnellii points to the long-CF calls and HDC as evolutionary singularities.

The first look to the echolocation in Mormoopidae (Figure 4) confirms non-overlapping harmonics in the calls of each species of Pteronotus. Non-overlapping harmonics have been identified in this review (see sections Brain Adaptations for Heteroharmonic Computations and Call Design and Target Range) as a promising feature that would support the HtHCS for target-range computation. In addition, both the most basal and one of the most recently evolved species of Pteronotus computes call-echo delay heteroharmonically (Suga, 1990; Hechavarría et al., 2013). As mentioned in the preceding text (section Introduction), it is likely that every other Pteronotus species will also use the HtHCS for ranging. In addition, we speculate that both Mormoops species will also compute target-range heteroharmonically. Not only are the echolocation calls of Mormoops of a multiharmonic structure but also they contain a prominent second harmonic and a faint first harmonic (O'Farrell and Miller, 1997; Macías et al., 2006), features that as dicussed here could be related to the HtHCS. Indirect support for a HtHCS comes from the observation that Mormoops lives in large colonies and is a specialized moth predator (Silva-Taboada, 1979; Goerlitz et al., 2012; Rolfe and Kurta, 2012), behaviors that might profit from using heteroharmonic computations (Suga, 1990). Arguing against the possibility of finding a heteroharmonic strategy in Mormoops is the fact that in this genus, the echolocation calls broadcasted while approaching a target show some degree of frequency overlapping (Macías et al., 2006; Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008). Frequency overlapping is not observed in the echolocation calls of Pteronotus species (Macías and Mora, 2003; Macías et al., 2006; Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008; Mora and Macías, 2011). However, if the HtHCS is finally demonstrated in Mormoops, it will support the theory that the common ancestor of Pteronotus and Mormoops already featured this echolocation trait.

The other three mormoopid echolocation traits, i.e., CF-FM calls, DSC and HDC are restricted to some mormoopid species (Figure 4). CF-FM calls (regardless of the duration of the CF component) are typical of Pteronotus and not of Mormoops, but using a long CF-component is a unique characteristic of P. parnellii. We therefore propose that the common ancestor of Pteronotus featured CF-FM calls. In this context, the long CF calls of the mustached bat, that allowed the species to echolocate at HDCs, are better explained as an evolutionary singularity probably produced by genetic change that introduced specialized modifications in cochlear development leading to an exceptionally sharp tuning to the CF call component [see discussion in Vater (1999); Kössl et al. (1999)]. DSC could have also characterized the Pteronotus ancestor since both P. parnellii and P. personatus compensate for flight-induced frequency shifts (Smotherman and Guillen-Servent, 2008; review: Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011). DSC in P. parnellii has been interpreted as instrumental to assure the processing of CF echoes carrying information about fluttering insects by an exceptionally sharply tuned auditory fovea (reviews: Neuweiler, 1990, 2003). There are no previous studies on the auditory system of P. personatus, but at least a disproportionate representation of neurons processing the resting sCF-component frequency and an enhanced sensitivity to this frequency range is to be expected. If it is assumed that the common ancestor of the genus Pteronotus already possessed DSC, it would be quite challenging to explain the loss of DSC in the most recent Pteronotus species. The most parsimonious hypothesis would be that the foraging strategy adopted by the smaller Pteronotus relies upon a more broadly tuned auditory system (Kössl et al., 1999) and like most FM bats, they can tolerate modest Doppler effects (Boonman et al., 2003). A detailed analysis of the possible evolutionary scenario for the acquisition of DSC is beyond the scope of this work, but excellent reviews on this echolocation attribute are available (Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011; Fenton et al., 2012).

