Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 22 August 2022
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Sugar-sweetened Beverages and Cognitive Function in Children View all 10 articles

Association of sugar-sweetened beverages with executive function in autistic children

\r\nShuolin Pan&#x;Shuolin Pan1†Xin Wang&#x;Xin Wang2†Lizi LinLizi Lin3Jiajie ChenJiajie Chen1Xiaoling ZhanXiaoling Zhan1Chengkai JinChengkai Jin1Xiaoxuan OuXiaoxuan Ou1Tingfeng GuTingfeng Gu1Jin Jing*Jin Jing1*Li Cai*Li Cai1*
  • 1Department of Maternal and Child Health, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition and Education Sciences, Ministry of Education; Institute for Brain Research and Rehabilitation, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Guangdong Provincial Engineering Technology Research Center of Environmental Pollution and Health Risk Assessment, Department of Occupational and Environmental Health, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

The association between sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) consumption and executive function (EF) among typically developing (TD) children has been investigated in previous studies but with inconsistent results. Furthermore, this relationship has been less investigated among autistic children who perform worse in EF compared with TD children. In this study, we aimed to investigate the association between SSB consumption and EF in autistic children, and whether the association between SSB and EF in autistic children is different from that in TD children. We recruited 106 autistic children and 207 TD children aged 6–12 years in Guangzhou, China. Children’s EF was assessed by using the Chinese version of parent-reported Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Stroop Color–Word Test, and working memory subscales of the Chinese version of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children, Fourth edition. Meanwhile, we assessed children’s dietary intake and SSB consumption with a validated Food Frequency Questionnaire. In this study, 70 (66.0%) autistic children consumed SSB and 20 (18.9%) of them consumed more than two servings SSB a week. Among autistic children, over two servings per week SSB consumption was associated with poorer performance in emotional control [β = 7.20, 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.94–13.46] and plan/Organize (β = 6.45, 95% CI: 0.27–12.63). The association between over two servings/week SSB consumption and emotional control among autistic children was significantly different from that among TD children (βASD = 7.20; βTD = −3.09, Z = 2.72, p = 0.006). Results of this study show that SSB consumption was associated with an impairment in some subscales of EF in autistic children. Furthermore, the association between SSB and EF in autistic children might be different from that in TD children.

Introduction

Consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) among children remains at a high level, especially in low- and middle-income countries, including China (13). SSBs such as carbonated beverages and sugar-sweetened fruit juice beverages, were suggested to have an association with an increased risk of children’s physical health problems, including dental caries, obesity(s) and other metabolic diseases (4, 5). Some previous studies suggested that SSB consumption was linked with mental problems, such as impaired cognitive function (6, 7), executive function (EF) (8), as well as behavioral problems, including hyperactivity problems and emotional symptoms (9, 10).

Executive function is an umbrella term for functions that include two dimensions and associate with a child’s cognitive functioning, behavior, emotional control, and social interaction (11). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder with impaired social communication and interaction, repetitive behaviors, and varying levels of intellectual disability (12). Compared with typically developing (TD) children, children with ASD performed significantly worse in EF (1316). Although EF impairment commonly exists in autistic children, high-order EF will have sustainable development and is sensitive to environmental factors [e.g., nutrition (17), air pollution (18), lifestyle (19, 20), and socioeconomic status (21)] during school age. Therefore, it is of significance to improve EF or reduce impairment of EF in autistic children by controlling some environmental factors, especially the highly modifiable dietary factors including SSB.

Studies in healthy animals have shown that fructose consumption could adversely influence synaptic plasticity and cognition (2228). In population studies, previous reports have investigated the association of SSB consumption with EF among school-aged TD children, but with inconsistent findings. Some studies indicated that SSB had an inverse relationship with all the indexes of EF (8, 9, 2932), but a few of them indicated that SSB had no relationship with some of the indexes or could be beneficial for children’s cognition EF (29, 33, 34). Inconsistencies in the findings could be partly explained by difference in assessments of EF, ethnicity, and economic development levels.

Although previous studies indicated that SSB consumption was inversely related with EF in school-aged TD children, few studies investigated the relationship of SSB with EF in autistic children (35, 36). Furthermore, several studies suggested that autistic children had a high preference for energy-dense foods, leading to a high consumption of sugar, juice, and sweetened beverages (3741). Since the food preference and EF of autistic children was different from TD children, it is reasonable to hypothesize that the association between SSB and EF in autistic children may be different from that in TD children. Therefore, we aimed to investigate (1) the association between SSB consumption and EF of autistic children; and (2) whether the association between SSB and EF in autistic children is different from that in TD children.

Materials and methods

Study design and participants

In this cross-sectional study, a total of 107 autistic children and 209 TD children aged 6–12 years were recruited from a study entitled “the Guangzhou Longitudinal Study of Children with ASD” in Guangzhou, China. The autistic children had to have a historical diagnosis of ASD, autism, or Asperger’s syndrome and be confirmed by two professional child psychiatrists (Xiuhong Li and Jin Jing) according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Revision (DSM-5) criteria.

