Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 14 May 2024
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Breakfast Around the Globe: Habits, Effects, and Novel Food for Thought View all 5 articles

Association between breakfast patterns and executive function among adolescents in Shanghai, China

Xuelai Wang&#x;Xuelai WangShuangxiao Qu&#x;Shuangxiao QuDongling YangDongling YangWenjuan QiWenjuan QiFengyun ZhangFengyun ZhangRong ZhuRong ZhuLijing SunLijing SunQiong YanQiong YanYue QiYue QiGuizhen YueGuizhen YueCancan YinCancan YinChunyan Luo
Chunyan Luo*
  • Division of Child and Adolescent Health, Shanghai Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Shanghai, China

Introduction: The aim of this cross-sectional study was to investigate the association between breakfast patterns and executive function among adolescents in Shanghai, China.

Methods: In 2022, we randomly recruited 3,012 adolescents aged 12–13 years from all administrative districts in Shanghai. Breakfast information was collected by parents using a one-day recall method. Executive function was measured using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Parent Version. Latent Class Analysis was performed to identify breakfast patterns based on the food groups in the Diet Quality Questionnaire for China.

Results: Breakfast patterns were classified into three categories: “Egg and milk foods”, “Grain foods”, and “Abundant foods”, except for adolescents who skipped breakfast. Logistic regression was used to estimate the multivariate odds ratio (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for the association between breakfast patterns and potential executive dysfunction. Adolescents in the “Abundant foods” class had a lower risk of executive dysfunction in terms of initiate (OR: 0.36; 95% CI: 0.17–0.76), and organization of materials (OR: 0.18; 95% CI: 0.04–0.94), compared to those who skipped breakfast. Similarly, the breakfast patterns of “Grain foods” and “Egg and milk foods” were associated with a lower risk of executive dysfunction, including initiate and working memory.

Discussion: Our findings suggest that breakfast patterns were associated with executive function. The improvement of breakfast patterns among adolescents should be a significant public health intervention.

1 Introduction

Adolescence is a transitional period between childhood and adulthood. Health during this period has an impact throughout the life course (1). The adaptive plasticity of adolescence offers an opportunity to rectify problems that have arisen from earlier life experiences (2). On the other hand, health-related behaviors typically begin or are reinforced during adolescence. These behaviors can have long-term effects on adolescents’ health as adults and increase the burden of disease in adulthood (3). However, adolescent health does not receives adequate attention because adolescence is often considered the healthiest time of life (4). Therefore, more research and evidence-based interventions are needed to improve the health of adolescents.

Adolescence is an important developmental period for maturation in brain function, particularly cognitive abilities (5). Executive function, also called cognitive control, refers to a set of cognitive processes that allow individuals to plan, monitor, and achieve short- and long-term goals (6). These executive processes are essential for almost every aspect of daily life. More importantly, numerous studies have found a longitudinal relationship between executive function with pronounced social skills and academic achievement (7, 8). Executive function skills emerge during the first few years of life and strengthen significantly throughout childhood, adolescence, and into early adulthood (9). The development of executive function is influenced by a variety of biological and environmental factors (10). Nutrition is a readily modifiable factor that can impact brain maturation in school-aged children (11). Thus, effective strategies are needed to enhance the development of executive function skills.

Breakfast is often considered the most important meal of the day. Dietary guidelines generally state that breakfast provides 20–25% of daily energy intake (12). Increasing evidence suggests that consuming breakfast has a positive impact on school performance and cognitive function (12, 13). Particularly, high-quality breakfast patterns were found to be associated with better cognitive performance among adolescents (1416). However, breakfast is the most frequently skipped meal among adolescents worldwide (17). An Australian census data showed that more than 27% of grade 4–12 students reported often skipping or always skipping breakfast (18). Furthermore, the nutritional quality of breakfast was relatively poor among adolescents. A survey of six major cities in China indicated that only 41.7% of students consumed a nutritious breakfast (19). It can be seen that adolescents and the general public lack awareness of the importance of breakfast.

