- 1Key Laboratory of Adolescent Health Assessment and Exercise Intervention of Ministry of Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
- 2College of Physical Education and Health, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
- 3College of Economics and Management, Shanghai Institute of Technology, Shanghai, China
- 4Lhasa Beijing Experimental Middle School, Lhasa, China
Objective: To estimate the association between sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) consumption and executive function (EF) among Chinese Tibetan adolescents.
Method: Using three stages by stratified cluster sampling, 1,427 Chinese Tibetan adolescents were recruited from Tibet, China. SSB consumption status was obtained by questionnaires and the three core EFs (inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) were tested by a modified Eriksen flanker task, N-back shift, and a more-odd shifting task. One-way ANOVA or Chi-square test was used to compare SSB consumption in different categories. Taking the SSB consumption 0 time/week group as the reference, general linear regression (for continuous variable) or Logical regression (classified variable) in three Models was conducted to analyze the relationship between SSB consumption and EF for Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents.
Result: After adjustment of all the covariant in Model 2, all the EF indexes were higher in Chinese Tibetan adolescents with SSB consumption ≥2 times/week than that with SSB consumption of 0 times/week by 21.33 ms (95%CI: 6.72, 35.93), 8.21 ms (95%CI: 7.06, 9.35), 90.46 ms (95%CI: 28.69, 152.23), 147.61 ms (95%CI: 81.42, 213.80), 116.18 ms (95%CI: 74.48, 157.87), 112.41 ms (95%CI: 71.30, 153.52) for incongruent RT, RT difference in incongruent and congruent, 1-back RT, 2-back RT, Heterogeneous RT, RT difference in Heterogeneous and Homogeneous respectively.
Conclusions: The results suggested that SSB consumption was associated with poorer EF in Chinese Tibetan adolescents. SSB consumption should be controlled for healthy brain development of Chinese Tibetan adolescents.
Introduction
Located on the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau, Xizang (Tibet) is a remote and low-income area in China, with GDP ranking last in 2021 (1). According to the sixth National census, there are 6.5 million permanent Tibetan residents on the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau, of whom 15% live between 2,500 and 3,000 meters above sea level, 75% live between 3,000 and 4,000 m above sea level, and 10% live above 4,000 m (2). With an average high of 4,000 m above sea level, Chinese Tibetan adolescents are suffering from hypoxia, which affected their body and brain development, including executive function (EF) (3).
Researches on EF has flourished since 2000 in developmental psychology (4). Considered as “air traffic control system of the brain,” EF is a collection of top-down control processes used when going on automatic or relying on instinct or intuition would be ill-advised, insufficient, or impossible, such as attentional control, working memory, inhibition, and problem-solving (5). Several studies have found concurrent or longitudinal relations between children's EF and diverse skills, including academic achievement (6), social (7), logical (8), and biological reasoning (9, 10). Several studies have suggested the poor EF of adolescents at high altitudes because of the sensitivity of the brain to environmental hypoxia (11, 12). To our best knowledge, we did not find the data on EF for Chinese Tibetan adolescents in Tibet, China.
As one of the factors influencing EF and underlying brain developmental processes in children, a balanced diet may provide an effective way to promote EF (13). Sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB), a class of very popular non-alcoholic beverages throughout the world, are characterized by high added sugar content, especially fructose-containing sugar (14). SSB was considered to be an important risk factor for obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease, mortality, and certain cancers worldwide (15, 16). In childhood, evidence supports links between SSB consumption and unhealthy weight gain, as well as other untoward health outcomes, such as dental caries, the earlier timing of puberty, higher blood pressure, and hyperactivity/inattention symptoms (17).
