Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 06 June 2024
Sec. Nutritional Epidemiology

Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and cognitive decline in US elderly participants

Jiaxin LiJiaxin LiJian SunJian SunYishuo ZhangYishuo ZhangBo ZhangBo ZhangLiya Zhou
Liya Zhou*
  • Changchun University of Chinese Medicine, Changchun, China

Objective: To investigate the association between the weight-adjusted-waist index (WWI) and cognitive decline in elderly Americans from 2011 to 2014.

Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted on 2,762 elderly participants from the National Health and Nutrition Examination (NHANES) between 2011 and 2014. WWI was calculated by dividing waist circumference (cm) by the square root of body weight (kg). Participants assessed their cognitive functions using tests such as the DSST, AFT, and CERAD W-L. In this research, multiple logistic regression, HIA, limited cubic spline (RCS), and threshold effect analysis methods were utilized to explore the relationship between cognitive decline and WWI.

Results: The study involved 2,762 participants aged 60 years and older, comprising 1,353 males (49%) and 1,409 females (51%), with a median age of 69.3 years (standard deviation = 6.7). The analysis revealed that the risk of cognitive decline was positively associated with the WWI. Fully adjusted models indicated significant correlations with the CERAD W-L [odds ratio (OR) = 1.24, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.06–1.46, p < 0.008], AFT (OR = 1.27, 95% CI = 1.08–1.49, p = 0.003), and DSST (OR = 1.56, 95% CI = 1.29–1.9, p < 0.001). Subgroup analysis demonstrated a consistent relationship across different population settings except for gender (average of interactions, p > 0.05). A J-shaped relationship between WWI and low DSST scores was observed using multivariate restricted cubic spline (RCS) regression (P for non-linearity <0.05), with the curve steepening when WWI ≥ 12.21 cm/√kg. Additionally, the study found that WWI was more strongly associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline than other obesity indicators such as Body Mass Index (BMI), waist circumference (WC), and A Body Shape Index (ABSI).

Conclusion: Our data have shown a significant positive association between the WWI and a higher risk of cognitive decline in older Americans, with a J-shaped non-linear relationship between WWI and DSST. In addition, our findings indicate that WWI was associated with greater cognitive decline than other markers of obesity.

Introduction

As the global population ages, the deterioration of cognitive functions among the elderly is becoming an increasingly prominent issue. Senile cognitive decline, characterized by a progressive weakening of cognitive abilities including memory loss, inattention, and slower thought processes, is often seen in clinical conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and mixed dementia. In 2015, approximately 46.8 million people were diagnosed with dementia globally, a number projected to rise to 130 million by 2050 (1). Cognitive decline significantly affects the elderly’s quality of life and social welfare, highlighting the importance of research and treatment for cognitive function decline in this demographic. The causes of cognitive decline in the elderly are complex, influenced by genetic, environmental, and lifestyle factors. While the role of genetics has been extensively explored, the effects of environmental and lifestyle factors are also being studied. Hence, managing these risk factors is crucial for slowing cognitive decline and preventing dementia.

Obesity is defined by an excessive accumulation of body fat, presenting significant health risks, and is often quantified using the Body Mass Index (BMI). Excessive nutrient intake can lead to insulin resistance (2), intestinal flora imbalance (3), oxidative stress, and other mechanisms (4), resulting in neuroinflammation and brain damage. As individuals age, their metabolic functions decline, increasing the likelihood of central obesity, a primary contributor to insulin resistance via oxidative stress (5). Central obesity is a key factor in inducing insulin resistance through oxidative stress. With higher levels of inflammatory cytokines (6), central obesity is more likely to cause brain structural abnormalities (7) and impact cognitive function. The weight-adjusted-waist index (WWI) (8) is a novel obesity index that primarily indicates central obesity by integrating the benefits of waist circumference (WC) and BMI (9). Currently, there is no definitive evidence connecting WWI with impaired cognitive function. We hypothesized a positive correlation between WWI levels and cognitive decline. To explore this, our study utilized data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) to investigate the relationship between WWI and cognitive performance deterioration in older American adults. Additionally, we performed a comparative analysis to evaluate how traditional obesity indices like BMI, waist circumference, and A Body Shape Index (ABSI) relate to cognitive decline.