In order to improve the situation of breakfast among adolescents in China, the latest Dietary Guidelines for Chinese School-age Children recommend that school-age children should have breakfast daily, and consume a nutritious breakfast with a variety of foods (20). It also emphasizes that a nutritious breakfast can improve cognitive performance. In fact, the Chinese dietary pattern has shifted significantly in recent decades (21). On the basis of traditional carbohydrate-rich foods, breakfast patterns have transitioned to a modern diet with a high intake of eggs and dairy products (22). To our knowledge, few studies have identified and described breakfast food consumption patterns and examined their association with executive function among adolescents in China. Therefore, in the current study, we described the most frequently observed breakfast patterns among adolescents in Shanghai, China. Furthermore, we aimed to test the hypothesis that better breakfast patterns, including breakfast foods, are associated with good executive function performance, by a cross-sectional study among adolescents aged 12 to 13 years.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design and population

Data was obtained from the 2022 Surveillance of Common Diseases and Health Influencing Factors (SCDHIF) of Students in Shanghai, China, which was an annual cross-sectional survey conducted by the Shanghai Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention (SCDC) (23). The 2022 SCDHIF was conducted from October 2022 to March 2023. The survey samples covered all administrative districts in Shanghai, including seven urban areas (districts of Huangpu, Xuhui, Jing’an, Changning, Hongkou, Yangpu, and Putuo) and nine suburbs (districts of Jinshan, Pudong, Fengxian, Minhang, Songjiang, Jiading, Qingpu, Chongming, and Baoshan).

We recruited participants aged 12 to 13 years old using a multi-stage cluster sampling. The sample size was calculated as follows:

n = p 1 p Z α / 2 2 d 2 = 1411

where p is the percentage of middle school students who do not have breakfast every day in Shanghai (2021), with 21.4%. The Z critical value for a 95% confidence interval is 1.96 for a two-tailed test. d is margin of error, which was cited as 0.1*p. The sample size was doubled to 2,822 in 16 districts, considering the gender stratification. The minimum sample size for each district was 177 participants, so we randomly selected one junior high school from each district. Due to an insufficient number of students sampled from schools in the Huangpu and Jinshan districts, we randomly selected an additional school from these districts. We then randomly sampled entire classes from the 7th grade of the selected schools to meet the minimum sample size requirement. As this survey was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic period. We were allowed to enter the school only when there were no students or teachers infected with COVID-19. If a COVID-19 case was found in school, students were required to undergo home quarantine. The survey was postponed until students were allowed to return to school. Therefore, the participants in this study were either not infected with COVID-19 or had recovered from COVID-19. Finally, a total of 3,012 students participated in the survey.

At the beginning of the survey, SCDC organized a training for investigators from district-level CDCs. Then, the investigators in each district randomly sampled one or two schools and obtained written informed consent from the school principal. The health teacher and class teachers assisted in the implementation. Participants’ parents were asked to record breakfast foods and complete the Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) questionnaire. All participants were assured of the anonymity and confidentiality of the information provided in the survey, and they were free to discontinue their participation at any time during the study. Participants who had completed the questionnaire and valid BRIEF scales were eligible. Those with incomplete questionnaire data or invalid BRIEF scales were excluded (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of participant inclusion.

2.2 Breakfast information collection

Breakfast information was recorded for one day by parents. Parents were asked to record breakfast information for one day by scanning a QR code and entering it into an online platform. The parents or caregivers who could not access the online platform used paper forms instead. According to the breakfast recording, we initially divided participants into eaten group and skipped group. For the eaten group, breakfast consumption was coded into 29 food groups using the Diet Quality Questionnaire (DQQ) tool, which has been adapted to represent foods in the Chinese context (24). DQQ for China contains commonly-consumed foods as follows: 01, staple foods made from grains; 02, whole grains; 03, white root/tubers; 04, legumes; 05, vitamin A-rich orange vegetables; 06, dark green leafy vegetables; 07, other vegetables; 08, vitamin A-rich fruits; 09, citrus; 10, other fruits; 11, grain-based sweets; 12, other sweets; 13, eggs; 14, cheese; 15, yogurt; 16, processed meats; 17, unprocessed red meat (ruminant); 18, unprocessed red meat (nonruminant); 19, poultry; 20, fish and seafood; 21, nuts and seeds; 22, packaged ultra-processed salty snacks; 23, instant noodles; 24, deep fried foods; 25, fluid milk; 26, sweetened tea/coffee/milk drinks; 27, fruit juice; 28, sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) (sodas); 29, fast food. The final breakfast consumption for participants in the eaten group comprised twenty-five food groups (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of DQQ food groups consumed by participants who had breakfast (%).

