Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 18 June 2024
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Addictions and Eating Behavior View all 4 articles

Emotional eating, internet overuse, and alcohol intake among college students: a pilot study with virtual reality

  • Faculty of Education and Psychology, Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (UFV), Madrid, Spain

Introduction: The term emotional eating (EE) describes the tendency to eat as an automatic response to negative emotions and has been linked to anxiety and depression, common symptoms among the university population. The EE tendencies have also been associated with excessive internet use and an increase in alcohol intake among young university students.

Methods: The aim of this study is to examine the relationship between the tendency towards EE and other health-compromising behaviors, such as excessive internet use or high alcohol intake. Additionally, it aims to investigate the association of these risky behaviors with the participants’ performance level in a virtual reality (VR) task that assesses their executive functioning, and to assess impulsivity and levels of anxiety and depression.

Results: The results associate EE with excessive internet (r = 0.332; p < 0.01). use but not with alcohol consumption. Alcohol consumption was not associated with anxiety, depression, or impulsivity, but it was related to altered executive functions in the VR task: flexibility and working memory explained 24.5% of the variance. By contrast, EE and internet overuse were not related to executive function but were associated with impulsivity, depression, and anxiety. Impulsivity and depressive symptoms accounted for 45% of the variance in EE. Depression, trait anxiety and impulsivity explained 40.6% of the variance in internet overuse.

Discussion: The results reveal distinct patterns of psychological and neuropsychological alterations associated with alcohol consumption compared to emotional eating (EE) and excessive internet use. These findings underscore significant differences in the contributing factors between addictions and other substance-free addictive behaviors. For a deeper understanding of the various contributing factors to EE in college students, further research is recommended.

1 Introduction

Eating behavior is a multifaceted phenomenon influenced not only by biological or genetic factors, but also by environmental and psychological factors (1). For more than 30 years, studies have consistently highlighted the significance of self-regulation and emotion in shaping the emergence of unhealthy eating habits (2, 3). Stress and negative emotions are frequently linked to increased food intake (4). One of the most dysfunctional eating habits is emotional eating. The term emotional eating (EE) describes the propensity to eat as an instinctual reaction to unpleasant feelings (5) and implies a disproportionate intake of food, regardless of whether or not the individual is actually hungry, but in a nonpathological way. It differs from binge eating disorder (BED), which was officially recognized in 2013 in the DSM-5 as a separate eating disorder characterized by recurrent episodes of binge eating accompanied by a feeling of loss of control over eating, along with marked distress regarding binge eating, but without the compensatory mechanisms observed in anorexia or bulimia nervosa such as self-induced vomiting or excessive laxative use (6).

EE has been linked to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general (non-clinical) population (7, 8). Episodes of emotional eating (EE) are typically accompanied by high-calorie, sugary, and/or high-fat food consumption. This eating pattern has been described as one behavioral mechanism linking depression to the development of obesity and abdominal fat (9). Individuals who are obese or overweight frequently experience this eating behavior pattern (10). Emotional eating was also strongly associated with psychological distress (11) and feelings of guilt or shame (12, 13).

University students exhibit elevated anxiety levels compared to the broader population with concerns about academic performance being a primary contributor to this distress (14, 15), and they also grapple with issues such as gaining autonomy from parents, financial constraints, and social competition or isolation, among various other concerns (16). Stress, anxiety, and depression emerge as prevalent psychological challenges faced during university education globally. Studies consistently indicate that university students tend to experience lower mental well-being compared to the general population (17, 18). When they fail to adapt adequately, they may exhibit maladaptive coping behaviors. Recent studies indicate a broad spectrum of risky behaviors among college students, such as heavy alcohol consumption and unhealthy eating patterns (19, 20). A considerable level of EE (29%) has been also found among the university population (8). Lack of effective stress management strategies can lead to resorting to unsuitable or harmful coping mechanisms like increased alcohol intake or EE. In a recent study, the frequency, and patterns of EE over the past 28 days were assessed among 335 female university students. The findings revealed that 51.3% of these episodes were associated with emotional states, notably anxiety (21), and it has been suggested that the intake of alcohol might serve as a substitute coping mechanism for managing negative emotions, particularly anxiety, replacing EE. A study employing person-centered longitudinal analysis over three years found that undergraduates tend to resort to excessive drinking as a means of relieving stress and managing social anxiety (22).

The existence of eating disorders, such as anorexia, bulimia, and binge-eating disorder, has also been associated with problematic internet use among university populations (23), suggesting that the duration of students’ internet usage influences eating behavior disorders (24). At a subclinical level, problematic internet and smartphone abuse has been linked to problems with self-control over eating and a greater propensity to consume automatically in reaction to negative emotions or sentiments in teenagers and young adults, as shown by a study involving 209 adolescents as part of a school-based mental health promotion program (25), consumption pattern previously described and known as EE (26). Since both EE and excessive internet use are probable products of a lack of emotional regulation, there is undoubtedly a connection between the two (27). Additionally, a recent study conducted with 551 adolescents aged 15–17 suggests that social pressure and anxiety may act as mediating factors in the association between EE and internet addiction (IA), particularly social media addiction via smartphones (28).