In recent decades, the global production and consumption of SSB have been increasing (18). SSB consumption has declined or plateaued in most western high-income countries since the early 2000s, while in many low-income and middle-income countries, the intake of SSB consumption is increasing, as widespread urbanization and economic development have increased the availability of these beverages (19, 20). In China, it was reported that the production of SSB exceeded 180 million tons in 2017, which was 440 times that of 1992 (21). Compared with 2014, the proportion of SSB non-drinkers among Chinese Tibetan primary school students in Lhasa decreased from 15.16 to 2.47 % in 2019 (22), which was lower than their peers in South China (34.7%) (23). Meanwhile, racial/ethnic disparities in SSB consumption were observed in multi-ethnic areas (24, 25). Verzeletti also emphasized that the ethnic background differences may have an impact on parental beliefs regarding the child's SSB consumption or on rules restricting the intake of SSB by the child (26).
Evidence from a systematic review suggested that less-healthy foods such as SSB were inversely associated with EF (27). The limited studies focusing on this topic has confirmed the association between SSB consumption and poorer EF among children in South China (28). Besides, population-based studies also observed associations between SSB and declined intelligence (29), and poor poorer cognitive performance (30, 31). Nevertheless, the association between SSB consumption and EF among Chinese Tibetan adolescents has been rarely investigated. Given the large population of Chinese Tibetan, the present study aimed to estimate the association between SSB consumption and EF among Chinese Tibetan adolescents.
Methods
Data Sources and Participants Recruitment
Data were obtained from a cross-sectional study of Chinese Tibetan adolescents in Tibet, China and the research was conducted from August 2019 to December 2020. The participants in this study should be: (1) Chinese Tibetan middle school students with IQ > 90 according to the Wechsler intelligence scale; (2) without physical disability. (3) No color blindness or color weakness; (4) right-handed (32, 33); (5) without depression, anxiety, and other adverse psychological emotions. (6) Born and grew up in Tibet, China with their parents as indigenous Chinese Tibetan.
The recruitment procedure includes three stages by stratified cluster sampling. Firstly, according to altitude, population distribution, geographical distribution, and the situation of economic development of Tibet in China, three cities (Lhasa, Nyingchi, and Nagqu) were selected (Figure 1). Secondly, four middle schools in each city were randomly selected for investigation. Thirdly, taking class as the smallest unit of cluster sampling, one class was randomly selected from each grade in each school. All the eligible students in the class were recruited as participants. A total of 1,427 Chinese Tibetan adolescents were recruited and 196 data were excluded because of missing values. Finally, 1,231 data were effective for the present study (86.26%; Figure 2).
This study was approved by the Human Experimental Ethics Committee of the East China Normal University (Approval No.: HR0782020). Written informed consent was obtained from the students and their parents before the study. The names of participants were coded to protect their privacy.
Procedure
Before the test, all the team members, composed of graduate students majoring in human sports science, were required to be trained until they were qualified for the test. A self-administered questionnaire collecting demographic information, data on SSB, and dietary intake was completed by the adolescents independently over a 40-min period in classrooms after school. To make sure each item of the questionnaire fully understood, the questionnaire was translated into Tibetan. The team members explained every item in detail to students when they administered the questionnaires and were available all the time for any questions raised by students. When completed, the questionnaires were withdrawn on the spot.
Sugar-Sweetened Beverage
Data on SSB consumption were obtained by questionnaires extracted from the Chinese National Survey on Students' Constitution and Health (CNSSCH) (34) and its validation and reliability have been confirmed in previous studies (35, 36). The SSB consumption was assessed by asking them the frequency of SSB consumption from the question “In the past 7 days, how many times did you drink SSB such as Coke, Sprite, Natural fruit juices, Nutrition Express, Red Bull?” (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, ≥5). The frequency of SSB consumption was aggregated and categorized into three groups (0 time/week, 1 time/week, and ≥2 times/week) to ensure an adequate number of participants in each group.