Phyllostomidae is a sister family of Mormoopidae (Teeling et al., 2003; Eick et al., 2005). Because mormoopids are heteroharmonic (Hechavarría et al., 2013) and phyllostomids homoharmonic (Hagemann et al., 2010, 2011), it is difficult to infer the ranging strategy of the common ancestor of the two families. No indications for homoharmonic echolocation are apparent within the family Mormoopidea. Therefore if the ancestor of Mormoopidae was homoharmonic, this strategy was completely replaced by the heteroharmonic strategy during the evolution of the family. On the other hand, if the ancestor of Mormoopidae used HtHCS, some evidence could still be found within the many species of Phyllostomidae. Since C. perspicillata uses the HmHCS, and this species is relatively recent in the phylogeny of phyllostomids (Rojas et al., 2011), one should look into more ancient taxa to try to find any indication of HtHCS. Macrotus, the most basal genus of Phyllostomidae, could by the right taxon to find out whether the HtHCS was lost before the first phyllostomids appeared or during their evolutionary history. The two extant species of Macrotus (Macrotus californicus and Macrotus waterhousii) are gleaning bats that emit multiharmonic calls with faint fundamental harmonics, but showing frequency overlap (Murray et al., 2009).

Outside the New World, the same features characterizing the echolocation of Mormoopidae are found in species of the families Rhinolophidae and Hipposideridae. CF-FM, DSC and HDC in those bat families and in Mormoopidae are frequently taken as good examples of convergent evolution to emphasize how perceptual challenges imposed by the environment can override phylogenetic constraints (Jones and Teeling, 2006; Jones and Holderied, 2007). Rhinolophid bats make use of the HtHCS for ranging, long CF-FM calls, high duty-cycle and Doppler shift compensation (review: Schnitzler and Denzinger, 2011). Hipposiderids show similar echolocation traits but with shorter CF calls, lower duty cycles and a less advanced DSC. Their calls show the same signal structure suggested here to be necessary to perform the HtHCS.

Conclusions and Future Directions

This review presents the hypothesis that the HtHCS for target-range estimation assisted the ancestors of mormoopid bats in categorizing target size, hunting for eared prey and inhabiting caves in large numbers. We suggest that the implementation of the HtHCS evolved in parallel to the ability of using CF calls, HDC echolocation, and DSC behavior. The detailed analysis of echolocation signal design and its task-dependent adaptations in acoustic parameters, on top of recent gene-based phylogenies obtained for the species in the family Mormoopidae, allow the identification of common principles in the evolution of target-range computation in mormoopids and other heteroharmonic bats. The following predictions might help to define some of the evolutionary building blocks for this echolocation strategy.

(1) Each species of the genera Pteronotus and Mormoops is predicted to perform HtHCS. This is supported by the findings that within Mormoopidae, the most ancient and the most recent lineages show HtHCS and within Moormopidae call designs are similar across species.

(2) Mormoopids should be able of dynamic harmonic hopping, i.e., individuals can shift energy between the high order harmonics. In theory, the HtHCS supports harmonic hopping to minimize high interference (i.e., from conspecifics) or to aid in the discrimination of different target sizes.

(3) If the ability of HtHCS computation characterized the ancestors of Noctilionoidea it may have prevailed at least in descendent species with limited frequency overlap between harmonics. The genera Noctilio and Macrotus are appropriate candidates to test this hypothesis.

(4) Intensity compensation is harmonic-dependent in mormoopids and other bats with HtHCS. Calls and echoes represent different harmonic interests for the heteroharmonic echolocator and therefore the rules describing the dynamic adjustment of call/echo intensity will distinguish one harmonic from the other.

(5) If the echolocation calls of mormoopids evolved to hunt eared prey, they will be relatively inaudible to moths if compared to calls from HtHCS bats of comparable size.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Our research on the echolocation of mormoopid bats has been supported by the VW Foundation, the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation and the DFG. We thank Brock Fenton, Amanda Adams, and Holger R. Goerlitz for providing examples of mormoopid calls.

Abbreviations

HtHCS, heteroharmonic computation strategy; HmHCS, homoharmonic computation strategy; HDC, high duty-cycle; LDC, low duty-cycle; CF, constant frequency; FM, frequency modulation; DSC, Doppler shift compensation.