Both groups conformed to the additional inclusion criteria as follows: (1) chronological age between 6 years 0 month and 12 years 11 months 30 days; (2) voluntarily participation of the children’s parents; (3) without known genetic or chromosomal abnormalities or severe visual or hearing impairment; and (4) without any other medical diagnosis of neuropsychiatric disorders such as ADHD, seizures, Tourette syndrome, head trauma, cerebral palsy, or other movement disorders that would interfere with study assessment.

In the current study, one autistic child had missed data from subscales score of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) and two TD children had an Intelligence Quotient (IQ) below 70. They were not included in any of the analysis. A subsample of 106 autistic children (89 boys and 17 girls) and 207 TD children (113 boys and 94 girls) were included for final analysis. All 313 participants took part in the Stroop Color–Word Test (SCWT). Because of its presupposition of literacy, only 76 autistic children and 173 TD children finished SCWT. At the research center, 99 autistic children and 207 TD children finishing the IQ test. They had complete scores of four index scores and a full score of IQ (the flowchart was in Supplementary Figure 1).

Procedure

Children underwent face-to-face measures performed by well-trained psychologists and research assistants at the research center. All the parents of the participants were provided with written consent. This study was approved by the Ethical Review Committee for Biomedical Research at Sun Yat-sen University (2015-No.29).

Measures

Assessment of executive function

We evaluated EF of children via parent-reported questionnaire and behavioral experiment.

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function is a parent-reported questionnaire for parents of children aged 6–18 years. Parents were asked to rate the 86 items by evaluating how often the problem bothered their child in the past 6 months. The 86 items are rated into three ranks, which are “never,” “sometimes,” and “often,” corresponding to the scores “1,” “2,” and “3,” respectively. The BRIEF comprises three composite indexes (i.e., behavioral regulation index, BRI; metacognition index, MI; and global executive composite, GEC). The BRI reflects the ability to shift cognitive set and modulate emotions and behaviors via appropriate inhibitory control, containing three subscales (i.e., inhibit, shift, and emotional control). The MI reflects the ability to initiate, plan, organize, and sustain future oriented problem-solving in working memory, containing initiate, working memory, plan/organize, organization, and monitor subscales. The GEC represents a sum of all the eight subscale scores. The BRI, MI, and GEC were converted into T-scores (mean = 50, standard deviation = 10) and standardized by gender and age. Higher scores indicate greater impairment in EF. Among the school-aged children in China, the subscales and total scores have good internal consistency (0.74–0.96), except for the initial subscale (0.61), and good test-retest reliability (0.68–0.89) (42).

Stroop Color–Word Test

The SCWT is a widely used neuropsychological measure that can assess inhibitory control (43). Due to its presupposition of literacy, the test is limited to school-aged children. In the current study, this test consists of three subtasks, namely, reading names of colors serially, naming the colors, and naming the color of ink instead of the words. Each subtask contains 10 trials, and each trail presents one kind of color or a color word. The E-Prime 2.0 on computer was used to run the task and record the response. The participants were asked to respond by pressing buttons on the keyboard as quickly and accurately as possible. The average reaction times (RTs) of all the correct answers would replace the RT of all the wrong answers. Stroop interference (SI) is the difference in RTs between naming the color and naming the color of the ink instead of the words. We recorded the correct rate of Stroop and SI to assess the participants’ performance on SCWT.

Working Memory Index

We also used Working Memory Index (WMI) in the Chinese version of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) to assess children’s EF (44). WISC-IV has an internal consistency ranging from 0.98 to 0.99 and test-retest reliability ranging from 0.71 to 0.86. The WMI is made up of Digit Span (DS) and Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS) subtests. DS includes Digit Span Forward (DSF) and Digit Span Backward (DSB), and scoring combines the total number of correctly repeated digit strings. The LNS subtests require children to repeat the sequence of letters and numbers provided randomly in a predetermined order. An arithmetic subtest may be used as a replacement if a participant cannot finish one of the two aforementioned subtests. In this subtest, children are required to answer verbally presented arithmetic problems. A higher score of WMI indicates greater working memory ability.

Dietary intake and sugar-sweetened beverage consumption

Dietary intake was assessed by using a validated Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) (45). Parents of children were required to report the frequency and amount of food their children consumed during the past 7 days. The list of foods included cereals, vegetables, fruits, dairy foods, soybeans, red meat and products, poultry and game, fish and shrimp, nuts, eggs, salt, SSBs, cooking oil, and water. To assist the interview, we provided food photographs with standard portion sizes.

For SSB consumption, parents were asked to answer the questions “During the past seven days, how many times had your child drunk SSB (e.g., Coca-Cola, Sprite, orange juice, etc.)?” and “How many glasses (250 milliliters per glass) of SSB had your child consumed on average each time?” In this study, we used the term “servings” to describe the intake of SSB, and 250 ml of SSB was defined as one serving. We also classified the SSB servings per week into three categories as “0 servings/week, >0–2 servings/week, and >2 servings/week.”