College students experience a phase of transition into adulthood where they have attained physical maturity but might not have achieved similar levels of psycho-social development (29). The onset of the college experience often brings about substantial lifestyle shifts, notably in dietary habits characterized by skipping meals, insufficient nutrition, and frequent consumption of fast food. Moreover, during this stage, the brain is highly susceptible to external influences, particularly in the final phases of neuromaturation, which represent a critical period for neurodevelopment (30). In associative areas, especially the prefrontal cortex, there is a notable increase in white matter between the ages of 18 and 22, which correspond to the first years of university (31). Additionally, throughout the first half of the first year of university, there have been changes found in the volume of grey matter (32). These changes have been linked to the executive function’s end of maturation, which occurs between the end of adolescence and the beginning of adulthood (33). The prolonged developmental phase explains the specific vulnerability of executive functions compared to other cognitive processes influenced by external factors, risky behaviors, or addictions. Executive functions (EF) are higher-level cognitive mechanisms that regulate lower-level cognitive processes and also impact behavioral control (34), and have an important role in the development of emotional regulation strategies: a heightened level of executive functions is somewhat connected to the effective implementation of emotion regulation strategies (35), and deficiencies in executive functions (working memory, planning, flexibility) are widespread among adolescents experiencing challenges in emotional regulation (36). Various studies have found alterations in the executive functions of university students associated with alcohol consumption (20, 30), abusive internet use (37), and EE (38). Regarding this, a systematic review demonstrates that proper functioning of executive functions is associated with adequate and correct self-regulation of eating behavior (2), and a longitudinal study conducted with 169 teens with type 1 diabetes shows that high levels of disordered eating practices in teenagers are associated with difficulties in EF (39).

In the evaluation of EF, discrepancies sometimes emerge between the scores obtained in traditional pen-and-paper tests and the performance of tasks in daily life. The utilization of virtual reality (VR) technology increasingly influences the process of neuropsychological assessment. By facilitating dynamic interaction and replicating three-dimensional environments, VR provides participants with an immersive experience that closely simulates real-life settings (40). VR also allows for the reproduction of multitasking environments, necessary to increase the ecological validity and predictive value of the functional performance of the individuals being evaluated (41).

Another relevant factor to consider in the increasing prevalence of these unhealthy and risky abusive behaviors among college students is impulsivity. Impulsivity results from a lack of inhibitory control, and it is defined as the tendency to act swiftly in response to any stimulus, whether internal or external, without prior evaluation of all available information and regardless of the potential consequences of the action. Numerous studies corroborate the pivotal role impulsivity plays in substance addictions (42), alcohol consumption (43), and it also appears to be a relevant factor in internet addiction (37, 44). Impulsivity has been directly linked to overeating (45), food addiction (46), and BED (47), serving as a mediating factor between EE and overweight (48).

The objectives of this study include:

1. To assess the levels of EE in a sample of college students and their relationship with other abusive behaviors, such as excessive internet usage and alcohol consumption.

2. To verify the relationship between these behaviors, impulsivity and executive functioning assessed through a virtual reality (VR) task.

3. To evaluate the distinct roles that depression and anxiety may play in EE, excessive internet usage and alcohol consumption.

2 Method

2.1 Participants

The sample consisted of 56 undergraduate students (age > 18), of whom 30.4% were men and 69.6% were women. The age range was between 18 and 26 years. The mean age of participants was 20.62 (SD = 2.4). All the participants were university students from Francisco de Vitoria University in Madrid, Spain. The students were selected through non-probabilistic and accidental sampling. Table 1 shows more details about sample characteristics. Inclusion criteria were not having any psychiatric diagnosis, not being receiving psychological treatment at the time of the study and not consuming psychoactive substances. The study protocol and design were approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Francisco de Vitoria, and it fully complied with the Helsinki Declaration.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of the sample.

2.2 Variables and instruments

Sociodemographic information. Participants provided information about their age, gender, and educational level through a questionnaire created ad hoc.

Emotional eating was measured using the Emotional Eater Questionnaire (EEQ) (49). The EEQ consists of 10 items on a 4-category Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3. The test provides a global score to distinguish between non-emotional eaters (score between 0–5), slightly emotional eaters (score between 6–10), emotional eaters (score between 11–20), and highly emotional eater (score between 21–30). The temporal stability shows a medium to high correlation in the test–retest average (r = 0.702; p < 0.001), and the internal consistency of the subscales ranges from α = 0.61 to α = 0.77. In our study, the internal consistency was α = 0.86.

Internet addiction was assessed using the Internet Addiction Test (IAT). This instrument was originally created in 1998 (50). For our study we used the Spanish version validated by Puerta-Cortés et al. (51). The IAT consists of 20 items with a Likert-type response format with 5 levels. The cutting point for identifying internet overuse is 40. The diagnostic validity showed 81% sensibility and 82.6% of specificity. Internal consistence values obtained in the validation study was good (α = 0.82). In the present study, internal consistence was also good (α =0.88).

Alcohol consumption was measured using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (52). This questionnaire was composed of 10 items with a five-point (0–4) Likert scale as a response format, except for the last two items, which have three answer options (0–2). Problematic alcohol consumption is considered from 8 points. AUDIT showed high sensibility and specificity (0.90 and 0.80 respectively). The validation of the test on a Spanish university population (53) found the internal consistency of the AUDIT test to be α = 0.75. In the present study, the internal consistency for the AUDIT was also good (α = 0.83).