Executive Function
In the present study, three core EFs (inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) were tested on the computer by a test tool developed by Chen et al. (37), which has been used in many previous studies (38–41). A modified Eriksen flanker task was used to assess the inhibition aspect of EF (42), and the response times (RT, ms) in the congruent and incongruent trials were recorded as result. The RT difference between incongruent and congruent trials was used as an index of inhibition. Shorter RT and RT differences between incongruent and congruent indicated better performance. The working memory aspect of EF was assessed by 1-back and 2-back tasks. The RT on correct trials was recorded as result and shorter RT reflected better performance. Adapted from Hillman et al. (43) and Salthouse et al. (44), a more-odd shifting task was employed to assess the cognitive flexibility aspect of EF. The RT difference between the heterogeneous and homogeneous blocks was used to estimate cognitive flexibility. Each test was conducted according to the details introduced in the previous paper (45). The stimulus presentation and response data collection were conducted using E-Prime software 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, USA).
The test was conducted in a computer classroom with at least one Chinese Tibetan teacher present for better communication. Before the test, the Chinese Tibetan teachers, who in charge of the test, were trained uniformly until they were qualified. A video explaining how to do the test was also played before the test to make the students better understand until the students were familiar with the test.
Covariant
Results from many studies have demonstrated that EF can be affected by obesity rate (46–48), dietary intake (49), physical activity (50), and VO2max (51, 52). Therefore, the present study took sociodemographic and dietary intake information, BMI, WC, Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity (MVPA), and VO2max as covariant.
Sociodemographic information including age (years), sex (boys or girls), siblings (0 or ≥1), father's and mother's education (without education,elementary school,junior middle school, senior middle school, college, or above) was obtained by questionnaires. Information on dietary intake was also collected by questionnaires separately with three questions “In the past 7 days, how many times did you have breakfast? (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7)”; “In the past 7 days, how many times did you have eggs or egg products? (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7)”; “In the past 7 days, how many times did you have milk or dairy products? (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7)”. Physical activity status was obtained by the following two questions: “In the past 7 days, how many times did you have physical activity (Moderate-intensity physical activity and Vigorous-intensity physical activity) on school days and weekends, respectively?” The students were asked to respond in the blanks. Moderate-intensity physical activity refers to activity that requires moderate effort, and makes you feel a little breathless, sweaty, or tired, such as cycling at normal speed, brisk walking, skating, etc. Vigorous-intensity physical activity refers to activity that requires a lot of effort, feeling breathless, sweaty, or very tiring, such as heavy lifting, running fast, playing with balls hardly, or cycling fast. If they answered more than 0 times, they were further asked about the duration each time, “On average, how long does each activity last?” Height, weight, and waist circumference (WC) were measured with participants lightly dressed and barely feet. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (in kilograms) over height (meters) square. The VO2max was estimated by 20 m SRT and the details measurement of 20 m SRT was provided in our previous study (53). All the instruments were calibrated to ensure the accuracy of the test. The test was carried out at the same time every day to reduce the deviation.
Statistical Analyses
For the continuous variables, mean and standard deviation (M ± SD) were used to express the result, and One-way ANOVA was used to compare SSB consumption in different categories. For the classified variable, data were expressed by percentage, and the Chi-square test was used to compare SSB consumption in different categories.
Taking the SSB consumption 0 time/week group as the reference, general linear regression (for continuous variable) or Logical regression (classified variable) was conducted to analyze the relationship between SSB consumption and EF for Chinese Tibetan adolescents. Three models (Crude Model, Model 1, Model 2)were used: Crude Model was conducted without adjustment; Model 1 was conducted after adjusting age, sex, siblings, parental education, BMI, WC, MVPA, and VO2max; Based on Model 1, Model 2 included breakfast, egg or egg products, and milk or dairy-products as additional control variables. We set dummy variables for SSB consumption and took them as continuous variables to estimate the dose-response relationship between SSB consumption and EF. The mean and standard deviation (SD) of RT for the three core EFs were calculated by age and sex. Executive dysfunction was defined as ≥1 SD from the mean.
All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (version 25.0; IBM Inc., Armonk, NY) and GraphPad Prism 8.0.2 (GraphPad Software, Inc., CA). The level of statistical significance was set at a two-tailed P <0.05.