Assessment of covariates

Social demography factors

Demographic information such as children’s gender, age, maternal and paternal education level, and per capita monthly household income was obtained via questionnaires.

Physical activity and sedentary time

Physical activity (PA) and sedentary time (ST) during the past 7 days were assessed via the International Physical Activity Questionnaire Short Form (IPAQ-SF). Parents were required to report the weekly frequency and duration of vigorous-intensity activities (VPA), moderate activities (MPA), and walking. ST includes after-school homework time and screen time of the participants (including watching television and taking online courses). Three levels of PA were classified as “HEPA active,” “Minimally active,” and “Inactive” according to the criteria based on different energy requirements of activities (46). We also classified the screen time per day into two categories as “<2 hours/day” and “≥2 hours/day.”

Anthropometric measurements

Anthropometric measurements were taken according to the standard protocol of the National Standard Test Method for Students’ Physical Health. The body mass index (BMI) of each participant was calculated as weight (kg) divided by height squared (m2). According to the Chinese Standards, we classified the BMI of the children into four categories, namely, underweight, healthy weight, overweight, and obese (47, 48).

Statistical analyses

Means and standard deviations were calculated to describe continuous variables and percentages to describe categorical variables. We compared demographic information between children with ASD and TD children using t-tests and Chi-square tests.

We used generalized linear models to investigate the associations of EF and SSB consumption. Crude models were fitted without any adjustments. Adjusted model 1 was fitted by adjusting the child’s age, gender, maternal and paternal education level, and per capita monthly household. Adjusted model 2 was fitted by further adjusting screen-exposure time, category of PA, category of BMI, and water intake. Adjusted model 3 further adjusted the category of IQ (<70 and ≥70). To investigate whether the association between SSB and EF of autistic children is different from that of TD children, we further implemented a test for coefficient estimate (β), based on the point estimate and standard error (SE):

Z = β A S D - β T D s e ( β A S D ) 2 + s e ( β A S D ) 2

All statistical analysis was conducted with the R 4.1.3 statistical software (R Core Team) (49). Coefficient estimate (β) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) were presented as the results. A two-sided p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Characteristics of autistic children and typically developing children

A total of 106 autistic children and 207 TD children aged 6–10 years participated in the investigation (Table 1). Among autistic children, the majority of children (84.0%) were boys. Among TD children, approximately half of the children were boys (54.6%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of children with ASD and TD children.

Autistic children had a significantly higher average BMI (16.6 ± 2.6 vs. 15.8 ± 2.4; p < 0.01) compared with TD children. Autistic children showed higher rates of obesity (15.1 vs. 6.3%) and lower rates of healthy weight (64.2 vs. 71.9%). The PA category also had a significant difference between children with ASD and TD children (inactive: 10.4 vs. 3.4%; minimally active: 43.4 vs. 36.2%; HEPA active: 46.2 vs. 60.4%; p < 0.01). Lower maternal education level and lower monthly household income (both p < 0.01) were noted in children with ASD.

Comparison of sugar-sweetened beverage in children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children

The mean (SD) of servings of SSB consumption per week was 1.3 (1.6) among autistic children and 1.2 (1.7) among TD children. A total of 20 (18.9%) autistic children and 26 (12.6%) TD children reported consuming no less than two servings of SSB per week. Thirty-six (34.0%) autistic children and 83 (40.1%) TD children reported consuming 0 servings of SSB per week (Figure 1). However, there was no statistically significant difference between the SSB consumption of autistic children and TD children (Supplementary Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption in children with ASD and TD children. SSB, sugar-sweetened beverage; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typically developing.

Executive function of autistic children and typically developing children

The scores of BRIEF in autistic children and TD children are shown in Figure 2. In parent-report BRIEF, the mean (SD) score of GEC in autistic children was 66.3 (9.2), while that in TD children was 53.9 (8.6). Besides, the mean (SD) of BRI and MI in autistic children was 60.8 (10.5) and 67.9 (9.1), while that in TD children was 48.6 (7.8) and 56.8 (9.3), respectively. In behavioral experiment, the mean (SD) of correct rate of Stroop was 0.9 (0.1) in both the groups. And the mean (SD) of SI and WMI in autistic children was 401.1 (464.3) and 90.1 (18.8), while that in TD children was 407.9 (384.1) and 104.6 (12.5), respectively.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scores of BRIEF in ASD and TD children. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typically developing; SD, standard deviation; GEC, global executive component; BRI, behavioral regulation index; MI, metacognition index.