Depression was measured with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), revised and updated in 1996 (54) and validated in a Spanish sample (55). The BDI consists of 21 items on a 4-category Likert scale (0–3). The test score allows for classification into minimal depression (0–13), mild depression (14–19), moderate depression (20–28) and severe depression (29–63). The internal consistence of the BDI in previous studies was α = 0.83. In the present study, the internal consistence was good (α = 0.90).

Anxiety was assessed by using the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), adapted to the Spanish population (56). This questionnaire consists of a 40-items scale divided into two parts: the first 20 items measure anxiety as state; and the las 20 items measure anxiety as trait. Each of the 20 items in each subsection uses a 4-category Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3. A reliability study showed high internal consistency (α = 0.93). In the present study, the internal consistency was also good (α = 0.81).”

Impulsivity was measured using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, a questionnaire that was originally created in 1987 and adapted to the Spanish population in 2001 (57). The scale consists of 30 items with 4 response options, from 1 (always) to 4 (never). Reliability analysis showed good values of internal consistency (α =0 0.81). In our study the internal consistency was adequate (α = 0.70).

Processing speed, planification, working memory and flexibility were measured with the Ice Cream Test (ICT), created by Giunti Psycometrics®, within Nesplora® package through virtual reality. This test has shown validity and reliability to measure executive functions (40). The task allows to measure attention, working memory, planification, flexibility and processing speed at the same time (58).

The participants were immersed in a VR experience where they had to assume the role of an ice cream vendor, following the instructions given at the beginning of the task. All task instructions are delivered audibly: “you’ll be working at the ice cream shop for a while. Customers come in groups of four, and you must serve them according to your boss’s instructions. Call your boss, and he’ll provide you with his priorities for serving customers. Click on the phone to give him a call.” The boss’s instructions outlined the order of attending to customers based on various criteria and the preparation of ice creams, which differed for each customer. Participants were provided with a recipe book, which they could consult whenever they deemed it necessary.

The test consists of 20 sequences divided into two rounds, A and B. Prior to the start of the task, all participants underwent a series of practice trials to ensure that the instructions were properly understood and that the task was manageable for each subject. In both parts A and B, the test operates with the same structure, environment, and task. Midway through the test, the initially learned series of ice creams changes, and a new set of ice cream variants must be learned to perform correctly in the second half of the test. The task requires to attend to customer requests and orders, utilize working memory to remember orders and manage inventory, plan, and organize tasks efficiently to serve customers. In round 2, participants must adapt to changing customer demands, demonstrating flexibility, and process orders quickly to serve customers effectively, thus measuring processing speed. In this way, upon finishing the test, four indices can be obtained for each participant: planning (correct assignments, assignment time, cognitive load), working memory (correct services without consultation), flexibility (interference between instructions, switching) and processing speed (duration of task completion).

The immersive and interactive nature of this VR experience can enhance the engagement of the assessment and potentially offer a more accurate representation of an individual’s executive functioning abilities in a real-world context.

2.3 Procedure

The study was conducted in two steps: first, using the Qualtrics platform,1 where participants completed the questionnaires. The second step took place in person, individually, in a calm and quiet environment. In the second step, participants completed the VR task (ICT). Participation was voluntary and required prior informed consent. Participants did not receive any rewards. This study received approval from the University Francisco de Vitoria Ethics Committee and was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.4 Data analysis

Firstly, we obtained descriptive statistics for all the measures. To analyze the association between psychological and neuropsychological variables and emotional eating, internet overuse, and alcohol consumption, we calculated Pearson correlation coefficients. Finally, we conducted a series of stepwise linear regression models, introducing the significantly associated variables along with age and gender as explanatory variables. The data obtained in the study was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences 25.0 (SPSS) for Windows.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive analysis and linear correlation among the studied variables

Table 2 displays the descriptive analysis of each measure. Scores exhibit moderate variability based on the range values, typical of a subclinical sample.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive analysis of participants’ values for evaluated variables.

The Pearson correlation coefficients between each of the measures are presented in Table 3. Considering the main study variables, EE and the propensity for internet addiction are related. Trait anxiety and depression, along with increased impulsivity are associated with EE. These same variables and additionally the processing speed (measure round 2) were positively associated to the internet overuse score. Concerning alcohol consumption, processing speed (measured in round 2) also exhibited a positive association with AUDIT scores, albeit in a different context; meanwhile, working memory and flexibility demonstrated an inverse correlation with this variable. However, alcohol intake is not linked to higher levels of anxiety and depression.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation analysis: Pearson correlation coefficient between the measures.

3.2 Explanatory models of emotional eating, internet overuse, and alcohol consumption

Considering the psychological and neuropsychological variables associated with the main study variables, a series of stepwise regressions were conducted. In these regressions EE, internet overuse and alcohol consumption were introduced as dependent variables and the previous variables with significant correlations were introduced as independent variables. In the same way, gender and age were also included as independent variables in the explanatory models. Table 4 shows the explanatory model of emotional eating that was obtained.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regression analysis: explanatory model of emotional eating.