Result
Among 1,231 Chinese Tibetan adolescents aged 13–18 from Tibet of China, 585 (47.5%) were boys with an average age of (15.77 ± 1.68) years. There are 634 (51.50%) adolescents with SSB consumption 1 time/week, and 409 (33.23%) adolescents with SSB consumption ≥2 time/week. Compared with non-SSB consumption, adolescents with SSB consumption ≥1 time/week have lower parental education, fewer breakfast times per week, higher BMI and waist circumference, and lower MVPA and VO2max levels (P < 0.05; Table 1).
All the EF index (including congruent RT, incongruent RT, RT difference in incongruent and congruent, 1-back RT, 2-back RT, Heterogeneous RT, Homogeneous RT, RT difference in Heterogeneous and Homogeneous) of Chinese Tibetan adolescents with different SSB consumption are significantly different (F = 20.44, 29.32, 174.78, 20.72, 43.87, 67.25, 9.77, and 60.23, respectively, P < 0.001; Table 2). Compared with non-SSB consumption, adolescents with SSB consumption ≥1 time/week have a longer reaction time, that is, the worse performance of EF (Table 2).
Table 2. The status of executive function for Chinese Tibetan adolescents with different SSB consumption in Tibet of China.
There is a significant correlation between SSB consumption and EF for Chinese Tibetan adolescents in Tibet of China (Table 3). After adjustment in Model 2, all the EF index were higher in Chinese Tibetan adolescents with SSB consumption ≥2 time/week than that with SSB consumption 0 time/week by 21.33 ms (95%CI: 6.72, 35.93), 8.21 ms(95%CI: 7.06, 9.35), 90.46 ms (95%CI: 28.69, 152.23), 147.61 ms (95%CI: 81.42, 213.80), 116.18 ms (95%CI: 74.48, 157.87), 112.41 ms (95%CI: 71.30, 153.52) for incongruent RT, RT difference in incongruent and congruent, 1-back RT, 2-back RT, Heterogeneous RT, RT difference in Heterogeneous and Homogeneous, respectively (P <0.05).
Table 3. The multiple linear regression of executive function for Chinese Tibetan adolescents with different SSB consumption (n = 1,231).
Table 4 shows the logistic regression of executive dyfunction for Chinese Tibetan adolescents with different SSB consumption. After adding additional control variables in Model 2, Chinese Tibetan adolescents with SSB consumption ≥2 times/week perform poorer on the three core EFs (inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility) than that with SSB consumption 0 time/week [OR = 5.91, (95%CI: 2.78, 12.59)], [2.98, (95%CI: 1.40, 6.34)], [2.80, (95% CI:1.16, 6.74)], respectively (P <0.05).
Table 4. The logistic regression of executive function for Chinese Tibetan adolescents with different SSB consumption (n = 1,231).
Discussion
The present study cross-sectionally analyzed the association between SSB consumption and EF in Chinese Tibetan adolescents in high-altitude areas of China. The result showed that the SSB consumption of Chinese Tibetan adolescents in high-altitude areas was related to the poor performance of the EF. After adjusting for demographic factors, sociodemographic information, dietary behaviors, and some physical status, there was still a significant correlation between SSB consumption and EF in Chinese Tibetan adolescents. At the same time, our study also observed that SSB consumption was associated with a higher risk of executive dysfunction.
Our study showed that 84.73% of Chinese Tibetan adolescents consumed at least one time of SSB in the past week, which was higher than American adolescents (60.7%) (54). Over the past decades, excessive SSB consumption is spreading into low and middle-income countries, leading to an increase in chronic non-communicable diseases, cancer, and all-cause mortality, bringing a huge medical burden to the country (55, 56). It was reported that SSB consumption of children and adolescents was associated with parental SSB consumption patterns and accessibility of SSB consumption (57), frequency of fast-food consumption (58), and time spent watching television or viewing advertisements (59). Rodent studies suggest that SSB consumption may activate a glucocorticoid-metabolic-brain-negative feedback pathway, which may turn off the stress response and thereby reinforce habitual SSB overconsumption (60). Hence, targeted efforts such as taxing SSB and increasing knowledge of SSB are needed to reduce intake of SSB consumption are needed.