Association between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and executive function in autistic children

The association between SSB consumption and EF in autistic children is summarized in Tables 2, 3 and Supplementary Table 2. In model 3, autistic children who consumed more than two servings of SSB per week were associated with significantly higher scores of emotional control (β = 7.20, 95% CI: 0.94 to 13.46, p = 0.024) and plan/organize (β = 6.45, 95% CI: 0.27 to 12.63, p = 0.041), compared with those who consumed none. The results indicated that autistic children who have more than two servings of SSB per week might have worse performance in emotional control and plan/organize than those who did not drink SSB. There was no significant association between SSB consumption and EF among TD children (Tables 2, 3 and Supplementary Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Association between SSB consumption and scores of BRIEF among the two groups, and the comparison between the coefficient estimate (β) of the two groups.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association between SSB consumption and behavioral experiments among the two groups, and the comparison between the β of the two groups.

Comparison of the correlation between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and executive function in two groups

We further investigated the difference between the correlation (β) between SSB and EF in the two groups (Tables 2, 3). The relationship between >2 servings/week SSB consumption and emotional control in autistic children significantly differed from that in TD children (Z = 2.72, p = 0.006). And the correlation between >2 servings/week SSB consumption and BRI was significantly different from that in TD children (Z = 2.58, p = 0.010). Besides, the correlation between SSB consumption and other subscales of BRIEF in autistic children, as well as the correlation between SSB consumption and their behavioral experiment results had no significant difference compared with that in TD children.

Discussion

In the current study, about two-thirds of autistic children consumed SSB. About one in five autistic children consumed an average of two servings of SSB per week. Higher SSB consumption was associated with worse performance on the BRIEF subscale of emotional control and plan/organize in autistic children. In addition, we observed that the relationship between >2 servings/week SSB consumption and EF in autistic children was different from that in TD children. However, we did not find significant associations of SSB consumption with inhibition-control ability measured by the SCWT and WMI measured by WISC-IV test.

Some of our findings were consistent with previous studies among TD children. Most of the previous studies investigated the association between consumption of SSB and EF in TD children (68, 29). A randomized, cross-over study of 29 school-aged children assessed children’s EF by using a selection of tests from the Cognitive Drug Research (CDR) computerized assessment system (29). This study reported that the glucose drink was associated with great declines in attention and episodic secondary memory but had no association with working memory. In a cross-sectional analysis of over 6,000 children in Guangzhou, higher SSB consumption was associated with poorer performance in all the subscales and composite scores of parent-reported BRIEF (8). In addition, a cohort study of 1,234 children in the United States used Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT-II) to assess verbal and non-verbal global intelligence and the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning for visual memory (7). The results revealed that additional SSB consumption in early childhood was associated with poorer verbal intelligence at mid-childhood. These studies all indicated that SSB consumption had an association with impairment of EF in children. The findings may vary in the indexes or strength of the association due to different assessment of EF, adjusted covariates, study population, and methods in quantifying SSB consumption.

Although the previous studies indicated that SSB consumption was inversely related with EF in school-aged TD children, few studies investigated the relationship of SSB with EF in autistic children. In our study, we focused on autistic children in China. We assessed children’s EF with the SCWT, WMI of WISC-IV, and the Chinese version of the parent-reported BRIEF. Besides social demography and economic factors, we also adjusted for the screen time category, water intake, and IQ category, which had a relationship with children’s EF or SSB consumption (5052). After adjustment for the covariates, our findings showed that having more than two servings of SSB per week was associated with increasing scores of emotional control and plan/organize in autistic children. Emotional control subscale describes children’s ability to modulate emotional responses appropriately. Plan/organize subscale describes the ability to anticipate future events, set goals, and understand and communicate key concepts. This finding indicated that in autistic children, a higher level of SSB consumption might have an inverse association with performance on appropriately modulating emotional responses and anticipating future events. Findings of our study add to the literature on the adverse associations of SSB consumption and EF in autistic children. More studies from different regions are needed to confirm this relationship in this population. In addition, the association between emotional control and SSB consumption in autistic children was significantly different from that in TD children. Some previous studies indicated that sensory sensitivity was commonly found in autistic population (12, 53). This kind of sensory sensitivity was associated with autistic person’s intolerance of uncertainty and subsequent anxiety (53). In this study, autistic children might be more sensitive to a higher level of SSB consumption and performed worse in emotional control and BRI. The BRI also reflects the ability to modulate emotions. This finding indicated that autistic children’s emotional control ability might be more susceptible to high SSB consumption than that of TD children.