References

Adams, J. K. (1989). Pteronotus davyi. Mamm. Species 346, 1–5. doi: 10.2307/3504116

CrossRef Full Text

Altes, R. A. (1980). Detection, estimation, and classification with spectrograms. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 67, 1232–1246. doi: 10.1121/1.384165

CrossRef Full Text

Betke, M., Hirsh, D. E., Makris, N. C., McCracken, G. F., Procopio, M., Hristov, N. I., et al. (2008). Thermal imaging reveals significantly smaller Brazilian free-tailed bat colonies than previously estimated. J. Mammal. 89, 18–24. doi: 10.1644/07-MAMM-A-011.1

CrossRef Full Text

Boonman, A., and Jones, G. (2002). Intensity control during target approach in echolocating bats; stereotypical sensori-motor behaviour in Daubenton's bats, Myotis daubentonii. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 2865–2874.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Boonman, A. M., Parsons, S., and Jones, G. (2003). The influence of flight speed on the ranging performance of bats using frequency modulated echolocation pulses. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 617–628. doi: 10.1121/1.1528175

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dávalos, L. M. (2006). The geography of diversification in the mormoopids (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 88, 101–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00605.x

CrossRef Full Text

de la Torre, J. A., and Medellin, R. A. (2010). Pteronotus personatus (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae). Mamm. Species 42, 244–250. doi: 10.1644/869.1

CrossRef Full Text

Dear, S. P., Fritz, J., Haresign, T., Ferragamo, M., and Simmons, J. A. (1993). Tonotopic and functional organization in the auditory cortex of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 1988–2009.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Duellman, W. E., and Pyles, R. A. (1983). Acoustic resource partitioning in anuran communities. Copeia 1983, 639–649. doi: 10.2307/1444328

CrossRef Full Text

Eick, G. N., Jacobs, D. S., and Matthee, C. A. (2005). A nuclear DNA phylogenetic perspective on the evolution of echolocation and historical biogeography of extant bats (Chiroptera). Mol. Biol. Evol. 22, 1869–1886. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msi180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Felix, I. I. R. A., and Portfors, C. V. (2007). Excitatory, inhibitory and facilitatory frequency response areas in the inferior colliculus of hearing impaired mice. Hear. Res. 228, 212–229. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Feng, A. S., Simmons, J. A., and Kick, S. A. (1978). Echo detection and target-ranging neurons in the auditory system of the bat Eptesicus fuscus. Science 202, 645–648. doi: 10.1126/science.705350

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fenton, M. B. (1994). Assessing signal variability and reliability – to thine ownself be true. Anim. Behav. 47, 757–764. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1108

CrossRef Full Text

Fenton, M. B., Faure, P. A., and Ratcliffe, J. M. (2012). Evolution of high duty cycle echolocation in bats. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 2935–2944. doi: 10.1242/jeb.073171

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fenton, M. B., Portfors, C. V., Rautenbach, I. L., and Waterman, J. M. (1998). Compromises: sound frequencies used in echolocation by aerial-feeding bats. Can. J. Zool. 76, 1174–1182. doi: 10.1139/z98-043

CrossRef Full Text

Fullard, J. H. (1987). The defensive function of auditory enhancers in the neotropical moth Antaea lichyi (Lepidoptera, Notodontidae). Can. J. Zool. 65, 2042–2046. doi: 10.1139/z87-311

CrossRef Full Text

Fullard, J. H. (1988). The tuning of moth ears. Experientia 44, 423–428. doi: 10.1007/BF01940537

CrossRef Full Text

Goerlitz, H. R., Genzel, D., and Wiegrebe, L. (2012). Bats' avoidance of real and virtual objects: Implications for the sonar coding of object size. Behav. Processes 89, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.10.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goerlitz, H. R., Hofstede, H. M., Zeale, M. R. K., Jones, G., and Holderied, M. W. (2010). An aerial-hawking bat uses stealth echolocation to counter moth hearing. Curr. Biol. 20, 1568–1572. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Griffin, D. R. (1959). Echoes of Bats and Men / Donald R. Griffin. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books. doi: 10.5962/bhl.title.7011

CrossRef Full Text

Griffin, D. R. (1962). Comparative studies of the orientation sounds of bats. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 7, 61–72.