Similar to our findings, Gui et al. also found out that SSB consumption was associated with poorer EF among children in Guangzhou, China, and the global executive index of children with SSB consumption ≥2 times/week increased by 1.62 times compared with non-SSB consumption children (28). A study of children and adolescents in the United States showed that an increase in daily SSB consumption was associated with a 2.4-point decline in the intelligence of children and adolescents as assessed by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (29). Though the association was observed between SSB consumption and EF, the bi-directional associations between them remain unclear. Obesity researchers emphasize the significance of executive-control systems for explaining the occurrence of non-homeostatic forms of dietary behavior and modulating cravings for and consumption of high-calorie foods (61). While research from other disciplines suggested SSB was inversely associated with EF (27). Hence, more longitudinal studies are needed to explore the causal relationship between them.
A systematic review suggested that the relation between EF and dietary intake is equivocal (49). In addition, a study focused on school-aged children aged 8–10 years in the United States found no association between SSB consumption and working memory, academic performance, cognition, and inhibitory control (62). The wide variety of measures used to assess EF and dietary intake may play in the relation between EF and dietary intake, making the overall interpretation of the literature more complicated. For example, EF can be used by questionnaires such as Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function or computerized tests (Computerized Dots Task, Computerized Neuro-psychological Test). Dietary intake can be measured by Food Frequency Questionnaire or Lab-Based Food Task. Besides, the non-uniform covariant may also affect the result. In sum, the relation between EF and SSB needs to be further confirmed.
This study has some strengths and limitations. The strength is that we controlled several covariant such as sociodemographic information (age, sex, siblings, father's and mother's education), information on dietary intakes (eggs, milk, breakfast), and physical status (BMI, WC, MVPA, VO2max,). However, the cross-sectional analysis of the present study can't decide a causal relationship. A prospective cohort study is needed in the future. Besides, we used self-report SSB consumption, which are inevitably affected by the recall ability. Meanwhile, the information on volumes of SSBs consumption by adolescents were not collected. At last, the impact of confounding variables on EF was not included in the study.
Conclusions
In conclusion, this study analyzed the relationship between SSB consumption and EF for Chinese Tibetan adolescents at high altitudes and concluded that SSB consumption is associated with poorer EF performance and executive dysfunction. Given the large population of Chinese Tibetan and the brain damage caused by hypoxia at high altitudes, it is necessary to make targeted efforts to reduce SSB consumption of Chinese Tibetan adolescents in high altitude, such as taxing SSB, increasing knowledge of SSB, or environmental interventions that alter the physical or social environment in which individuals make beverage choices. Longitudinal studies and clinical trials are further needed to clarify the direction of causality and to investigate the underlying mechanism.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without unduereservation.
Ethics Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Human Experimental Ethics Committee of the East China Normal University (Approval No.: HR0782020). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: FZ, XY, and CB. Methodology: YL and ML. Validation, supervision, and funding acquisition: XY. Formal analysis and visualization: FZ. Investigation: FZ and ML. Resources, writing—original draft preparation, and project administration: ML. Data curation: XG. Writing—review and editing: FZ and YL. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the Shanghai Planning Project of Philosophy and Social Science (Award No.: 2020BTY001).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank all the participants and their parents for their cooperation in our research.
References
1. Chinese National Bureau of Statistics. Available online at: https://data.stats.gov.cn/easyquery.htm?cn=E0103 (accessed May 7, 2022).
2. The Population Census Office of the State Council of China. The Sixth National Population Census of China. (2010). Available online at: http://www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/pcsj/rkpc/6rp/indexch.htm (accessed May 09, 2022).