For EE, depression and impulsivity resulted as explanatory variables. The combined model, comprising both variables, accounted for 45% of the variance in EE (F = 22.293; p > 0.001). Trait anxiety, gender, and age did not emerge as significant explanatory variables. For this model, the assumptions of linear regression were met. The Durbin-Watson statistic for residual independence was 2.047, and the collinearity criteria displayed appropriate values: tolerance level > 0.1 (1.00) and FIV < 10 (1.004).

Regarding internet overuse scores, Table 5 shows the explanatory model obtained. Levels of depression, trait anxiety, and impulsivity were significant explanatory variables. The complete model explained 40.6% of the variance in internet overuse (F = 11.412; p > 0.001). However, processing speed (round 2), gender, and age did not result as significant explanatory variables of internet overuse. Conditions for linear regression were also met. The Durbin-Watson test value was 1.711, and values for no collinearity were appropriate: tolerance level > 0.1 (0.85–0.932) and FIV < 10 (1.00–1.176).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Regression analysis: explanatory model of internet overuse.

Lastly, Table 6 shows results of explanatory model of alcohol consumption. We identified working memory and flexibility as significant explanatory variables. However, processing speed (round 2), gender, and age were not significant. The complete model explained 24.5% of the variance in alcohol consumption (F = 8.428; p = 0.001). Similarly, the requirements for linear regressions were satisfactorily met. The Durbin-Watson test value was 2.057, and for no collinearity, we obtained tolerance levels >0.1 (0.978–1.000) and FIV < 10 (1.000–1.023).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Regression analysis: explanatory model of alcohol consumption.

4 Discussion

Our findings uncover specific patterns of psychological and neuropsychological changes linked to EE and excessive internet use when contrasted with alcohol consumption. These differences suggest that in university students, there are diverse underlying mechanisms and behavioral pathways, shedding light on the unique impacts of each behavior on cognition and mental health.

Regarding the first objective (to assess the levels of EE and their relationship with excessive internet usage and alcohol consumption): the average level of EE in the sample of students studied falls within the upper range of slightly emotional eaters (score between 6–10), very close to the range corresponding to emotional eaters (score between 11–20), according to the validation of the instrument conducted with a Spanish clinical population (participants in a weight-reduction program) (49). Although individuals with excess weight score higher on measures of EE than those within the “normal” weight category (59), our result is consistent with the estimated prevalence of EE found in earlier research, whereby 20–45% of adult non-clinical samples self-identify as emotional eaters (8, 60). According to accounts, these episodes frequently appear to be an attempt to prevent, manage, or deal with unpleasant feelings like sadness or anxiety (61). Approximately 44.7% of Spanish university students had mental distress suggestive of anxiety, while 13.5% showed indicators of depression, according to a survey involving approximately 706 individuals (62) and this would be in line with the prevalence data of EE found in this study. Additionally, it has been established that the EE propensity is linked to the tendency toward excessive internet use, but not to an increase in alcohol consumption. Consistently, it has been found that addictive internet use via smartphones in adolescents and young adults is associated with issues of control over eating, particularly with a greater tendency to eat automatically as a reaction to unpleasant feelings or emotions (25). Both excessive internet use and EE have been linked to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general (non-clinical) population (7), and the likely connection between EE and excessive internet use is probably due to both being consequences of a lack of emotional regulation (27). There’s a proposal suggesting that alcohol consumption among university students could be influenced more by contextual factors, such as increased independence, reduced parental supervision, heightened social homogeneity, and the presence of alcohol-related social activities, rather than being primarily linked to psychological distress or difficulties in emotional regulation (63), and this would account for the absence of a correlation between alcohol consumption and excessive eating (EE) or internet overuse. On the other hand, if both EE and alcohol consumption can be understood as two different inadequate coping strategies for negative emotions, it may be that this stress response varies depending on variables such as gender (21), which, due to limitations related to sample size, we have not been able to consider in this pilot study.

Regarding objective 2 (verify the relationship between these behaviors, impulsivity and executive functioning assessed through a VR task). It is found that alcohol consumption is related to flexibility and working memory: lower cognitive flexibility and poorer performance in working memory during the VR task predict a higher tendency for alcohol intake. The relationship between alcohol consumption and executive issues in young population has been described in previous studies (20, 30), although other studies have not found this connection (64). These differences could suggest that self-report questionnaires or conventional neuropsychological tests do not accurately capture the complexity and dynamic character of real-life circumstances. To address these limitations, in our study is that the executive functioning of students has been assessed using an ecological VR task: the Ice Cream Test (ICT). Tools for neuropsychological assessments based on VR may provide improved validity and accuracy for evaluating a variety of cognitive skills, including executive functions (40, 58, 65, 66). There has not been any observed change in executive functions associated with the inclination toward EE or excessive internet use, in contrast to the effects seen with alcohol consumption. In line with the findings outlined in relation to objective 1, alcohol consumption seems to be linked to factors separate from those driving EE and internet overuse. However, both patterns of behavior, EE and excessive internet use, have been associated with increased impulsivity, which serves as a predictor in both instances. Previous studies have shown that decreased inhibitory control or impulsivity are linked to EE through a variety of different processes (67, 68). Impulsivity is defined as the tendency to act quickly in response to any stimulus (external or internal) without prior evaluation of all available information, hence, without considering the potential consequences of the action. From a neuropsychological perspective, impulsivity stems from a lack of inhibitory control, and response inhibition (which allows for the interruption or non-execution of inappropriate behavior) is a skill related to the integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (69), an area still undergoing maturation in university students. Impulsivity plays a fundamental role in addictive behaviors, including non-substance addictions (44, 70). Impulsive behavior is maintained through positive reinforcement and is geared toward seeking pleasure and fulfilling individual needs. Impulsivity seems to underlie the onset of these behaviors, EE, and excessive/problematic internet use. In relation to eating behavior, there’s a proposed concept of an ‘eating continuum’ ranging from EE (considered non-pathological) to binge eating disorder (BED), suggesting an escalation in the severity of overeating behaviors coupled with a rise in deficits in emotion control and inhibition (47), and it would be important to take these factors, impulsivity and lack of inhibitory control, into account for preventive treatments for obesity or eating disorders.