Habersetzer, J. (1981). Adaptive echolocation sounds in the bat Rhinopoma hardwickei– a field-study. J. Comp. Physiol. 144, 559–566. doi: 10.1007/BF01326841

CrossRef Full Text

Hagemann, C., Esser, K. H., and Kossl, M. (2010). Chronotopically organized target-distance map in the auditory cortex of the short-tailed fruit bat. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 322–333. doi: 10.1152/jn.00595.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hagemann, C., Vater, M., and Kössl, M. (2011). Comparison of properties of cortical echo delay-tuning in the short-tailed fruit bat and the mustached bat. J. Comp. Physiol. 197, 605–613. doi: 10.1007/s00359-010-0530-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hechavarría, J. C., Macias, S., Vater, M., Mora, E. C., and Kossl, M. (2013). Evolution of neuronal mechanisms for echolocation: specializations for target-range computation in bats of the genus Pteronotus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133, 570–578. doi: 10.1121/1.4768794

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heller, K. G., and Helversen, O. (1989). Resource partitioning of sonar frequency bands in rhinolophoid bats. Oecologia 80, 178–186. doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0679-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Henson, O. W., Henson, M. M., Kobler, J. B., and Pollak, G. D. (1980). “The constant frequency component of the biosonar signals of the bat, Pteronotus parnellii parnellii,” in Animal Sonar Systems, eds R. G. Busnel and J. F. Fish (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 913–916. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-7254-7_56

CrossRef Full Text

Herd, R. M. (1983). Pteronotus parnellii. Mamm. Species 209, 1–5. doi: 10.2307/3503910

CrossRef Full Text

Hernández, O., Espinosa, N., Pérez-González, D., and Malmierca, M. S. (2005). The inferior colliculus of the rat: a quantitative analysis of monaural frequency response areas. Neuroscience 132, 203–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hiryu, S., Hagino, T., Riquimaroux, H., and Watanabe, Y. (2007). Echo-intensity compensation in echolocating bats (Pipistrellus abramus) during flight measured by a telemetry microphone. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 1749–1757. doi: 10.1121/1.2431337

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hiryu, S., Shiori, Y., Hosokawa, T., Riquimaroux, H., and Watanabe, Y. (2008). On-board telemetry of emitted sounds from free-flying bats: compensation for velocity and distance stabilizes echo frequency and amplitude. J. Comp. Physiol. A 194, 841–851. doi: 10.1007/s00359-008-0355-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holderied, M. W., Jones, G., and von Helversen, O. (2006). Flight and echolocation behaviour of whiskered bats commuting along a hedgerow: range-dependent sonar signal design, Doppler tolerance and evidence for ‘acoustic focussing’. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 1816–1826. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Houston, R. D., Boonman, A. M., and Jones, G. (2004). “Do echolocation signal parameters restrict bats' choice of prey,” in Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins, ed J. A. Thomas, C. F. Moss, M. Vater (Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press), 339–345.

Hristov, N. I., Betke, M., Theriault, D. E. H., Bagchi, A., and Kunz, T. H. (2010). Seasonal variation in colony size of Brazilian free-tailed bats at Carlsbad Cavern based on thermal imaging. J. Mammal. 91, 183–192. doi: 10.1644/08-MAMM-A-391R.1

CrossRef Full Text

Ibañez, C., Guillén, A., Juste, J., and Perez-Jordá, J. L. (1999). Echolocation calls of Pteronotus davyi (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae) from Panama. J. Mammal. 80, 924–928. doi: 10.2307/1383261

CrossRef Full Text

Ibañez, C., Lopez-Wilchis, R., Juste, J., and León-Galván, M. A. (2000). Echolocation calls and a noteworthy record of Pteronotus gymnonotus (Chiroptera, Mormoopidae) from Tabasco, Mexico. Southwest. Nat. 45, 345–347. doi: 10.2307/3672841

CrossRef Full Text

Jen, P. H. S., and Wu, C. H. (2008). Echo duration selectivity of the bat varies with pulse-echo amplitude difference. Neuroreport 19, 373–377. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f52c61

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, G. (1996). Does echolocation constrain the evolution of body size in bats? Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 69, 111–128.