2.3 Assessment of executive function

In the current study, we used Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) Parent Form to evaluate adolescents’ executive function in the past 6 months. This questionnaire offers an ecological assessment of executive function behaviors in the home environments of children aged 5 to 18 years, which is widely used in epidemiological studies (25, 26). The Chinese version of BRIEF demonstrates high internal consistency, with a reliable test–retest of 0.68–0.89, internal consistency of 0.74–0.96, and internal consistency of 0.74–0.96 (27). The BRIEF consists of 86 items grouped into eight clinical domains of executive function, including inhibit, shift, emotional control, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, organization of materials, and monitor. These clinical scales combine to form two indexes: the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) and the Metacognition Index (MI), as well as one composite summary score, the Global Executive Composite (GEC). The BRI consists of the Inhibit, Shift, and Emotional Control scales, while the MI consists of other scales. The GEC is a summary score of all eight clinical scales. According to the BRIEF professional manual, all clinical scales and indexes were converted into t-scores adjusted for age and sex. A higher t-score indicates more executive function problems. Scores ≥60 (e.g., 1 SD from the mean) were classified as “elevated executive dysfunction” (25).

2.4 Covariates

We collected data on participants’ gender, maternal education, family affluence status, participants’ sleep duration, and whether it was a school day or weekend. Maternal education was categorized as senior high school or below, college, undergraduate, and graduate. Information on family socioeconomic status (SES) was collected separately using the Family Affluence Scale (FAS), which has been proven to be a reliable and valid measure of SES for adolescents in China (28). The FAS scale was categorized into three levels of affluence: low, middle, and high. Sleep duration was found to be associated with breakfast patterns among adolescents (29). Thus, we collected bedtime from the night before and wake-up time from the day to calculate sleep duration. The National Sleep Foundation recommends that children aged 6–13 years should get between 9 and 11 h of sleep (30). The sleep duration in the current study was categorized as “<9 h” and “≥9 h”. In addition, we distinguished between school-day and weekends based on the filling date, as different schedules may affect wake-up time and breakfast time.

2.5 Statistical analyses

We conducted Latent Class Analysis (LCA) to identify mutually exclusive classes of breakfast patterns based on the consumption of 25 food groups using the software Mplus (version 8.3). This approach enables the identification of distinct profiles adopted by subgroups of individuals who follow similar breakfast patterns. The classes can then be used to determine whether a specific breakfast pattern is associated with elevated executive dysfunction outcomes. The best fitting latent class was selected based on the model Akaike Information Criteria (AIC), Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), Adjusted Bayesian Information Criteria (aBIC), entropy, Lo–Mendell–Rubin (LMR) and Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Tests (BLRT). Lower values of AIC, BIC, and aBIC, along with higher entropy, indicate a better model fit. LMR and BLRT compare k and k − 1 class models. A low and significant p value indicates that the k model is superior to the k − 1 class model (p < 0.05).

Data were presented as the mean and standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables, or as the number and percentage for categorical variables. To examine the differences in characteristics of breakfast patterns, a Chi-square test for categorical variables was conducted. We used binary logistic regression to examine the association of breakfast patterns with executive function, with breakfast skipping as the reference group. The unadjusted model was first used. We further adjusted for maternal education (high school or less, junior college, undergraduate, graduate, and missing value), family affluence status (low, middle, high, and missing value), sleep duration (<9 h and ≥9 h), and school day (yes and no). The results of the logistic regression analysis were presented as odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (95% CI).

Additionally, to assess potential effect modification, we performed stratified analyses by gender (boys, girls), maternal education (below undergraduate, undergraduate), and family affluence status (low, middle, high). We tested the statistical significance of the interactions using the likelihood ratio test. We also conducted a sensitivity analysis to test the robustness of our findings by excluding individuals with missing values. Data analysis was conducted using Stata version 14 software (StataCorp, College Station, TX), and a two-sided p-value of <0.05 indicated statistical significance.

3 Results

3.1 LCA of breakfast food groups

Breakfast patterns were identified using LCA. A 5-class model was initially tested, but it did not yield the optimal LMR-LRT and BLRT values, so it was not considered further. Both the 3-class and 4-class models showed significant p-values for LMR-LRT and BLRT tests. However, the 3-class model presented lower AIC, BIC, and aBIC values, suggesting a better fit for the current study (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Model-fit indexes for latent class analysis models.