Materials and methods

Study population

This study incorporated data from two cycles of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), part of a national program conducted by the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) to assess nutrition and health in the United States. The survey methodology was approved by the NCHS Research Ethics Review Committee, and all participants provided written informed consent. The study design and data are publicly accessible at www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/. Cognitive function data were collected during the 2011–2012 and 2013–2014 NHANES cycles, involving a total of 19,931 individuals. Participants under 60 years old, those with incomplete cognitive data, or missing WWI measurements were excluded, leaving 2,762 eligible participants for analysis (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the screening process for the selection of eligible participants.

Measurement of WWI

WWI (cm/√kg) was calculated by dividing weight (kg) by the square root of waist (cm). Health technicians, who were trained in body measurement techniques, assessed waist circumference and weight at a mobile examination center (MEC). Information on waist circumference and weight was made publicly available on the NHANES website through the “Body Measurement” feature.

WWI= WC( cm ) weight( kg )

Additionally, we investigated the correlation between obesity indices, including waist circumference (WC), body mass index (BMI), A Body Shape Index (ABSI) and cognitive function decline. ABSI was determined using the formula WC/(BMI2/3 × height1/2). In order to avoid ABSI has a large regression coefficient, we multiplied this value by 1,000.

Cognitive function

A battery of assessments was used to gauge cognitive function in the mobile testing center (MEC), which included the Alzheimer’s Disease Registry Consortium (CERAD) word learning sub-test, the Animal Fluency Test (AFT), and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST). These tests specifically assess executive function, linguistic skills, working memory, and processing speed in the elderly. For example, the CERAD test involves three initial learning trials and one delayed recall trial. Participants are required to read aloud 10 unrelated words individually and then recall as many words as possible immediately after. The AFT and DSST are conducted prior to the delayed recall of the CERAD test. The final score for the CERAD test is the sum of the three learning trials and the delayed recall, each scored from 0 to 10. The Animal Fluency Test measures language fluency by asking participants to name as many animals as they can in 1 minute, with each correct name scoring one point. The DSST tasks participants with matching numbers to symbols within a set time to evaluate processing speed, sustained attention, and working memory. Currently, there is no specific threshold set for low cognitive performance on the CERAD, AFT, or DSST tests. For this study, we adopted the 25th percentile (lowest quartile) of scores as the cutoff point, following the precedent set by existing research and aligning with methodologies described in the literature (1012).

Covariates

We included a range of covariates previously identified in the literature as related to obesity factors and cognitive function. These primarily included social and economic variables (e.g., age), health-related behavior variables (e.g., drinking status), and health-related variables (e.g., diabetes). Detailed descriptions and classifications of these covariates are provided in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The classifications of covariates.