Jones, G. (1999). Scaling of echolocation call parameters in bats. J. Exp. Biol. 202, 3359–3367.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jones, G., and Barlow, K. E. (2004). “Cryptic species of echolocating bats,” in Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins, ed J. A. Thomas, C. F. Moss, and M. Vater (Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press), 345–349.

Jones, G., and Holderied, M. W. (2007). Bat echolocation calls: adaptation and convergent evolution. Proc. Biol. Sci. 274, 905–912. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0200

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, G., and Teeling, E. C. (2006). The evolution of echolocation in bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 149–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.01.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalko, E. K. V. (1995). Insect pursuit, prey capture and echolocation in Pipistrelle bats (Microchiroptera). Anim. Behav. 50, 861–880. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80090-5

CrossRef Full Text

Kalko, E. K. V., and Schnitzler, H. U. (1993). Plasticity in echolocation signals of European Pipistrelle bats in search flight – implications for habitat use and prey detection. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33, 415–428. doi: 10.1007/BF00170257

CrossRef Full Text

Kobler, J. B., Wilson, B. S., Henson, O. W., and Bishop, A. L. (1985). Echo intensity compensation by echolocating bats. Hear. Res. 20, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(85)90161-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kössl, M., Mayer, F., Frank, G., Faulstich, M., and Russell, I. J. (1999). Evolutionary adaptations of cochlear function in Jamaican mormoopid bats. J. Comp. Physiol. A 185, 217–228. doi: 10.1007/s003590050381

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kössl, M., Voss, C., Mora, E. C., Macias, S., Foeller, E., and Vater, M. (2012). Auditory cortex of newborn bats is prewired for echolocation. Nat. Comm. 3, 773. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1782

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lancaster, W. C., and Kalko, E. K. V. (1996). Mormoops blainvillii. Mamm. Species 544, 1–5.

Lima, S. L., and O'Keefe, J. M. (2013). Do predators influence the behaviour of bats? Biol. Rev. doi: 10.1111/brv.12021. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lawrence, B. D., and Simmons, J. A. (1982). Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 71, 585–590. doi: 10.1121/1.387529

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Macías, S., and Mora, E. C. (2003). Variation of echolocation calls of Pteronotus quadridens (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae) in Cuba. J. Mammal. 84, 1428–1436. doi: 10.1644/BWG-019

CrossRef Full Text

Macías, S., Mora, E. C., and García, A. (2006). Acoustic identification of mormoopid bats: a survey during the evening exodus. J. Mammal. 87, 324–330. doi: 10.1644/05-MAMM-A-124R1.1

CrossRef Full Text

MacSwiney, M. C., Clarke, F. M., and Racey, P. A. (2008). What you see is not what you get: the role of ultrasonic detectors in increasing inventory completeness in Neotropical bat assemblages. J. Appl. Ecol. 45, 1364–1371. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2664.2008.01531.x

CrossRef Full Text

Mancina, C. A., García-Rivera, L., and Miller, B. W. (2012). Wing morphology, echolocation, and resource partitioning in syntopic Cuban mormoopid bats. J. Mammal. 93, 1308–1317. doi: 10.1644/11-MAMM-A-331.1

CrossRef Full Text

Margoliash, D., and Fortune, E. S. (1992). Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch's HVc. J. Neurosci. 12, 4309–4326.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Misawa, H., and Suga, N. (2001). Multiple combination-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. Hear. Res. 151, 15–29. doi: 10.1016/S0300-2977(00)00079-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Monroy, J. A., Carter, M. E., Miller, K. E., and Covey, E. (2011). Development of echolocation and communication vocalizations in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol. 197, 459–467. doi: 10.1007/s00359-010-0614-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mora, E. C., Ibañez, C., Macías, S., Juste, J., López, I., and Torres, L. (2011). Plasticity in the echolocation inventory of Mormopterus minutus (Chiroptera: Molossidae). Acta Chiropterol. 13, 179–187. doi: 10.3161/150811011X578723