Class 1 (n = 354, 17.92% of breakfast eaten group) was characterized by a medium level of eggs and fluid milk, and a low level of other food groups. This class was designated as the “Egg and milk foods” class. The largest class, Class 2 (n = 1,445, 73.16% of breakfast eaten group) was characterized by the highest consumption of staple foods made from grains, such as rice, noodles, steamed buns, and bread. We designated it as the “Grain foods” class. Class 3 (n = 176, 8.91% of breakfast eaten group) was characterized by the highest levels of eggs, fluid milk, vegetables, fruits, meats, seafood, nuts, and seeds, as well as a high level of staple foods made from grains. Class 1 was designated as the “Abundant foods” class (Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Response probabilities of breakfast intake food groups in three classes. 01, Staple foods made from grains; 02, Whole grains; 03, White root/tubers; 04, Legumes; 05, Vitamin A-rich orange vegetables; 06, Dark green leafy vegetables; 07, Other vegetables; 08, Vitamin A-rich fruits; 09, Citrus; 10, other fruits; 11, grain-based sweets; 12, eggs; 13, cheese; 14, yogurt; 15, processed meats; 16, unprocessed red meat (ruminant); 17, unprocessed red meat (nonruminant); 18, poultry; 19, fish and seafood; 20, nuts and seeds; 21, deep fried foods; 22, fluid milk; 23, sweetened tea/coffee/milk drinks; 24, fruit juice; 25, fast food.

3.2 Characteristics of study participants

Characteristics of study participants across categories of breakfast intake are shown in Table 2. Boys were more likely to be classified under the “Egg and milk foods” class, while girls were more inclined to be categorized under the “Grain foods” and “Abundant foods” classes. Participants who consumed breakfast were more likely to come from middle-class socioeconomic families, have better maternal education, and have shorter sleep duration (Table 2). The description of BRIEF scores of participants belonging to different breakfast patterns is presented in Supplementary Table S1.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study population characteristics according to breakfast patterns.

3.3 Associations between breakfast patterns with executive function

Compared to those who skipped breakfast, participants who consumed breakfast with “Grain foods” had a lower risk of elevated inhibit score (OR: 0.56; 95% CI: 0.31–1.00). The inverse association was attenuated after adjusting for covariates (OR: 0.62; 95% CI: 0.34–1.14). For the same comparison, participants who consumed “Grain foods” and “Abundant foods” had a 57% lower risk (OR: 0.43; 95% CI: 0.25–0.74) and a 64% lower risk (OR: 0.36; 0.17–0.76) of elevated initiate score, respectively. There was a nonsignificant inverse association between “Egg and milk foods” class and an elevated initiate score (OR: 0.56; 95% CI: 0.30–1.03). In contrast, participants who consumed “Egg and milk foods” had a 49% lower risk (OR: 0.51; 95% CI: 0.29–0.88) and a 41% lower risk (0.59; 95% CI: 0.36–0.95) of elevated working memory score. No significant inverse association was observed between “Abundant foods” class and working memory score (OR: 0.64; 95% CI: 0.35–1.19) (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association between breakfast patterns with executive dysfunction (n = 2081).

3.4 Stratified analyses

In stratified analyses, although the inverse association between breakfast dietary pattern and executive function risk was present across all prespecified groups, including gender, maternal education, and family affluence status (Supplementary Tables S2–S4), a stronger inverse association was observed for girls, participants with lower family affluence status, or those with maternal education below graduate level.

3.5 Sensitivity analyses

In the sensitivity analysis, when we excluded those with unknown values, the results were essentially the same (Supplementary Table S5).

4 Discussion

In the current study, we identified different breakfast patterns among adolescents aged 12–13 years old in Shanghai, including the “Egg and milk foods” class, “Grain foods” class, and “Abundant foods” class. Adolescents who skipped breakfast tended to have more obvious executive dysfunction than their peers who ate breakfast. Additionally, adolescents who had abundant foods for breakfast were at a lower risk of executive dysfunction.

Breakfast is often described as the most important meal of the day, contributing to 25–30% of total daily energy (31). Brain imaging study indicated that the level of glucose metabolism in the brain is much higher in childhood than in adults (32). The rate of glucose metabolism in the brain remains elevated until 9–10 years of age before it declines to the adult level by late adolescence. A well-balanced diet ensures a continuous supply of glucose to support the high brain metabolism in children (33). Breakfast can help stabilize glucose levels throughout the morning, which improves memory, concentration, and makes students more alert as well (34). In other words, individuals who skip breakfast may experience lower blood sugar levels, leading to decreased cortical excitability and difficulties in concentration (13). Grains are a good source of carbohydrates, which mostly break down into glucose, the brain’s primary energy source (34). We found that over 80% of breakfast-eating participants from the “Grain foods” and “Abundant foods” classes consumed grains such as steamed buns, rice, noodles, wontons, and bread. Additionally, their consumption of grains was positively associated with initiate performance. A Korean intervention study reported that consuming a rice-based breakfast has a positive effect on cognitive function in adolescents who usually skip breakfast (15). A study focused on older adults also found that increased consumption of whole grains was linked to a slower rate of global cognitive decline (35). Further research is needed to determine which specific grain foods have a greater impact on executive function and related mechanisms.