Statistical analysis

We employed the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test to ascertain the normal distribution of variables. Variables with a normal distribution are presented as mean ± SD, while those with a skewed distribution are expressed as median (interquartile range [IQR]). Categorical variables are reported as frequency (%). To examine differences among various WWI groups, we employed one-way analysis of variance for normally distributed data, Kruskal-Wallis H tests for skewed data, and Chi-square or Fisher’s exact tests for categorical data. A binary logistic regression model (showing odds ratios [OR] and 95% confidence intervals [CI]) was used to evaluate the influence of WWI on cognitive decline, adjusting for key covariates. WWI was analyzed as a continuous variable. The choice of these confounders was informed by existing scientific literature (11, 13, 14). Three models were developed: Model 1 without any covariate adjustments; Model 2 adjusted for age, sex, race, education level, marital status, and poverty-to-income ratios; and Model 3 further adjusted for BMI, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, hypertension, stroke, coronary heart disease, and chronic bronchitis. To assess trends and explore potential nonlinearity, WWI was divided into categorical quintiles. A generalized additive model (GAM) with natural splines (Section 4) was used to explore the nonlinear relationship between WWI and cognitive decline by including a quadratic term in the regression model. If a nonlinear relationship was indicated, a two-piecewise regression model was planned to identify the threshold effect of WWI on cognitive function based on smooth plot visualization. Effect sizes and p-values from these models were documented and compared. The subgroup analysis was conducted using the following variables: Gender, age (60–69 vs. 70–79 or ≥ 80 years), BMI (<25 vs. 25–30 or ≥ 30 kg/m2) and history of relevant diseases. A multi-variable logical regression model is used to evaluate the heterogeneity of subgroups, and the interaction is tested in comparison. All analyses were performed using Version 1.9 of the Free Statistics analysis platform, located in Beijing, China, and R Statistical Software (version 4.2.2, http://www.R-project.org, The R Foundation). A two-sided p value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

After implementing inclusion and exclusion criteria, the study involved 2,762 participants, comprising 1,353 (49%) males and 1,409 (51%) females, with a median age of 69.3 years (standard deviation = 6.7). The ethnic composition included 9% Mexican American, 10.4% other Hispanic, 23.6% non-Hispanic Black, 47.2% non-Hispanic White, 8.4% non-Hispanic Asian, and 1.4% from other racial groups. The mean values of the cognitive function tests were as follows: CERAD W-L 25.0 (6.4), AFT 16.7 (5.5), and DSST 46.3 (17.1). Table 2 provides a detailed view of the participants’ clinical characteristics, with columns displaying the number of quintile divisions based on WWI. Compared to the lowest quintile, individuals in the highest quintile of WWI tend to be older, have a higher proportion of women, include more Mexican American, other Hispanic, and non-Hispanic White individuals, and have a larger number of widowed and divorced participants. Additionally, this group shows a significantly higher prevalence of diabetes, hypertension, coronary heart disease, dyslipidemia, stroke, and chronic bronchitis.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the study population, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011–2014 (N = 2,762).

Relationship between weight-adjusted-waist index and cognitive decline

WWI was a continuous variable positively associated with the likelihood of cognitive decline in univariate logistic regression analyses (CERAD W-L OR = 1.28, 95%CI = 1.14–1.44, p < 0.001; AFT OR = 1.23, 95%CI = 1.1–1.38, p < 0.001; DSST OR = 1.46, 95%CI = 1.29–1.66, p< 0.001) (Table 3). The relationship between WWI and cognitive function remained significant even after adjusting for age and sex. Further adjustments revealed that each one standard deviation (SD) increase in WWI increased the risk of scoring low on the CERAD W-L by 28% (95% CI = 1.06–1.46, p < 0.008), increased the risk of scoring low on the AFT by 27% (95% CI = 1.08–1.49, p = 0.003), and increased the risk of scoring low on the DSST by 56% (95% CI = 1.29–1.9, p < 0.001) (Table 3, Model 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association between WWI and cognitive decline.

When WWI was divided into five alleles, individuals in the highest group (Q5) showed a significantly increased likelihood of experiencing cognitive decline by 45, 52, and 94% higher compared to those in the lowest quintile, for CERAD W-L (OR = 1.45, 95% CI = 1.01–1.07, p = 0.043), AFT (OR = 1.52, 95% CI = 1.08–2.14, p = 0.017), and DSST (OR = 1.94, 95% CI = 1.27–2.96, p = 0.002), respectively, as reported in Table 3, Model 3.