According to previous studies, the biological mechanism underlying the relationship between SSB consumption and EF is plausible. Most of these studies proposed that a high level of sucrose or fructose would increase inflammation and oxidative stress and decrease neurotrophins, which may be one of the pathways to explain the relationship. Animal evidence from general rat models indicated that 4 weeks of sucrose–fructose drinks feeding would induce increased expression of pro-inflammatory mediator genes such as IL-1β and IL-6 (22, 28). In the hypothalamus of sucrose–fructose fed rats, Toll-like receptor 4 (TLR4) as well as nuclear factor kappa-light-chain-enhancer of activated B cells (NF-κB) have higher levels than the control group. As for autism, many studies indicated that levels of brain cytokines, including IL-6 and IL-1β, were significantly higher in autistic children compared with TD controls (5459). Some studies demonstrated that brain IL-6 could mediate autism-like behaviors including heightened anxiety and deficits (60, 61). In an animal study, the researchers developed a mouse model that overexpresses IL-6 in the brain, and they discovered that the elevation of IL-6 in the mouse brain could produce autistic features, including impaired cognitive abilities, deficits in learning, decreased social interactions, as well as abnormal anxiety-like traits and habituation (60). Another study in 223 pre-school aged children indicated that stereotypical behavior and impaired social interactions in autistic children were more pronounced, as certain cytokine (IL-1β, IL-6, and IL-8) levels increased (62). Therefore, SSB consumption may further elevate the level of autistic children’s brain IL-6 and cause severer EF impairment. Compared with TD children, autistic children might be more sensitive to increased brain IL-6 level because of the original IL-6 dysregulation (62, 63). In addition, several studies indicated that IL-6 was associated with emotional problems (6470). Many studies found elevated IL-6 levels and its membrane-bound receptors in depressed individuals (6770). Therefore, we might be able to speculate that sucrose–fructose drinks could impair children’s EF by increasing the levels of brain IL-6 and IL-1β and this impairment would be greater in autistic children.

In this study, there were about one-fifth of autistic children who consumed no less than two servings of SSB per week, which contained about 50 g of sugar. However, children are advised to drink less or no SSB according to the current dietary guideline for Chinese (71) or Americans (72). Therefore, autistic children are suggested to decrease the consumption of SSB and improve adherence to nutrition guidelines. Parents, community, and schools should help these children to achieve better dietary quality. There were some limitations in this study. First, it is not possible to infer the causality of SSB consumption and EF based on the cross-sectional study design. Prospective cohort studies would be needed to confirm the long-term associations. Second, recall bias and information bias were inevitable because SSB consumption, children’s performance on EF, and demographic factors were based on parent-reported questionnaires in our study. However, the face-to-face interview and other quality control method may help us to reduce the recall bias. Third, although we adjusted for many individual confounders in the models, we cannot rule out the effects of other unmeasured potential confounding factors such as parental smoking and parental mental status.

Conclusion

We found that SSB consumption was inversely related to EF in autistic children, and the emotional control ability of autistic children might be more susceptible to SSB. These findings highlight the necessity of limiting autistic children’s SSB consumption.

Data availability statement

The generated datasets are available by request to the corresponding author JJ, jingjin@mail.sysu.edu.cn.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Review Committee for Biomedical Research, Sun Yat-sen University (2015-No.29). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