The results showed that 43.67% of participants in the eaten group had eggs for breakfast. Both the “Egg and milk foods” and “Grain foods” classes were associated with superior working memory performance. Eggs are a major source of choline, an essential nutrient that contributes to brain development and function (36). Choline is required to produce acetylcholine, a crucial neurotransmitter for memory, mood, and other brain and nervous system functions (35). A few observational studies have shown a link between higher choline intakes and cognitive performance in adults (37, 38), such as better verbal memory, visual memory, and global cognition. Note that humans cannot produce enough choline to meet daily requirements; it needs to be provided through the diet. Eggs provide one of the highest amounts of choline in any natural foods. A randomized controlled study of children aged 9–14 years indicated that egg yolks resulted in higher short-term learning and memory performance (39). As a primary breakfast food, eggs meet the daily choline requirement and may enhance the executive performance of adolescents throughout the day. Although we found no significant difference between the “Abundant foods” class and the skipped breakfast group on working memory, the odds ratio of the “Abundant foods” class showed a downward trend compared to the skipped breakfast group, which may have been influenced by the sample size. A replication study with a larger sample size should be conducted to validate this finding.

Fluid milk was identified as the primary breakfast item across all three breakfast patterns in the study population. They had better executive performance compared to those who skipped the breakfast, which is consistent with several studies (14, 26, 40). An observational study in Chile reported that adolescents who consumed dairy for breakfast showed higher cognitive performance (14). One study from China found that a high dairy intake was related to better executive function performance compared to low intake among children aged 6–12 (26). An interventional study of overweight and obese adults indicated that a high dairy diet has the potential to improve working memory (40). Milk is rich in a wide range of nutrients, including proteins, fats, and micronutrients such as vitamins B12 and calcium, which are relevant to the brain development and function (41).

In the breakfast-eating group, around 20% of participants had unprocessed red meat (nonruminant) for their breakfast. This food group is typically was found in meat buns, wontons, dumplings, and other foods that contain pork filling. Although red meat is considered a limited food from the perspective of preventing noncommunicable diseases (NCDs) prevention (39), its role in improving child cognitive development cannot be ignored. Previous studies have indicated that higher intakes of unprocessed red meat are associated with better general cognitive ability (40, 42). Red meat contributes a wide range of dietary minerals and vitamins essential for neurocognitive development, such as iron, zinc, and vitamin B12 (43). Iron, for example, plays a major role in brain development by being involved in different enzyme systems (44). Iron deficiency at any stage of life could have adverse effects on neurophysiological function (45). However, the evidence regarding the effects of red meat on the executive function of adolescents was limited (46). It is worth exploring in depth whether red meat might have beneficial effects on specific executive domains for adolescents.

Through stratified analysis, we found that breakfast patterns were significantly more associated with the clinical domains and global executive function of adolescents whose mothers had lower education level. Similar results were presented in the adolescents with lower family affluence status. A cohort study from Australia indicated that a healthy dietary pattern among adolescents was positively associated with higher maternal education level and better family functioning (47). A study conducted in Hong Kong suggested that adolescents from low-income families were more vulnerable to diet-related health issues (48). They received poor food guidance from their family and developed the misconception that “healthy food is expensive” (48). Therefore, adolescents from low social status should be the primary focus of dietary interventions, particularly for breakfast. Improving breakfast patterns can have a greater impact on the executive function of adolescents from low social status compared to their high-status counterparts. It is possible to be a feasible and affordable intervention for low-income families.

The main strength of this study was being the first to identify breakfast patterns and investigate their association with executive function among adolescents China. The findings from this study contribute to an important direction of scientific research, monitoring, and intervention for the local population. There are some limitations in this study. First, this cross-sectional study can only demonstrate the association between breakfast patterns and executive function; it cannot determine a causal relationship. Second, we only investigated one-day breakfast information, which partially reflected breakfast patterns among adolescents in Shanghai. Third, we have not taken into account food intake throughout the day, which could have been a confounding factor between breakfast patterns and executive function performance. The implementation of this study was also affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. As much as possible, we have avoided conducting surveys while student is ill. Even if COVID-19 patients recover, they may experience a reduced appetite, which can impact their breakfast consumption. Finally, since the participants in our study were 12–13 years old, further research is needed to concentrate on adolescents of different age groups and identify changes in breakfast patterns as adolescents grow older.