The multivariate-adjusted limited cubic spline analysis revealed a J-shaped curve depicting the relationship between WWI and lower scores on CERAD W-L, AFT, and DSST tests (CERAD W-L P for nonlinearity<0.05, AFT P for nonlinearity = 0.004, DSST P for nonlinearity = 0.008) as shown in Figure 2. This analysis indicated a nonlinear dose–response connection, with the prevalence of cognitive impairment increasing as WWI increases In threshold analysis, when WWI was less than 12.21 cm/√kg, the odds ratio of low DSST was 1.256 (95%CI: 1.043–1.512, p = 0.016); when WWI is ≥12.21 cm/√kg, the odds ratio of low DSST was 1.966 (95%CI: 1.014–3.81, p = 0.0453), both of which had statistical significance. The population with WWI ≥ 12.21 cm/√kg accounted for 14.81% of the total population and 73.96% of the top quintile population. When the threshold was 12.716 cm/√kg, the OR value of low CERAD W-L was 1.186 (95%CI: 1.024–1.373, p = 0.00226), which was statistically significant. However, this significant relationship disappeared at WWI ≥ 12.716 cm/√kg (Table 4).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. After covariate adjustment, restricted cubic spline plots for the outcome of cognitive deterioration by WWI levels. (A) CERAD W-L Test. (B) AFT. (C) DSST. The backdrop histograms, which are colored light blue, show the percentage of the research population’s WWI density distribution. The computed adjusted odds ratios are shown by heavy center lines, while the 95% confidence intervals are shown by shaded ribbons. The odds ratio of 1.0 is shown by the horizontal dotted lines (Reference point).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Threshold effect analysis of relationship of WWI with cognitive decline.

Stratified analyses based on additional variables

The study performed stratified analyses across different subgroups to evaluate whether the relationship between WWI and cognitive decline varied among these groups. After stratification by factors such as gender, age, BMI, and history of relevant diseases, the results showed no significant interactions in any of the subgroups except gender (AFT p = 0.046, DSST p = 0.048), with all interaction p-values exceeding 0.05 (Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effect of WWI on cognitive decline in different subgroup (gender, age, health factors). (A) CERAD W-L Test. (B) AFT. (C) DSST.

The weight-adjusted-waist index exhibited a stronger correlation with cognitive decline compared to other obesity markers (BMI, WC, and ABSI)

The indicators of obesity, such as waist circumference (WC), body mass index (BMI), and a body shape index (ABSI), and their association with cognitive decline, were analyzed using smooth curve fitting, as illustrated in Figure 4. A segmented regression model was applied to define the intervals, and threshold effects were calculated, with the results presented in Table 5.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Restricted cubic spline plots for Cognitive decline outcome by BMI, WC and ABSI levels after covariate adjustment. (A) CERAD W-L Test. (B) AFT. (C) DSST. The background histograms (light blue color) represent the per cent of the density distribution of WWI in the study population. Heavy central lines represent the estimated adjusted odds ratios, with shaded ribbons denoting 95% confidence intervals. The horizontal dotted lines represent the odds ratio of 1.0 (Reference point).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Threshold effect analysis of relationship of BMI, WC and ABSI with cognitive decline.

Additionally, we compared the odds ratio (OR) values of WWI with those of other obesity indicators, calculated their z-scores, and utilized them for a multifactor analysis (Table 6). The findings showed that WWI was more strongly associated with poor cognitive function than other obesity markers such as WC, BMI, and ABSI. Specifically, the OR values were as following: CERAD W-L, WWI: OR = 1.24, BMI: OR = 1, WC: OR = 1, ABSI: OR = 1.01; AFT, WWI:OR = 1.27, BMI: OR = 0.99, WC: OR = 1, ABSI: OR = 1.01; DSST, WWI: OR = 1.56, BMI: OR = 0.98, WC: OR = 0.99, ABSI: OR = 1.02. These results suggest that WWI may be a more effective predictor of low cognitive function than other commonly used obesity markers.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Association between WWI,BMI,WC,ABSI and cognitive decline.