Regarding objective three (to evaluate the role that depression and anxiety may have on these behaviors), trait depression and anxiety are related to excessive internet use and EE, but not to alcohol consumption. Depression emerges as a predictive variable for EE and internet addiction, but trait anxiety only predicts internet overuse, but not EE. A recent study finds that, of all the emotions described as triggers for episodes of loss of control over food intake (anxiety, depression, boredom, or happiness), depression was the factor most clearly associated with these episodes (3). Our results align with that conclusion, and from a clinical perspective, it appears that, in order to develop successful programs to prevent EE and obesity, sadness and depressive tendencies must be taken into consideration.

In our sample of university students, alcohol consumption is not associated with higher levels of anxiety or depression, reiterating that alcohol consumption among Spanish university students is driven by different factors compared to excessive eating (EE) and internet abuse. Emotional needs are not at all the sole motivator for university students to drink. Alcohol consumption is very common in this stage as most young people include alcohol in their leisure activities, and it forms a part of their recreational culture (71, 72).

Despite the phenomenological parallels between addictions and certain abusive acts (substance-free addictions by certain authors) (73, 74), our findings suggest that there are significant distinctions between alcohol intake and other substance-free addictive behaviors in terms of the components involved. Cognitive assessment via the VR task revealed neuropsychological impairment among individuals who consumed alcohol, but not among emotional eaters or those prone to excessive internet use. However, in the latter groups, other psychological factors such as impulsivity, anxiety, and depression appear to be significant contributors. Given the severe detrimental effects of internet overuse (behavior of growing prevalence) (75), both on personal and socio-familial aspects, it is important to consider these results for the implementation of prevention and intervention programs.

Further research is advised to better understand the various causal factors that lead to EE among college students. The study of the prevalence and frequency of EE during the formative years at university, as well as the exploration of the associated risk factors, is necessary. This knowledge will be necessary to support intervention techniques and programs run by public health and policy experts, and college students represent an important demographic for interventions aimed at preventing obesity. The use of VR as an assessment tool can evaluate participants’ performance in more ecological scenarios and provide more reliable results. But it is necessary to develop a more appropriate VR environment in the future for the assessment of cognitive functions, one that incorporates contexts relevant to participants’ concerns, such as EE, internet overuse, and alcohol consumption. This pilot study is the first step toward a more extensive study using this methodology.

This study has several limitations. Firstly, as we have already mentioned at the beginning of this introduction, the small sample size, largely due to the time and resources required to conduct the VR Tasks. A larger sample would allow us to include sociodemographic variables in the analysis, resulting in more broadly generalizable findings. Additionally, all participants were university students from Francisco de Vitoria University in Madrid. It would be desirable to compare the results obtained in this study with those obtained from students in other institutions and with non-student youth of the same age range. Moreover, longitudinal research is required. In reference to impulsivity, it would have also been desirable to assess it through a VR task rather than solely relying on a self-report questionnaire, although the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale shows good values of internal consistency in our study (α =0 0.70).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Universidad Francisco de Vitoria. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

CM-G: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MC-C: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EB-B: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by Universidad Francisco de Vitoria.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants for taking part in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Footnotes

References

1. Berthoud, HR. Metabolic and hedonic drives in the neural control of appetite: who is the boss? Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2011) 21:888–96. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dohle, S, Diel, K, and Hofmann, W. Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite. (2018) 124:4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Braden, A, Barnhart, WR, Kalantzis, M, Redondo, R, Dauber, A, Anderson, LN, et al. Eating when depressed, anxious, bored, or happy: an examination in treatment-seeking adults with overweight/obesity. Appetite. (2023) 184:106510. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2023.106510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Macht, M. How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite. (2008) 50:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lindeman, M, and Stark, K. Emotional eating and eating disorder psychopathology. Eat Disord. (2001) 9:251–9. doi: 10.1080/10640260127552

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Available at: http://repo.iain-tulungagung.ac.id/5510/5/BAB2.pdf

Google Scholar

7. Nolan, LJ, and Jenkins, SM. Food addiction is associated with irrational beliefs via trait anxiety and emotional eating. Nutrients. (2019) 11:1485. doi: 10.3390/nu11081711