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, we found that a breakfast pattern with abundant foods was associated with good executive function performance among adolescents in China. It is recommended that adolescents should have a variety of foods for breakfast, such as grains, eggs, milk, red meat, and other dietary categories like fruits or nuts. We also demonstrated that breakfast patterns were more strongly linked to executive function in female adolescents and adolescents whose mothers had lower education levels or who came from low-income families. Further research is required to validate our results. If these findings hold true, it is necessary to implement nutritional monitoring and intervene with adolescents who have poor breakfast habits. Breakfast interventions may be a beneficial strategy to reduce the gap in executive function among adolescents from low-income families.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the Shanghai Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2022-13). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

XW: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. SQ: Project administration, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. DY: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. WQ: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. FZ: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. RZ: Data curation, Software, Writing – review & editing. LS: Resources, Writing – review & editing. QY: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. YQ: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. GY: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CY: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by National Social Science Foundation of China (22BTY105).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank health staff from each district CDC and community-based health service centers. We also thank the participating teachers, parents and students for their commitment to this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2024.1373129/full#supplementary-material

References

1. WHO . Health for the world’s adolescents: a second chance in the second decade. WHO (2014).

Google Scholar

2. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine; Health and Medicine Division; Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Committee on the Neurobiological and Socio-behavioral Science of Adolescent Development and Its Applications In: EP Backes and RJ Bonnie, editors. The promise of adolescence: Realizing opportunity for all youth, vol. 2. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US) (2019) Adolescent Development. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK545476/

Google Scholar

3. Mokdad, AH, Forouzanfar, MH, Daoud, F, Mokdad, AA, El Bcheraoui, C, Moradi-Lakeh, M, et al. Global burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors for young people’s health during 1990–2013: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. Lancet. (2016) 387:2383–401. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(16)00648-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Patton, GC, Sawyer, SM, Santelli, JS, Ross, DA, Afifi, R, Allen, NB, et al. Our future: a lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet. (2016) 387:2423–78. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(16)00579-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Larsen, B, and Luna, B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 94:179–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Berthelsen, D, Hayes, N, White, SLJ, and Williams, KE. Executive function in adolescence: associations with child and family risk factors and self-regulation in early childhood. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:903. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00903

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cancela, J, Burgo, H, and Sande, E. Physical fitness and executive functions in adolescents: cross-sectional associations with academic achievement. J Phys Ther Sci. (2019) 31:556–62. doi: 10.1589/jpts.31.556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ben-Asher, E, Porter, BM, Roe, MA, Mitchell, ME, and Church, JA. Bidirectional longitudinal relations between executive function and social function across adolescence. Dev Psychol. (2023) 59:1587–94. doi: 10.1037/dev0001580

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Best, JR, and Miller, PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1641–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Costello, SE, Geiser, E, and Schneider, N. Nutrients for executive function development and related brain connectivity in school-aged children. Nutr Rev. (2021) 79:1293–306. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuaa134

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Deoni, SCL . Neuroimaging of the developing brain and impact of nutrition. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. (2018) 89:155–74. doi: 10.1159/000486500

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Giménez-Legarre, N, Miguel-Berges, ML, Flores-Barrantes, P, Santaliestra-Pasías, AM, and Moreno, LA. Breakfast characteristics and its association with daily micronutrients intake in children and adolescents-a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. (2020) 12:12. doi: 10.3390/nu12103201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Adolphus, K, Lawton, CL, Champ, CL, and Dye, L. The effects of breakfast and breakfast composition on cognition in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Adv Nutr. (2016) 7:590s–612s. doi: 10.3945/an.115.010256

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Peña-Jorquera, H, Campos-Núñez, V, Sadarangani, KP, Ferrari, G, Jorquera-Aguilera, C, and Cristi-Montero, C. Breakfast: a crucial meal for Adolescents’ cognitive performance according to their nutritional status. The Cogni-action project. Nutrients. (2021) 13:13. doi: 10.3390/nu13041320

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kim, HS, Jung, SJ, Mun, EG, Kim, MS, Cho, SM, and Cha, YS. Effects of a Rice-based diet in Korean adolescents who habitually skip breakfast: a randomized, parallel group clinical trial. Nutrients. (2021) 13:13. doi: 10.3390/nu13030853