LC and JJ: conceptualization, supervision, project administration, and funding acquisition. SP: methodology and formal analysis, and writing—original draft preparation. XW and LC: study design and manuscript revision. SP, XW, JC, XZ, CJ, XO, and TG: data collection. XW: data curation. SP, XW, LL, JC, XZ, CJ, XO, TG, LC, and JJ: writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Key-Area Research and Development Program of Guangdong Province (2019B030335001), the Social-Area Science and Technology Research Program of Foshan City (2120001008276), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81872639 and 82103794).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the children and their parents for their kind support throughout the course of this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.940841/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Smirk E, Mazahery H, Conlon CA, Beck KL, Gammon C, Mugridge O, et al. Sugar-Sweetened beverages consumption among New Zealand children aged 8-12 years: A cross sectional study of sources and associates/correlates of consumption. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:2277. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-12345-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dai J, Soto MJ, Dunn CG, Bleich SN. Trends and patterns in sugar-sweetened beverage consumption among children and adults by race and/or ethnicity, 2003–2018. Public Health Nutr. (2021) 24:2405–10. doi: 10.1017/S1368980021001580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Xu X, Piao W, Fang H, Guo Q, Ju L, Cai S, et al. Beverage consumption of children and adolescents aged 6-17 years – China, 2016–2017. China CDC Wkly. (2021) 3:279–84. doi: 10.46234/ccdcw2021.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Li S, Cao M, Yang C, Zheng H, Zhu Y. Association of Sugar-sweetened beverage intake with risk of metabolic syndrome among children and adolescents in Urban China. Public Health Nutr. (2020) 23:2770–80. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019003653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Malik VS, Popkin BM, Bray GA, Despres JP, Willett WC, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of metabolic syndrome and type 2 diabetes: A meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. (2010) 33:2477–83. doi: 10.2337/dc10-1079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Reichelt AC, Killcross S, Hambly LD, Morris MJ, Westbrook RF. Impact of adolescent sucrose access on cognitive control, recognition memory, and parvalbumin immunoreactivity. Learn Mem. (2015) 22:215–24. doi: 10.1101/lm.038000.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cohen JFW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Young J, Oken E. Associations of prenatal and child sugar intake with child cognition. Am J Prev Med. (2018) 54:727–35. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gui Z, Huang S, Chen Y, Zhao Y, Jiang N, Zhang S, et al. Association between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and executive function in children. Nutrients. (2021) 13:4563. doi: 10.3390/nu13124563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Geng M, Jiang L, Wu X, Ding P, Liu W, Liu M, et al. Sugar-sweetened beverages consumption are associated with behavioral problems among preschoolers: A population based cross-sectional study in China. J Affect Disord. (2020) 265:519–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zhang Y, Wu X, Wang Q, Zong Q, Wang R, Li T, et al. The relationship between sugar-sweetened beverages, takeaway dietary pattern, and psychological and behavioral problems among children and adolescents in China. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:573168. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.573168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (Ef) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. (2002) 8:71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Lord C, Brugha TS, Charman T, Cusack J, Dumas G, Frazier T, et al. Autism spectrum disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2020) 6:5. doi: 10.1038/s41572-019-0138-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Seng GJ, Tseng WL, Chiu YN, Tsai WC, Wu YY, Gau SS. Executive functions in youths with autism spectrum disorder and their unaffected siblings. Psychol Med. (2021) 51:2571–80. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chen SF, Chien YL, Wu CT, Shang CY, Wu YY, Gau SS. Deficits in executive functions among youths with autism spectrum disorders: An age-stratified analysis. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:1625–38. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hutchison SM, Muller U, Iarocci G. Parent reports of executive function associated with functional communication and conversational skills among school age children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:2019–29. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03958-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lai CLE, Lau Z, Lui SSY, Lok E, Tam V, Chan Q, et al. Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2017) 10:911–39. doi: 10.1002/aur.1723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Costello SE, Geiser E, Schneider N. Nutrients for executive function development and related brain connectivity in school-aged children. Nutr Rev. (2021) 79:1293–306. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuaa134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sagiv SK, Kogut K, Gaspar FW, Gunier RB, Harley KG, Parra K, et al. Prenatal and childhood polybrominated diphenyl ether (pbde) exposure and attention and executive function at 9-12 years of age. Neurotoxicol Teratol. (2015) 52:151–61. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jirout J, LoCasale-Crouch J, Turnbull K, Gu Y, Cubides M, Garzione S, et al. How lifestyle factors affect cognitive and executive function and the ability to learn in children. Nutrients. (2019) 11:1953. doi: 10.3390/nu11081953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kvalo SE, Bru E, Bronnick K, Dyrstad SM. Does increased physical activity in school affect children’s executive function and aerobic fitness? Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2017) 27:1833–41. doi: 10.1111/sms.12856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Zhang H, Lee ZX, White T, Qiu A. Parental and social factors in relation to child psychopathology, behavior, and cognitive function. Transl Psychiatry. (2020) 10:80. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0761-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hsu TM, Konanur VR, Taing L, Usui R, Kayser BD, Goran MI, et al. Effects of sucrose and high fructose corn syrup consumption on spatial memory function and hippocampal neuroinflammation in adolescent rats. Hippocampus. (2015) 25:227–39. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Agrawal R, Noble E, Vergnes L, Ying Z, Reue K, Gomez-Pinilla F. Dietary fructose aggravates the pathobiology of traumatic brain injury by influencing energy homeostasis and plasticity. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. (2016) 36:941–53. doi: 10.1177/0271678X15606719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sanguesa G, Cascales M, Grinan C, Sanchez RM, Roglans N, Pallas M, et al. Impairment of novel object recognition memory and brain insulin signaling in fructose- but not glucose-drinking female rats. Mol Neurobiol. (2018) 55:6984–99. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0863-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Spagnuolo MS, Iossa S, Cigliano L. Sweet but bitter: Focus on fructose impact on brain function in rodent models. Nutrients. (2020) 13:1. doi: 10.3390/nu13010001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Xu MX, Yu R, Shao LF, Zhang YX, Ge CX, Liu XM, et al. Up-regulated fractalkine (Fkn) and its receptor Cx3cr1 are involved in fructose-induced neuroinflammation: Suppression by curcumin. Brain Behav Immun. (2016) 58:69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yin Q, Ma Y, Hong Y, Hou X, Chen J, Shen C, et al. Lycopene attenuates insulin signaling deficits, oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, and cognitive impairment in fructose-drinking insulin resistant rats. Neuropharmacology. (2014) 86:389–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Li JM, Ge CX, Xu MX, Wang W, Yu R, Fan CY, et al. Betaine recovers hypothalamic neural injury by inhibiting astrogliosis and inflammation in fructose-fed rats. Mol Nutr Food Res. (2015) 59:189–202. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.201400307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wesnes KA, Pincock C, Richardson D, Helm G, Hails S. Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite. (2003) 41:329–31. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2003.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cohen JF, Gorski MT, Gruber SA, Kurdziel LB, Rimm EB. The effect of healthy dietary consumption on executive cognitive functioning in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Br J Nutr. (2016) 116:989–1000. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516002877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tung SEH, Ch’ng YZ, Karnan TV, Chong PN, Zubaidah JO, Chin YS. Fluid Intake, hydration status and its association with cognitive function among adolescents in Petaling Perdana, Selangor, Malaysia. Nutr Res Pract. (2020) 14:490–500. doi: 10.4162/nrp.2020.14.5.490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D, Chou CP, Pentz MA. Relationships between executive cognitive function and lifetime substance use and obesity-related behaviors in fourth Grade Youth. Child Neuropsychol. (2012) 18:1–11. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.555759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wolraich MLLS, Stumbo PJ, Stegink LD, Appelbaum MI, Kiritsy MC. Effects of diets high in sucrose or aspartame on the behavior and cognitive performance of children. N Engl J Med. (1994) 330:301–7. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199402033300501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Taljaard C, Covic NM, van Graan AE, Kruger HS, Smuts CM, Baumgartner J, et al. Effects of a multi-micronutrient-fortified beverage, with and without sugar, on growth and cognition in South African schoolchildren: A randomised, double-blind, controlled intervention. Br J Nutr. (2013) 110:2271–84. doi: 10.1017/S000711451300189X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jones CRG, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Marsden AJS, Tregay J, et al. The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2018) 11:95–109. doi: 10.1002/aur.1873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Berenguer C, Rosello B, Colomer C, Baixauli I, Miranda A. Children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. relationships between symptoms and executive function, theory of mind, and behavioral problems. Res Dev Disabil. (2018) 83:260–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Buro AW, Gray HL, Kirby RS, Berkman K, Agazzi H, Shaffer-Hudkins E. Diet quality in an ethnically diverse sample of children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder compared with nationally representative data. Disabil Health J. (2021) 14:100981. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2020.100981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Canals-Sans J, Esteban-Figuerola P, Morales-Hidalgo P, Arija V. Do children with autism spectrum disorders eat differently and less adequately than those with subclinical Asd and typical development? Epined epidemiological study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:361–75. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04928-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Park HJ, Choi SJ, Kim Y, Cho MS, Kim YR, Oh JE. Mealtime behaviors and food preferences of students with autism spectrum disorder. Foods. (2020) 10:49. doi: 10.3390/foods10010049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bandini LG, Anderson SE, Curtin C, Cermak S, Evans EW, Scampini R, et al. Food selectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. J Pediatr. (2010) 157:259–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Baraskewich J, von Ranson KM, McCrimmon A, McMorris CA. Feeding and eating problems in children and adolescents with autism: A scoping review. Autism. (2021) 25:1505–19. doi: 10.1177/1362361321995631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Qian Y, Wang YF. Reliability and validity of behavior rating scale of executive function parent form for school age children in China. Beijing Da Xue Xue Bao Yi Xue Ban. (2007) 39:277–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