CrossRef Full Text

Mora, E. C., and Macías, S. (2007). Echolocation calls of Poey's flower bat (Phyllonycteris poeyi) unlike those of other phyllostomids. Naturwissenschaften 94, 380–383. doi: 10.1007/s00114-006-0198-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mora, E. C., and Macías, S. (2011). Short Cf-Fm and Fm-short CF calls in the echolocation behavior of Pteronotus macleayii (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae). Acta Chiropterol. 13, 457–463. doi: 10.3161/150811011X624947

CrossRef Full Text

Mora, E. C., Rodríguez, A., Macías, S., Quiñonez, I., and Mellado, M. (2005). The echolocation behaviour of Nycticeius cubanus (Chiroptera, Vespertilionidae): inter-and intra-individual plasticity in vocal signatures. Bioacoustics 15, 175–193. doi: 10.1080/09524622.2005.9753546

CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., Redish, D., Gounden, C., and Kunz, T. H. (1997). Ontogeny of vocal signals in the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus. Anim. Behav. 54, 131–141. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0410

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., and Surlykke, A. (2010). Probing the natural scene by echolocation in bats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:33. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murray, K. L., Fraser, E., Davy, C., Fleming, T. H., and Fenton, M. B. (2009). Characterization of the echolocation calls of bats from Exuma, Bahamas. Acta Chiropterol. 11, 415–424. doi: 10.3161/150811009X485639

CrossRef Full Text

Neuweiler, G. (1990). Auditory adaptations for prey capture in echolocating bats. Physiol. Rev. 70, 615–641.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Neuweiler, G. (2003). Evolutionary aspects of bat echolocation. J. Comp. Physiol. 189, 245–256. doi: 10.1007/s00359-003-0406-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neuweiler, G., Metzner, W., Heilmann, U., Rubsamen, R., Eckrich, M., and Costa, H. H. (1987). Foraging behavior and echolocation in the rufous horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus rouxi) of Sri-Lanka. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 20, 53–67. doi: 10.1007/BF00292166

CrossRef Full Text

O'Farrell, M. J., and Miller, B. W. (1997). A new examination of echolocation calls of some neotropical bats (Emballonuridae and Mormoopidae). J. Mammal. 78, 954–963. doi: 10.2307/1382955

CrossRef Full Text

O'Neill, W. E., and Suga, N. (1979). Target range-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science 203, 69–73. doi: 10.1126/science.758681

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Neill, W. E., and Suga, N. (1982). Encoding of target range and its representation in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurosci. 2, 17–31.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Portfors, C. V., and Wenstrup, J. J. (1999). Delay-tuned neurons in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat: implications for analyses of target distance. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 1326–1338.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Portfors, C. V., and Felix, I. I. R. A. (2005). Spectral integration in the inferior colliculus of the CBA/CaJ mouse. Neuroscience 136, 1159–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pye, J. D. (1979). Why ultrasound. Endeavour 3, 57–62. doi: 10.1016/0160-9327(79)90067-X

CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., Tian, B., and Hauser, M. (1995). Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science 268, 111–114. doi: 10.1126/science.7701330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rezsutek, M., and Cameron, G. N. (1993). Mormoops megalophylla. Mamm. Species 448, 1–5. doi: 10.2307/3504289

CrossRef Full Text

Rodríguez-Durán, A., and Kunz, T. H. (1992). Pteronotus quadridens. Mamm. Species 395, 1–4. doi: 10.2307/3504218

CrossRef Full Text

Rojas, D., Vale, A., Ferrero, V., and Navarro, L. (2011). When did plants become important to leaf-nosed bats? Diversification of feeding habits in the family Phyllostomidae. Mol. Ecol. 20, 2217–2228. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05082.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rolfe, A. K., and Kurta, A. (2012). Diet of mormoopid bats on the Caribbean Island of Puerto Rico. Acta Chiropterol. 14, 369–377. doi: 10.3161/150811012X661684