Discussion

In this extensive cross-sectional study using NHANES data from 2011 to 2014, it was discovered that the weight-adjusted waist index (WWI) might be significantly linked to cognitive function decline, displaying a J-shaped non-linear relationship (P for nonlinearity <0.05). Notably, the relationship curve becomes steeper at WWI levels exceeding 12.21 cm/√kg, especially in the DSST test. No interaction was observed between WWI and cognitive decline, indicating consistent findings across different subgroups. By placing the WWI quintile, the low cognitive impairment test was significantly more associated with WWI (> 12.06 cm/√kg) than the lowest WWI category (< 10.89 cm/√kg). Additionally, our results showed that WWI had a stronger correlation with reduced cognitive function compared to other obesity indices, suggesting that WWI could be a more effective predictor of cognitive decline than other measures related to obesity. Obesity has been shown to diminish cognitive function across the lifespan (15). Interestingly, several prior studies have confirmed the correlation between obesity indicators such as BMI and WC and impaired cognitive function. Momtaz et al. (16) conducted a cohort study on the elderly population in Malaysia and discovered a significant correlation between low cognitive performance and BMI. Similarly, Huang et al. (17) identified a positive correlation between higher BMI and cognitive decline in younger individuals. Benito-Leon et al. (18) reported that individuals who were overweight or obese scored lower on cognitive function tests, a finding supported by related Mendelian randomization studies (19, 20). In addition to BMI, studies have also revealed a potential relationship between hippocampal volume, waist-to-hip ratio, and visceral obesity, with waist-to-hip ratio being positively associated with high white matter signal intensity (21, 22). Obesity in older adults may result in reduced metabolic activity in the frontal lobe and anterior cingulate gyrus, as well as shrinkage of the hippocampus and thalamus, impacting cognitive functions (2328). A meta-analysis conducted by Tang et al. (7), which included 21 studies and 2,060,687 participants, found that high WC is a risk factor for developing cognitive impairments, underscoring the role of central obesity in cognitive decline. While BMI and WC are commonly employed to assess obesity, they are not without their flaws, and there are ongoing discussions about the “obesity paradox” (29, 30). Consequently, traditional obesity indicators like WC and BMI may not fully capture the role of central obesity in predicting cognitive impairment. WWI, as a newer standard for measuring obesity, not only minimizes the association with BMI but also incorporates WC. This allows for a more precise and comprehensive assessment of central obesity, thereby providing a clearer depiction of the relationship between obesity and cognitive function. Recent studies have demonstrated effectiveness of WWI in differentiating muscle mass from fat mass, leading to its broader application in various medical domains including metabolic diseases, kidney diseases, and cardiovascular diseases, among others (3133). Furthermore, WWI has shown a greater correlation and predictive power in some diseases compared to traditional obesity indices (34, 35). In our study, WWI was found to have a much stronger correlation with poor cognitive function than obesity markers such as BMI, WC, and ABSI (CERAD W-L, WWI: 1.24, BMI: 1, WC: 1, ABSI: 1.01; AFT, WWI: 1.27, BMI: 0.99, WC: 1, ABSI: 1.01; DSST, WWI: 1.56, BMI: 0.98, WC: 0.99, ABSI: 1.02). These findings further support the potential of WWI as a predictor of obesity-related diseases.