43. Stroop JR. Rnq1:studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. (1935) 18:643–62. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Zhang HC. The revision of Wisc-IV Chinese version. Psychol Sci. (2009) 32:1177–9. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2009.05.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lai L. J. Reliability and Validity of A Food Questionnaire of Chinese Surveillance on Students’ Constitution and Health in Primary School Students in Guangzhou. Ph.D. thesis, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou (2017).

Google Scholar

46. Lv YJ, Lai LJ, Li DL, Cai L, Zhu YN, Feng CY, et al. Study on the reliability and validity of physical health surveillance system questionnaire for primary school students in Guangzhou. Maternal Child Health Care China. (2020) 35:1511–6. doi: 10.19829/j.zgfybj.issn.1001-4411.2020.08.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. National Health and Family Planning Commission of People’s Republic of China. Screening for overweight and obesity among school-age children and adolescents, WS/T 586—2018. Beijing: National Health and Family Planning Commission of People’s Republic of China (2018).

Google Scholar

48. National Health and Family Planning Commission of People’s Republic of China. Screening standard for malnutrition of school-age children and adolescents, WS/T 456—2014. Beijing: National Health and Family Planning Commission of People’s Republic of China (2018).

Google Scholar

49. R Core Team. (2022). R A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

50. Khan NA, Raine LB, Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Cohen NJ, Kramer AF, et al. The relationship between total water intake and cognitive control among prepubertal children. Ann Nutr Metab. (2015) 66:38–41. doi: 10.1159/000381245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Laurent JS, Watts R, Adise S, Allgaier N, Chaarani B, Garavan H, et al. Associations among body mass index, cortical thickness, and executive function in children. JAMA Pediatr. (2020) 174:170–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Warren C, Riggs N, Pentz MA. Executive function mediates prospective relationships between sleep duration and sedentary behavior in children. Prev Med. (2016) 91:82–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Normansell-Mossa KM, Top DN Jr., Russell N, Freeston M, Rodgers J, South M. Sensory sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty influence anxiety in autistic adults. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:731753. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.731753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Vargas DL, Nascimbene C, Krishnan C, Zimmerman AW, Pardo CA. Neuroglial Activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann Neurol. (2005) 57:67–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.20315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Li X, Chauhan A, Sheikh AM, Patil S, Chauhan V, Li XM, et al. Elevated immune response in the brain of autistic patients. J Neuroimmunol. (2009) 207:111–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2008.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Singh VK, Warren RP, Odell JD, Cole P. Changes of soluble interleukin-2, interleukin-2 receptor, T8 antigen, and interleukin-1 in the serum of autistic children. Clin Immunol Immunopathol. (1991) 61:448–55. doi: 10.1016/s0090-1229(05)80015-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Schwarz E, Guest PC, Rahmoune H, Wang L, Levin Y, Ingudomnukul E, et al. Sex-specific serum biomarker patterns in adults with asperger’s syndrome. Mol Psychiatry. (2011) 16:1213–20. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Croonenberghs J, Bosmans E, Deboutte D, Kenis G, Maes M. Activation of the inflammatory response system in autism. Neuropsychobiology. (2002) 45:1–6. doi: 10.1159/000048665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Zimmerman AW, Jyonouchi H, Comi AM, Connors SL, Milstien S, Varsou A, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid and serum markers of inflammation in autism. Pediatr Neurol. (2005) 33:195–201. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Wei H, Chadman KK, McCloskey DP, Sheikh AM, Malik M, Brown WT, et al. Brain Il-6 elevation causes neuronal circuitry imbalances and mediates autism-like behaviors. Biochim Biophys Acta. (2012) 1822:831–42. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2012.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Smith SE, Li J, Garbett K, Mirnics K, Patterson PH. Maternal immune activation alters Fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:10695–702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2178-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ashwood P, Krakowiak P, Hertz-Picciotto I, Hansen R, Pessah I, Van de Water J. Elevated plasma cytokines in autism spectrum disorders provide evidence of immune dysfunction and are associated with impaired behavioral outcome. Brain Behav Immun. (2011) 25:40–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Emanuele E, Orsi P, Boso M, Broglia D, Brondino N, Barale F, et al. Low-grade endotoxemia in patients with severe autism. Neurosci Lett. (2010) 471:162–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Vojtechova I, Petrasek T, Maleninska K, Brozka H, Tejkalova H, Horacek J, et al. Neonatal immune activation by lipopolysaccharide causes inadequate emotional responses to novel situations but no changes in anxiety or cognitive behavior in wistar rats. Behav Brain Res. (2018) 349:42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Peters AT, Ren X, Bessette KL, George N, Kling LR, Thies B, et al. Inflammation, depressive symptoms, and emotion perception in adolescence. J Affect Disord. (2021) 295:717–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. La Fratta I, Tatangelo R, Campagna G, Rizzuto A, Franceschelli S, Ferrone A, et al. The plasmatic and salivary levels of Il-1beta, Il-18 and Il-6 are associated to emotional difference during stress in young male. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:3031. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21474-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Strawbridge R, Arnone D, Danese A, Papadopoulos A, Herane Vives A, Cleare AJ. Inflammation and Clinical response to treatment in depression: A meta-analysis. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2015) 25:1532–43. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Rizavi HS, Ren X, Zhang H, Bhaumik R, Pandey GN. Abnormal gene expression of proinflammatory cytokines and their membrane-bound receptors in the lymphocytes of depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 240:314–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Kohler CA, Freitas TH, Maes M, de Andrade NQ, Liu CS, Fernandes BS, et al. Peripheral cytokine and chemokine alterations in depression: A meta-analysis of 82 studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2017) 135:373–87. doi: 10.1111/acps.12698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Dowlati Y, Herrmann N, Swardfager W, Liu H, Sham L, Reim EK, et al. A meta-analysis of cytokines in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 67:446–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. China National Society. Dietary guidelines for Chinese residents, 2022. Beijing: People’s Medical Publishing House (2022).

Google Scholar

72. USDA. Dietary guidelines for Americans, 2020–2025. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture (2020).

Google Scholar

Keywords: sugar-sweetened beverage, children, autism spectrum disorder, executive function, cross-sectional study

Citation: Pan S, Wang X, Lin L, Chen J, Zhan X, Jin C, Ou X, Gu T, Jing J and Cai L (2022) Association of sugar-sweetened beverages with executive function in autistic children. Front. Nutr. 9:940841. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.940841

Received: 10 May 2022; Accepted: 20 July 2022;
Published: 22 August 2022.

Edited by:

Fenghua Sun, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Reviewed by:

Lu Qin, Penn State Milton S. Hershey Medical Center, United States
Eric Tsz-Chun Poon, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Copyright © 2022 Pan, Wang, Lin, Chen, Zhan, Jin, Ou, Gu, Jing and Cai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Li Cai, caili5@mail.sysu.edu.cn; Jin Jing, jingjin@mail.sysu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.