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Carlos, M, Elena, B, and Teresa, IM. Are adherence to the mediterranean diet, emotional eating, alcohol intake, and anxiety related in university students in Spain? Nutrients. (2020) 12:1–15. doi: 10.3390/nu12082224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Konttinen, H, Van Strien, T, Männistö, S, Jousilahti, P, and Haukkala, A. Depression, emotional eating and long-term weight changes: a population-based prospective study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2019) 16:1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0791-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barnhart, WR, Kalantzis, MA, and Braden, AL. Mindfulness facets differentially relate to self-reported negative and positive emotional eating types in treatment-seeking adults with overweight/obesity. Eat Weight Disord. (2023) 28:54–11. doi: 10.1007/s40519-023-01578-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bemanian, M, Mæland, S, Blomhoff, R, Rabben, ÅK, Arnesen, EK, Skogen, JC, et al. Emotional eating in relation to worries and psychological distress amid the covid-19 pandemic: a population-based survey on adults in Norway. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:1–10. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Wong, M, and Qian, M. The role of shame in emotional eat. Eat Behav. (2016) 23:41–7. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.07.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bernabéu, E, Marchena, C, and Iglesias, MT. Factor structure and psychometric properties of emotional eater questionnaire (EEQ) in Spanish colleges. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:9090. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17239090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Brenneisen Mayer, F, Souza Santos, I, Silveira, PSP, Itaqui Lopes, MH, de Souza, ARND, Campos, EP, et al. Factors associated to depression and anxiety in medical students: a multicenter study. BMC Med Educ. (2016) 16:282–9. doi: 10.1186/s12909-016-0791-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ishii, T, Tachikawa, H, Shiratori, Y, Hori, T, Aiba, M, Kuga, K, et al. What kinds of factors affect the academic outcomes of university students with mental disorders? A retrospective study based on medical records. Asian J Psychiatr. (2018) 32:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.11.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Adams, DR, Meyers, SA, and Beidas, RS. The relationship between financial strain, perceived stress, psychological symptoms, and academic and social integration in undergraduate students. J Am Coll Heal. (2016) 64:362–70. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2016.1154559

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Stallman, HM. Psychological distress in university students: a comparison with general population data. Aust Psychol. (2010) 45:249–57. doi: 10.1080/00050067.2010.482109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mutalik, N, Moni, S, Choudhari, S, and Bhogale, G. Depression, anxiety, stress among college students in Bagalkot: a college based study. Int J Indian Psychol. (2016) 3:179–86. doi: 10.25215/0304.210

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Angustias Sánchez-Ojeda, M, De Luna-Bertos, E, De, E, and Bertos, L. Hábitos de vida saludable en la población universitaria. Nutr Hosp. (2015) 31:1910–9. doi: 10.3305/nh.2015.31.5.8608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Brotóns, EB, and De la Peña Álvarez, C. Cognitive repercussions of alcohol consumption on academic performance at university: a preliminary study. Electron J Res Educ Psychol. (2019) 17:609–38. doi: 10.25115/ejrep.v17i49.2643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Constant, A, Gautier, Y, Coquery, N, Thibault, R, Moirand, R, and Val-Laillet, D. Emotional overeating is common and negatively associated with alcohol use in normal-weight female university students. Appetite. (2018) 129:186–91. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.07.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Brook, CA, and Willoughby, T. Social anxiety and alcohol use across the university years: adaptive and maladaptive groups. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:835–45. doi: 10.1037/dev0000110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hinojo-Lucena, FJ, Aznar-Díaz, I, Cáceres-Reche, MP, Trujillo-Torres, JM, and Romero-Rodríguez, JM. Problematic internet use as a predictor of eating disorders in students: a systematic review and meta-analysis study. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2151. doi: 10.3390/nu11092151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Tayhan Kartal, F, and Yabancı, AN. Relationship between eating disorders and internet and smartphone addiction in college students. Eat Weight Disord. (2021) 26:1853–62. doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-01027-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Park, EJ, Hwang, SSH, Lee, MS, and Bhang, SY. Food addiction and emotional eating behaviors co-occurring with problematic smartphone use in adolescents? Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:4939. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19094939

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Annesi, JJ. Psychosocial predictors of decay in healthy eating and physical activity improvements in obese women regaining lost weight: translation of behavioral theory into treatment suggestions. Transl Behav Med. (2016) 6:169–78. doi: 10.1007/s13142-016-0401-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Domoff, SE, Sutherland, EQ, Yokum, S, and Gearhardt, AN. Adolescents’ addictive phone use: associations with eating behaviors and adiposity. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:2861. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ding, N, and Xu, Z. China adolescents comparisons on social media and emotional eating: a moderated analysis. Child Adolesc Soc Work J. (2023) 40:107–17. doi: 10.1007/s10560-021-00750-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Arnett, JJ. Adolescence and emerging adulthood. Fifth Edition. Boston: Pearson Education (2017).