Cognitive function tests in the NHANES survey comprised the CERAD test, the animal fluency test, and the number symbol substitution test. These tests evaluated immediate and delayed learning ability, verbal fluency, and executive and attention skills, respectively. Existing research indicates that different factors impact various functional areas of the brain, leading to diverse outcomes across these tests. However, our study identified a significant correlation between the WWI and lower scores on all three cognitive function tests, suggesting that WWI has a comprehensive influence on cognitive decline. While only a limited number of previous studies have investigated the relationship between WWI and cognitive performance, our results align with these studies, confirming that WWI is positively associated with cognitive decline. However, our study adds new insights, by identifying a nonlinear positive correlation and a J-shaped dose–response relationship in the DSST scores, evident in both unadjusted and adjusted models. We also refined the inclusion of covariates by considering detailed past medical histories, including diabetes, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, history of stroke, coronary heart disease, and chronic bronchitis. This approach highlights how obesity is connected not only to metabolic and respiratory disorders but also to increased risks of cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases (36). In addition, some studies have found that cognitive dysfunction that cognitive dysfunction might begin in the initial stages of chronic airway damage and worsen further as the disease deteriorates (37), but relevant evidence is still needed to confirm it. Our study has many advantages. Including the use of a nationally representative sample of elderly individuals from the National Survey on the Health and Nutritional Status of the Elderly. For the first time, we compared the effects of the WWI on cognitive decline against traditional obesity indicators, affirming superiority of WWI. Moreover, various confounding factors were also meticulously accounted for to accurately assess the relationship between WWI and cognitive function. However, our study is not without its limitations. Firstly, to better delineate the potential risk relationship between WWI and low cognitive function during statistical analysis, we categorized the lowest four quintiles of test scores as indicative of low cognitive function, transforming continuous variables into categorical ones. Secondly, the cross-sectional design of our study limits our ability to establish a causal relationship between WWI and cognitive decline. This limitation is a primary focus for our next phase of research, where we aim to corroborate the universality of this relationship through more comprehensive and clinically robust data from multiple countries. Additionally, despite our rigorous consideration of numerous significant confounding factors, we cannot completely rule out the influence of unmeasured confounders related to the risk of cognitive decline. Thus, future studies should aim to address these limitations to validate and expand upon our findings.

Conclusion

In elderly individuals in the United States, an increase in the WWI was associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline. Furthermore, the relationship between WWI and cognitive decline proved stronger than with other obesity indices, indicating that WWI could serve as a reliable indicator of cognitive decline.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/index.htm, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by National Center for Health Statistics Research Ethics Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

JL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft. YZ: Formal analysis, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. BZ: Formal analysis, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. LZ: Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the Chinese Medicine Science and Technology Project of Jilin Province (2024064).

Acknowledgments

We thank the National Center for Health Statistics at the CDC for designing, collecting, and providing the NHANES data for public use.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2024.1390282/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Prince, M, Ali, G-C, Guerchet, M, Prina, AM, Albanese, E, and Wu, Y-T. Recent global trends in the prevalence and incidence of dementia, and survival with dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2016) 8:23. doi: 10.1186/s13195-016-0188-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kothari, V, Luo, Y, Tornabene, T, O’Neill, AM, Greene, MW, Geetha, T, et al. High fat diet induces brain insulin resistance and cognitive impairment in mice. Biochim Biophys Acta Mol basis Dis. (2017) 1863:499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2016.10.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Saiyasit, N, Chunchai, T, Prus, D, Suparan, K, Pittayapong, P, Apaijai, N, et al. Gut dysbiosis develops before metabolic disturbance and cognitive decline in high-fat diet–induced obese condition. Nutrition. (2020) 69:110576. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2019.110576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hajiluian, G, Abbasalizad Farhangi, M, Nameni, G, Shahabi, P, and Megari-Abbasi, M. Oxidative stress-induced cognitive impairment in obesity can be reversed by vitamin D administration in rats. Nutr Neurosci. (2017) 21:744–52. doi: 10.1080/1028415x.2017.1348436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Barzilai, N, Huffman, DM, Muzumdar, RH, and Bartke, A. The critical role of metabolic pathways in aging. Diabetes. (2012) 61:1315–22. doi: 10.2337/db11-1300

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Santos, AL, and Sinha, S. Obesity and aging: molecular mechanisms and therapeutic approaches. Ageing Res Rev. (2021) 67:101268. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101268

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Tang, X, Zhao, W, Lu, M, Zhang, X, Zhang, P, Xin, Z, et al. Relationship between central obesity and the incidence of cognitive impairment and dementia from cohort studies involving 5,060,687 participants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 130:301–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Park, Y, Kim, NH, Kwon, TY, and Kim, SG. A novel adiposity index as an integrated predictor of cardiometabolic disease morbidity and mortality. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:16753. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-35073-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Qin, Z, Chang, K, Yang, Q, Yu, Q, Liao, R, and Su, B. The association between weight-adjusted-waist index and increased urinary albumin excretion in adults: a population-based study. Front Nutr. (2022) 9:941926. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.941926