CrossRef Full Text

Saillant, P. A., Simmons, J. A., and Dear, S. P. (1993). A computational model of echo processing and acoustic imaging in frequency-modulated echolocating bats – the spectrogram correlation and transformation receiver. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94, 2691–2712. doi: 10.1121/1.407353

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H. U., and Denzinger, A. (2011). Auditory fovea and Doppler shift compensation: adaptations for flutter detection in echolocating bats using CF-FM signals. J. Comp. Physiol. A 197, 541–559. doi: 10.1007/s00359-010-0569-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H. U., Moss, C. F., and Denzinger, A. (2003). From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 386–394. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00185-X

CrossRef Full Text

Schuller, G. (1984). Natural ultrasonic echoes from wing beating insects are encoded by collicular neurons in the cf-fm bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. J. Comp. Physiol. 155, 121–128. doi: 10.1007/BF00610937

CrossRef Full Text

Schuller, G., O'Neill, W. E., and Radtke-Schuller, S. (1991). Facilitation and delay sensitivity of auditory cortex neurons in cf-fm bats, Rhinolophus rouxi and Pteronotus p.parnellii. Eur. J. Neurosci. 3, 1165–1181. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00051.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siemers, B. M., and Schnitzler, H. U. (2004). Echolocation signals reflect niche differentiation in five sympatric congeneric bat species. Nature 429, 657–661. doi: 10.1038/nature02547

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silva-Taboada, G. (1979). Los Murcielagos de Cuba. Havana: Editorial Academia.

Simmons, J. A. (1971). Echolocation in bats: signal processing of echoes for target range. Science 171, 925–928. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3974.925

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A. (1973). The resolution of target range by echolocating bats. J Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 157–173. doi: 10.1121/1.1913559

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A. (2012). Bats use a neuronally implemented computational acoustic model to form sonar images. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 311–319. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., and Stein, R. A. (1980). Acoustic imaging in bat sonar – echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation. J. Comp. Physiol. 135, 61–84. doi: 10.1007/BF00660182

CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Fenton, M. B., and O'Farrell, M. J. (1979). Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Science 203, 16–21. doi: 10.1126/science.758674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Neretti, N., Intrator, N., Altes, R. A., Ferragamo, M. J., and Sanderson, M. I. (2004). Delay accuracy in bat sonar is related to the reciprocal of normalized echo bandwidth, or Q. PNAS 101, 3638–3643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308279101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smotherman, M., and Guillen-Servent, A. (2008). Doppler-shift compensation behavior by Wagner's mustached bat, Pteronotus personatus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 4331–4339. doi: 10.1121/1.2912436

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Speakman, J. R., and Racey, P. A. (1991). No cost of echolocation for bats in flight. Nature 350, 421–423. doi: 10.1038/350421a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stoffberg, S., Jacobs, D. S., and Matthee, C. A. (2011). The divergence of echolocation frequency in horseshoe bats: moth hearing, body size or habitat? J. Mamm. Evol. 18, 117–129. doi: 10.1007/s10914-011-9158-x

CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N. (1990). Cortical computational maps for auditory imaging. Neural Netw. 3, 3–21. doi: 10.1016/0893-6080(90)90043-K

CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N., and Horikawa, J. (1986). Multiple time axes for representation of echo delays in the auditory-cortex of the moustached bat. J. Neurophysiol. 55, 776–805.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Suga, N., and Jen, P. H. S. (1976). Neural vs mechanical tuning curves in cf-fm bats, Pteronotus parnellii rubiginosus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 59, S18. doi: 10.1121/1.2002485

CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N., and O'Neill, W. E. (1979). Neural axis representing target range in the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science 206, 351–353. doi: 10.1126/science.482944

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N., O'Neill, W. E., and Manabe, T. (1978). Cortical-neurons sensitive to combinations of information-bearing elements of biosonar signals in mustache bat. Science 200, 778–781. doi: 10.1126/science.644320

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N., O'Neill, W. E., and Manabe, T. (1979). Harmonic-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science 203, 270–274. doi: 10.1126/science.760193