Google Scholar

30. García-Moreno, LM, Expósito, J, Sanhueza, C, and Angulo, MT. Actividad prefrontal y alcoholismo de fin de semana en jóvenes. Adicciones. (2008) 20:271–9. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.269

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gogtay, N, Giedd, JN, Lusk, L, Hayashi, KM, Greenstein, D, Vaituzis, AC, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2004) 101:8174–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bennett, CM, and Baird, AA. Anatomical changes in the emerging adult brain: a voxel-based morphometry study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2006) 27:766–77. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Blakemore, SJ, and Choudhury, S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2006) 47:296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A, and Wager, TD. The Unity and Diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Abdul-Raheem, M, Kosonogov, V, and Lyusin, D. Are emotion regulation strategies impacted by executive functions? An experimental study. SSRN Electron J. (2021). doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3880625

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hammud, G, Avital-Magen, A, Schusheim, G, Barzuza, I, and Engel-Yeger, B. How self-regulation and executive functions deficits affect quality of life of children/adolescents with emotional regulation disorders. Children. (2023) 10:1622. doi: 10.3390/children10101622

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Müller, SM, Wegmann, E, García Arias, M, Bernabéu Brotóns, E, Marchena Giráldez, C, and Brand, M. Deficits in executive functions but not in decision making under risk in individuals with problematic social-network use. Compr Psychiatry. (2021) 106:152228. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2021.152228

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chávez-Hernández, ME. Correlation of executive functions, academic achievement, eating behavior and eating habits in university students of Mexico City. Front Educ. (2023) 8:1–9. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1268302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cecilia-Costa, R, Hansmann, M, McGill, DE, Volkening, LK, and Laffel, LM. Association of executive function problems and disordered eating behaviours in teens with type 1 diabetes. Diabet Med. (2021) 38:e14652–15. doi: 10.1111/dme.14652

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Climent Martínez, G, Luna Lario, P, Bombín González, I, Cifuentes Rodríguez, A, Tirapu Ustárroz, J, and Díaz, OU. Evaluación neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas mediante realidad virtual. Rev Neurol. (2014) 58:465–75. doi: 10.33588/rn.5810.2013487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bombín González, I, Cifuentes Rodríguez, A, Climent Martínez, G, Luna Lario, P, Cardas Ibáñez, J, Tirapu Ustárroz, J, et al. Validez ecológica y entornos multitarea en la evaluación de las funciones ejecutivas. Rev Neurol. (2014) 59:77. doi: 10.33588/rn.5902.2013578

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Verdejo-García, A, Lawrence, AJ, and Clark, L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2007.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Herman, AM, and Duka, T. Facets of impulsivity and alcohol use: what role do emotions play? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 106:202–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Müller, SM, Wegmann, E, Arias, MG, Brotóns, EB, Giráldez, CM, and Brand, M. Decision making and risk propensity in individuals with tendencies towards specific internet-use disorders. Brain Sci. (2022) 12:201. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12020201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Atalayer, D. Dürtüsellik ve Aşırı Yeme Arasındaki İlişki: Psikolojik ve Nörobiyolojik Yaklaşımlar. Psikiyatr Guncel Yaklasimlar-Curr Approaches Psychiatry. (2018) 10:1. doi: 10.18863/pgy.358090

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Pivarunas, B, and Conner, BT. Impulsivity and emotion dysregulation as predictors of food addiction. Eat Behav. (2015) 19:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Arexis, M, Feron, G, Brindisi, MC, Billot, PÉ, and Chambaron, S. A scoping review of emotion regulation and inhibition in emotional eating and binge-eating disorder: what about a continuum? J Eat Disord. (2023) 11:197–29. doi: 10.1186/s40337-023-00916-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Decker, R, Pinhatti, MM, DeCastro, TG, and Bizarro, L. Alimentação emocional: Associações entre impulsividade, depressão, sexo e peso corporal em jovens adultos. Psico. (2021) 52:e35452. doi: 10.15448/1980-8623.2021.1.35452

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Garaulet, M, Canteras, M, Morales, E, López-Guimera, G, Sánchez-Carracedo, D, and Corbalán-Tutau, MD. Validation of a questionnaire on emotional eating for use in cases of obesity: the emotional eater questionnaire (EEQ). Nutr Hosp. (2012) 27:645–51. doi: 10.1590/S0212-16112012000200043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Liebert, MA, and Young, KS. Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyber Psychology Behav. (1998) 1:237–44. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Puerta-Cortés, DX, Carbonell, X, and Chamarro, A. Análisis de las propiedades psicométricas de la versión en español del Internet Addiction Test. Trastor Adict. (2012) 14:99–104. doi: 10.1016/S1575-0973(12)70052-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TF, de la Fuente, JR, and Grant, M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption‐II. Addiction. (1993) 88:791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. García-Carretero, MÁ, Moreno-Hierro, L, Robles Martínez, M, de Los Ángeles Jordán-Quintero, M, Morales-García, N, and O’Ferrall-González, C. Alcohol consumption patterns of university students of health sciences. Enferm Clin. (2019) 29:291–6. doi: 10.1016/j.enfcli.2019.01.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Ball, R, and Ranieri, WF. Comparison of Beck depression inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. (1996) 67:588–97. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sanz, J, García-Vera, MP, Espinosa, R, Fortún, M, and Vázquez, C. Adaptación española del Inventario Para la Depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): 3. Propiedades psicométricas en pacientes con trastornos psicológicos Spanish adaptation of the Beck psychometric features in patiens with. Clínica ysalud. (2005) 16:121–42.