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gong, Z, Song, W, Gu, M, Zhou, X, and Tian, C. Association between serum iron concentrations and cognitive impairment in older adults aged 60 years and older: a dose-response analysis of National Health and nutrition examination survey. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0255595. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dong, X, Li, S, Sun, J, Li, Y, and Zhang, D. Association of Coffee, decaffeinated coffee and caffeine intake from coffee with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES) 2011–2014. Nutrients. (2020) 12:840. doi: 10.3390/nu12030840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Peeri, NC, Egan, KM, Chai, W, and Tao, M-H. Association of magnesium intake and vitamin D status with cognitive function in older adults: an analysis of US National Health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES) 2011 to 2014. Eur J Nutr. (2020) 60:465–74. doi: 10.1007/s00394-020-02267-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wang, X, Wang, R, Zhang, Z, Luo, C, Zhao, Z, Ruan, J, et al. Level-specific associations of urinary antimony with cognitive function in US older adults from the National Health and nutrition examination survey 2011–2014. BMC Geriatr. (2022) 22:663–11. doi: 10.1186/s12877-022-03351-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wang, A, Zhao, M, Luo, J, Zhang, T, and Zhang, D. Association of Dietary Vitamin K Intake with Cognition in the elderly. Front Nutr. (2022) 9:900887. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.900887

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wang, C, Chan, JSY, Ren, L, and Yan, JH. Obesity reduces cognitive and motor functions across the lifespan. Neural Plast. (2016) 2016:1–13. doi: 10.1155/2016/2473081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Momtaz, YA, Haron, SA, Hamid, TA, Ibrahim, R, and Tanjani, PT. Body mass index (BMI) and cognitive functions in later life. Curr Alzheimer Res. (2018) 15:195–200. doi: 10.2174/1567205014666171004114246

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Huang, T, Chen, Z, Shen, L, Fan, X, and Wang, K. Associations of cognitive function with BMI, body fat mass and visceral fat in young adulthood. Medicina. (2019) 55:221. doi: 10.3390/medicina55060221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Benito-León, J, Mitchell, AJ, Hernández-Gallego, J, and Bermejo-Pareja, F. Obesity and impaired cognitive functioning in the elderly: a population-based cross-sectional study (NEDICES). Eur J Neurol. (2013) 20:899. doi: 10.1111/ene.12083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Marioni, RE, Yang, J, Dykiert, D, Mõttus, R, Campbell, A, Davies, G, et al. Assessing the genetic overlap between BMI and cognitive function. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:1477–82. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mina, T, Yew, YW, Ng, HK, Sadhu, N, Wansaicheong, G, Dalan, R, et al. Adiposity impacts cognitive function in Asian populations: an epidemiological and Mendelian randomization study. Lancet Reg Health West Pac. (2023) 33:100710. doi: 10.1016/j.lanwpc.2023.100710

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Isaac, V, Sim, S, Zheng, H, Zagorodnov, V, Tai, ES, and Chee, M. Adverse associations between visceral adiposity, brain structure, and cognitive performance in healthy elderly. Front Aging Neurosci. (2011) 3:12. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Jagust, W, Harvey, D, Mungas, D, and Haan, M. Central obesity and the aging brain. Arch Neurol. (2005) 62:1545–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.10.1545