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Adolphus, K, Hoyland, A, Walton, J, Quadt, F, Lawton, CL, and Dye, L. Ready-to-eat cereal and milk for breakfast compared with no breakfast has a positive acute effect on cognitive function and subjective state in 11–13-year-olds: a school-based, randomised, controlled, parallel groups trial. Eur J Nutr. (2021) 60:3325–42. doi: 10.1007/s00394-021-02506-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gibney, MJ, Barr, SI, Bellisle, F, Drewnowski, A, Fagt, S, Hopkins, S, et al. Towards an evidence-based recommendation for a balanced breakfast-a proposal from the international breakfast research initiative. Nutrients. (2018) 10:10. doi: 10.3390/nu10101540

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sincovich, A, Moller, H, Smithers, L, Brushe, M, Lassi, ZS, Brinkman, SA, et al. Prevalence of breakfast skipping among children and adolescents: a cross-sectional population level study. BMC Pediatr. (2022) 22:220. doi: 10.1186/s12887-022-03284-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. National Institute for Nutrition and Health CCfDCaP . White paper on breakfast behavior of Chinese residents. (2022)

Google Scholar

20. Na, Z, Wenli, Z, Man, Z, and Guansheng, M. Interpretation on Dietary Guidelines for Chinese School-aged Children (2022). Chin J School Health. (2022) 43:805. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2022.06.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Popkin, BM . Synthesis and implications: China’s nutrition transition in the context of changes across other low- and middle-income countries. Obes Rev. (2014) 15:60–7. doi: 10.1111/obr.12120

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Li, L, Cao, W, Xu, J, Pan, H, Yang, T, Xu, P, et al. Breakfast food varieties of children aged 6-17 in China from 2010 to 2012. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. (2019) 48:395–8. doi: 10.19813/j.cnki.weishengyanjiu.2019.03.029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Yang, D, Luo, C, Feng, X, Qi, W, Qu, S, Zhou, Y, et al. Changes in obesity and lifestyle behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic in Chinese adolescents: a longitudinal analysis from 2019 to 2020. Pediatr Obes. (2022) 17:e12874. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12874

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ma, S, Herforth, AW, Vogliano, C, and Zou, Z. Most commonly-consumed food items by food group, and by province, in China: implications for diet quality monitoring. Nutrients. (2022) 14:14. doi: 10.3390/nu14091754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gui, Z, Huang, S, Chen, Y, Zhao, Y, Jiang, N, Zhang, S, et al. Association between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and executive function in children. Nutrients. (2021) 13:13. doi: 10.3390/nu13124563

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zeng, X, Cai, L, Gui, Z, Shen, T, Yang, W, Chen, Q, et al. Association between dairy intake and executive function in Chinese children aged 6-12 years. Front Nutr. (2022) 9:879363. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.879363

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Qian, Y, and Wang, YF. Reliability and validity of behavior rating scale of executive function parent form for school age children in China. Beijing Da Xue Xue Bao. (2007) 39:277–83. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1671-167X.2007.03.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Liu, Y, Wang, M, Tynjälä, J, Villberg, J, Lv, Y, and Kannas, L. Socioeconomic differences in adolescents’ smoking: a comparison between Finland and Beijing, China. BMC Public Health. (2016) 16:805. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3476-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Liu, A, Fan, J, Ding, C, Yuan, F, Gong, W, Zhang, Y, et al. The Association of Sleep Duration with breakfast patterns and snack behaviors among Chinese children aged 6 to 17 years: Chinese National Nutrition and health surveillance 2010-2012. Nutrients. (2022) 14:14. doi: 10.3390/nu14112247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hirshkowitz, M, Whiton, K, Albert, SM, Alessi, C, Bruni, O, DonCarlos, L, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health. (2015) 1:40–3. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chinese Nutrition Society. Chinese Dietary Guidelines . People’s medical publishing house (2022). 2022 p.