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N., O'Neill, W. E., Kujirai, K., and Manabe, T. (1983). Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 1573–1626.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sullivan, W. E. (1982). Neural representation of target distance in auditory cortex of the echolocating bat Myotis lucifugus. J. Neurophysiol. 48, 1011–1032.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Surlykke, A., and Kalko, E. K. V. (2008). Echolocating bats cry out loud to detect their prey. PLoS ONE 3:e2036. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A., and Moss, C. F. (2000). Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and the laboratory. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 2419–2429. doi: 10.1121/1.1315295

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teeling, E. C., Madsenb, O., Murphyc, W. J., Springerd, M. S., and O'Brien, S. J. (2003). Nuclear gene sequences confirm an ancient link between New Zealand's short-tailed bat and South American noctilionoid bats. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 28, 308–319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Teeling, E. C., Springer, M. S., Madsen, O., Bates, P., O'brien, S. J., and Murphy, W. J. (2005). A molecular phylogeny for bats illuminates biogeography and the fossil record. Science 307, 580–584. doi: 10.1126/science.1105113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thabah, A., Rossiter, S. J., Kingston, T., Zhang, S., Parsons, S., Mya Mya, K., et al. (2006). Genetic divergence and echolocation call frequency in cryptic species of Hipposideros larvatus s.l. (Chiroptera: Hipposideridae) from the Indo-Malayan region. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 88, 119–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00602.x

CrossRef Full Text

Thies, W., Kalko, E. K. V., and Schnitzler, H. U. (1998). The roles of echolocation and olfaction in two Neotropical fruit-eating bats, Carollia perspicillata and C. castanea, feeding on Piper. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42, 397–409. doi: 10.1007/s002650050454

CrossRef Full Text

Van den Bussche, R. A., and Weyandt, S. E. (2003). Mitochondrial and nuclear DNA sequence data provide resolution to sister-group relationships within Pteronotus (Chiroptera: Mormoopidae). Acta Chiropterol. 5, 1–13. doi: 10.3161/001.005.0101

CrossRef Full Text

Vater, M. (1999). “Evolutionary plasticity and ontogeny in the bat cochlea,” in Ontogeny, Functional Ecology and Evolution of Bats, eds R. A. Adams and S. C. Pedersen (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 137–174.

Wenstrup, J. J., Nataraj, K., and Sanchez, J. T. (2012). Mechanisms of spectral and temporal integration in the mustached bat inferior colliculus. Front. Neural Circuits 6:75. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00075.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wenstrup, J. J., and Portfors, C. V. (2011). Neural processing of target distance by echolocating bats: functional roles of the auditory midbrain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 2073–2083. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wenstrup, J., and Suthers, R. (1984). Echolocation of moving targets by the fish-catching bat, Noctilio leporinus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 155, 75–89. doi: 10.1007/BF00610933

CrossRef Full Text

Wong, D., and Shannon, S. L. (1988). Functional zones in the auditory cortex of the echolocating bat, Myotis lucifugus. Brain Res. 453, 349–352. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90176-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: target-range, echolocation, heteroharmonic computation, Mormoopidae, call-echo delay

Citation: Mora EC, Macías S, Hechavarría J, Vater M and Kössl M (2013) Evolution of the heteroharmonic strategy for target-range computation in the echolocation of Mormoopidae. Front. Physiol. 4:141. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00141

Received: 08 February 2013; Accepted: 25 May 2013;
Published online: 12 June 2013.

Edited by:

Cynthia F. Moss, University of Maryland, USA

Reviewed by:

Noritaka Ichinohe, National Institute of Neuroscience, Japan
Zoltan M. Fuzessery, University of Wyoming, USA

Copyright © 2013 Mora, Macías, Hechavarría, Vater and Kössl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Emanuel C. Mora, Research Group of Bioacoustics and Neuroethology, Department of Animal and Human Biology, Faculty of Biology, Havana University, 25 St. No. 455, Vedado, Havana, 10400, Cuba e-mail: emanuel@fbio.uh.cu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.