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ho, A, Raji, C, Parikshak, N, Becker, J, Lopez, O, Kuller, L, et al. Brain structure and obesity. NeuroImage. (2009) 47:S109. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(09)70976-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bolzenius, JD, Laidlaw, DH, Cabeen, RP, Conturo, TE, McMichael, AR, Lane, EM, et al. Brain structure and cognitive correlates of body mass index in healthy older adults. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 278:342–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Taki, Y, Kinomura, S, Sato, K, Inoue, K, Goto, R, Okada, K, et al. Relationship between body mass index and gray matter volume in 1,428 healthy individuals. Obesity. (2008) 16:119–24. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Volkow, ND, Wang, G, Telang, F, Fowler, JS, Goldstein, RZ, Alia-Klein, N, et al. Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity. (2009) 17:60–5. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Walther, K, Birdsill, AC, Glisky, EL, and Ryan, L. Structural brain differences and cognitive functioning related to body mass index in older females. Hum Brain Mapp. (2010) 31:1052–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20916

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Willette, AA, and Kapogiannis, D. Does the brain shrink as the waist expands? Ageing Res Rev. (2015) 20:86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Geng, J, Deng, L, Qiu, S, Bian, H, Cai, B, Li, Y, et al. Low lean mass and cognitive performance: data from the National Health and nutrition examination surveys. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2021) 33:2737–45. doi: 10.1007/s40520-021-01835-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Dinu, M, Colombini, B, Pagliai, G, Vannetti, F, Pasquini, G, Molino Lova, R, et al. BMI, functional and cognitive status in a cohort of nonagenarians: results from the Mugello study. Eur Geriatr Med. (2020) 12:379–86. doi: 10.1007/s41999-020-00417-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yu, S, Wang, B, Guo, X, Li, G, Yang, H, and Sun, Y. Weight-adjusted-waist index predicts newly diagnosed diabetes in Chinese rural adults. J Clin Med. (2023) 12:1620. doi: 10.3390/jcm12041620

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Li, X, Wang, L, Zhou, H, and Xu, H. Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and chronic kidney disease: a cross-sectional study. BMC Nephrol. (2023) 24:266. doi: 10.1186/s12882-023-03316-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fang, H, Xie, F, Li, K, Li, M, and Wu, Y. Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and risk of cardiovascular diseases in United States adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Cardiovasc Disord. (2023) 23:435. doi: 10.1186/s12872-023-03452-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Wen, Z, and Li, X. Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and female infertility: a population-based study. Front Endocrinol. (2023) 14:1175394. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1175394

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zhang, D, Shi, W, Ding, Z, Park, J, Wu, S, and Zhang, J. Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and heart failure: Results from National Health and nutrition examination survey 1999–2018. Front Cardiovasc Med. (2022) 9:1069146. doi: 10.3389/fcvm.2022.1069146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Khudiakova, AD, Polonskaya, YV, Shramko, VS, Shcherbakova, LV, Striukova, EV, Kashtanova, EV, et al. Blood Adipokines/cytokines in young people with chronic bronchitis and abdominal obesity. Biomol Ther. (2022) 12:1502. doi: 10.3390/biom12101502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Dal Negro, R, Bonadiman, L, Tognella, S, Briccolo, F, and Turco, P. Extent and prevalence of cognitive dysfunction in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, chronic non-obstructive bronchitis, and in asymptomatic smokers, compared to normal reference values. Int J Chron Obstruct Pulmon Dis. (2014) 9:675–83. doi: 10.2147/copd.s63485

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive function, obesity, weight-adjusted-waist index, NHANES, cross-sectional study

Citation: Li J, Sun J, Zhang Y, Zhang B and Zhou L (2024) Association between weight-adjusted-waist index and cognitive decline in US elderly participants. Front. Nutr. 11:1390282. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2024.1390282

Received: 23 February 2024; Accepted: 16 May 2024;
Published: 06 June 2024.

Edited by:

Evelyn Frias-Toral, Catholic University of Santiago de Guayaquil, Ecuador

Reviewed by:

Weiyi Gong, Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention, China
Wenxue Lin, Temple University, United States
Katarina Sebekova, Comenius University, Slovakia

Copyright © 2024 Li, Sun, Zhang, Zhang and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Liya Zhou, Zhouliya1963@outlook.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.