Google Scholar

32. Kuzawa, CW, Chugani, HT, Grossman, LI, Lipovich, L, Muzik, O, Hof, PR, et al. Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2014) 111:13010–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323099111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bellisle, F . Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. Br J Nutr. (2004) 92:S227–32. doi: 10.1079/bjn20041171

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Slavin, J, and Carlson, J. Carbohydrates. Adv Nutr. (2014) 5:760–1. doi: 10.3945/an.114.006163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zeisel, SH . Choline: critical role during fetal development and dietary requirements in adults. Annu Rev Nutr. (2006) 26:229–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.26.061505.111156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. National Institutes of Health Office of Dietary Supplements . Choline fact sheet for health professionals, Available at: https://ods.od.nih.gov/factsheets/Choline-HealthProfessional/

Google Scholar

37. Poly, C, Massaro, JM, Seshadri, S, Wolf, PA, Cho, E, Krall, E, et al. The relation of dietary choline to cognitive performance and white-matter hyperintensity in the Framingham offspring cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. (2011) 94:1584–91. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.008938

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Nurk, E, Refsum, H, Bjelland, I, Drevon, CA, Tell, GS, Ueland, PM, et al. Plasma free choline, betaine and cognitive performance: the Hordaland health study. Br J Nutr. (2013) 109:511–9. doi: 10.1017/s0007114512001249

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Herforth, AW, Wiesmann, D, Martínez-Steele, E, Andrade, G, and Monteiro, CA. Introducing a suite of low-burden diet quality indicators that reflect healthy diet patterns at population level. Curr Dev Nutr. (2020) 4:nzaa168. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzaa168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hepsomali, P, and Groeger, JA. Diet and general cognitive ability in the UK biobank dataset. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:11786. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-91259-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cohen Kadosh, K, Muhardi, L, Parikh, P, Basso, M, Jan Mohamed, HJ, Prawitasari, T, et al. Nutritional support of neurodevelopment and cognitive function in infants and young children-an update and novel insights. Nutrients. (2021) 13:13. doi: 10.3390/nu13010199

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ylilauri, MPT, Hantunen, S, Lönnroos, E, Salonen, JT, Tuomainen, TP, and Virtanen, JK. Associations of dairy, meat, and fish intakes with risk of incident dementia and with cognitive performance: the Kuopio Ischaemic heart disease risk factor study (KIHD). Eur J Nutr. (2022) 61:2531–42. doi: 10.1007/s00394-022-02834-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Salter, AM . The effects of meat consumption on global health. Rev Sci Tech. (2018) 37:47–55. doi: 10.20506/rst.37.1.2739

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Nyaradi, A, Li, J, Hickling, S, Foster, J, and Oddy, WH. The role of nutrition in children’s neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:97. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Ferreira, A, Neves, P, and Gozzelino, R. Multilevel impacts of Iron in the brain: the cross talk between neurophysiological mechanisms, cognition, and social behavior. Pharmaceuticals. (2019) 12:12. doi: 10.3390/ph12030126

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Dalile, B, Kim, C, Challinor, A, Geurts, L, Gibney, ER, Galdos, MV, et al. The EAT-lancet reference diet and cognitive function across the life course. Lancet Planet Health. (2022) 6:e749–59. doi: 10.1016/s2542-5196(22)00123-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ambrosini, GL, Oddy, WH, Robinson, M, O’Sullivan, TA, Hands, BP, de Klerk, NH, et al. Adolescent dietary patterns are associated with lifestyle and family psycho-social factors. Public Health Nutr. (2009) 12:1807–15. doi: 10.1017/s1368980008004618

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Siu, JY, Chan, K, and Lee, A. Adolescents from low-income families in Hong Kong and unhealthy eating behaviours: implications for health and social care practitioners. Health Soc Care Community. (2019) 27:366–74. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Crichton, GE, Murphy, KJ, Howe, PR, Buckley, JD, and Bryan, J. Dairy consumption and working memory performance in overweight and obese adults. Appetite. (2012) 59:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.03.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: breakfast patterns, skipping breakfast, adolescents, executive function, latent class analysis

Citation: Wang X, Qu S, Yang D, Qi W, Zhang F, Zhu R, Sun L, Yan Q, Qi Y, Yue G, Yin C and Luo C (2024) Association between breakfast patterns and executive function among adolescents in Shanghai, China. Front. Nutr. 11:1373129. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2024.1373129

Received: 19 January 2024; Accepted: 23 April 2024;
Published: 14 May 2024.

Edited by:

Tobias Otterbring, University of Agder, Norway

Reviewed by:

Cunjian Bi, Chizhou University, China
Jia Zhao, Southwest University, China
Stacey Finkelstein, Stony Brook University, United States

Copyright © 2024 Wang, Qu, Yang, Qi, Zhang, Zhu, Sun, Yan, Qi, Yue, Yin and Luo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chunyan Luo, bHVvY2h1bnlhbkBzY2RjLnNoLmNu

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.