Google Scholar

56. Guillén-Riquelme, A, and Buela-Casal, G. Estructura factorial del cuestionario de ansiedad estado-rasgo (STAI) para pacientes diagnosticados con depresión. Salud Ment. (2015) 38:293–8. doi: 10.17711/SM.0185-3325.2015.040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Salinas, JM, Aguilar-Luzón, MC, and Fabregat, M. Propiedades psicométricas de la versión española de la Escala de Impulsividad de Barratt (BIS-11). Univ Granada. (2018) 16:84648. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.18575.84648

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Fernandez, MA, Rebon-Ortiz, F, Saura-Carrasco, M, Climent, G, and Diaz-Orueta, U. Ice cream: new virtual reality tool for the assessment of executive functions in children and adolescents: a normative study. Front Psychol. (2023) 14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1196964

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Dressler, H, and Smith, C. Food choice, eating behavior, and food liking differs between lean/normal and overweight/obese, low-income women. Appetite. (2013) 65:145–52. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.01.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Gibson, EL. The psychobiology of comfort eating: implications for neuropharmacological interventions. Behav Pharmacol. (2012) 23:442–60. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328357bd4e

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Arnow, B, Kenardy, J, and Agras, WS. The emotional eating scale: the development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. Int J Eat Disord. (1995) 18:79–90. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(199507)18:1<79::AID-EAT2260180109>3.0.CO;2-V

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Fernández-Rodríguez, C, Soto-López, T, and Cuesta, M. Needs and demands for psychological care in university students. Psicothema. (2019) 31:414–21. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.78

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lorant, V, Nicaise, P, Soto, VE, and D’Hoore, W. Alcohol drinking among college students: college responsibility for personal troubles. BMC Public Health. (2013) 13:615. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Herrero-Montes, M, Alonso-Blanco, C, Paz-Zulueta, M, Pellico-López, A, Ruiz-Azcona, L, Sarabia-Cobo, C, et al. Relationship of binge drinking with impairments affecting memory and executive function among university students: a cross-sectional study in northern Spain. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:11508. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111508

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Borgnis, F, Baglio, F, Pedroli, E, Rossetto, F, Uccellatore, L, Oliveira, JAG, et al. Available virtual reality-based tools for executive functions: a systematic review. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:1–41. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.833136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kim, H, Kim, DJ, Kim, S, Chung, WH, Park, KA, Kim, JDK, et al. Effect of virtual reality on stress reduction and change of physiological parameters including heart rate variability in people with high stress: an open randomized crossover trial. Front Psych. (2021) 12:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.614539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Stapleton, P, and Whitehead, M. Dysfunctional eating in an australian community sample: the role of emotion regulation, impulsivity, and reward and punishment sensitivity. Aust Psychol. (2014) 49:358–68. doi: 10.1111/ap.12070

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Sims, RC, Bennett, NK, Mwendwa, DT, Ali, MK, Levy, SA, Callender, CO, et al. Executive function and negative eating behaviors in severely obese African Americans. Ethn Dis. (2014) 24:328–34.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

69. Fuster, JM. The prefrontal cortex. Fourth edition. San Diego: Elsevier (2008).

Google Scholar

70. Thomée, S. Mobile phone use and mental health. A review of the research that takes a psychological perspective on exposure. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:2692. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15122692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Currell, CK, and Jeglic, EL. An examination of alcohol and drug use among urban college students. J Subst Use. (2010) 15:272–82. doi: 10.3109/14659890903110590

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Ward, BW, and Gryczynski, J. Alcohol use and participation in organized recreational sports among university undergraduates. J Am Coll Heal. (2007) 56:273–80. doi: 10.3200/JACH.56.3.273-280

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Contreras-Rodríguez, O, Albein-Urios, N, Vilar-López, R, Perales, JC, Martínez-Gonzalez, JM, Fernández-Serrano, MJ, et al. Increased corticolimbic connectivity in cocaine dependence versus pathological gambling is associated with drug severity and emotion-related impulsivity. Addict Biol. (2016) 21:709–18. doi: 10.1111/adb.12242

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Walther, B, Morgenstern, M, and Hanewinkel, R. Co-occurrence of addictive behaviours: personality factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. Eur Addict Res. (2012) 18:167–74. doi: 10.1159/000335662

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Flores, PLM, and Paredes, NM. Behavioural addictions: a new pandemic. Kranion. (2021) 16:131–7. doi: 10.24875/KRANION.M21000015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional eating, alcohol intake, internet addiction, executive functions, virtual reality, impulsivity, depression

Citation: Marchena-Giráldez C, Carbonell-Colomer M and Bernabéu-Brotons E (2024) Emotional eating, internet overuse, and alcohol intake among college students: a pilot study with virtual reality. Front. Nutr. 11:1400815. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2024.1400815

Received: 14 March 2024; Accepted: 06 June 2024;
Published: 18 June 2024.

Edited by:

Arthur Eumann Mesas, University of Castilla La Mancha, Spain

Reviewed by:

Jing Sun, Peking University, China
Renne Rodrigues, Federal University of the Southern Frontier, Brazil

Copyright © 2024 Marchena-Giráldez, Carbonell-Colomer and Bernabéu-Brotons. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Bernabéu-Brotons, e.bernabeu.prof@ufv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.