3. Hackett PH, Roach RC. High-altitude illness. N Engl J Med. (2001) 345:107–14. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200107123450206
4. Doebel S. Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2020) 15:942–56. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904771
5. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
6. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn Individ Differ. (2011) 21:327–36. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007
7. Wolf S, McCoy DC. The role of executive function and social-emotional skills in the development of literacy and numeracy during preschool: a cross-lagged longitudinal study. Dev Sci. (2019) 22:e12800. doi: 10.1111/desc.12800
8. Doebel S, Rowell SF, Koenig MA. Young children detect and avoid logically inconsistent sources: the importance of communicative context and executive function. Child Dev. (2016) 87:1956–70. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12563
9. Zaitchik D, Iqbal Y, Carey S. The effect of executive function on biological reasoning in young children: an individual differences study. Child Dev. (2014) 85:160–75. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12145
10. Richland LE, Burchinal MR. Early executive function predicts reasoning development. Psychol Sci. (2013) 24:87–92. doi: 10.1177/0956797612450883
11. Yan X, Zhang J, Gong Q, Weng X. Adaptive influence of long term high altitude residence on spatial working memory: an fMRI study. Brain Cogn. (2011) 77:53–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.06.002
12. Jansen GF, Krins A, Basnyat B, Odoom JA, Ince C. Role of the altitude level on cerebral autoregulation in residents at high altitude. J Appl Physiol. (2007) 103:518–23. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01429.2006
13. Costello SE, Geiser E, Schneider N. Nutrients for executive function development and related brain connectivity in school-aged children. Nutr Rev. (2021) 79:1293–306. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuaa134
14. Hu D, Cheng L, Jiang W. Sugar-sweetened beverages consumption and the risk of depression: a meta-analysis of observational studies. J Affect Disord. (2019) 245:348–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.015
15. Malik VS, Hu FB. The role of sugar-sweetened beverages in the global epidemics of obesity and chronic diseases. Nat Rev Endocrinol. (2022) 18:205–18. doi: 10.1038/s41574-021-00627-6
16. Qin P, Li Q, Zhao Y, Chen Q, Sun X, Liu Y. Sugar and artificially sweetened beverages and risk of obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, and all-cause mortality: a dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. (2020) 35:655–71. doi: 10.1007/s10654-020-00655-y
17. Scharf RJ, DeBoer MD. Sugar-sweetened beverages and children's health. Annu Rev Public Health. (2016) 37:273–93. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032315-021528
18. Pan American Health Organization. Pan American Health Organization Nutrient Profile Model.Washington, DC: Pan American Health Organization (2016), p. 11.
19. Malik VS, Willett WC, Hu FB. Global obesity: trends, risk factors and policy implications. Nat Rev Endocrinol. (2013) 9:13–27. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2012.199
20. Singh GM, Micha R, Khatibzadeh S, Lim S, Ezzati M, Mozaffarian D. Estimated global, regional, and national disease burdens related to sugar-sweetened beverage consumption in 2010. Circulation. (2015) 132:639–66. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.114.010636
21. China Beverage Industry Association. 2018 China Beverage Industry Sustainable Development Report. Beijing: China Beverage Industry Association (2018).
22. Wang H, Shen W, Degyi Y. Investigation and analysis of dietary behavior of Tibetan primary school students in Lhasa from 2014 and 2019. Chin J Sch Health. (2020) 41:998–1004. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2020.07.011
23. He B, Long W, Li X, Yang W, Chen Y, Zhu Y. Sugar-sweetened beverages consumption positively associated with the risks of obesity and hypertriglyceridemia among children aged 7-18 years in South China. J Atheroscler Thromb. (2018) 25:81–9. doi: 10.5551/jat.38570
24. Roesler A, Rojas N, Falbe J. Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption, perceptions, and disparities in children and adolescents. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2021) 53:553–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2021.04.004
25. van de Gaar VM, van Grieken A, Jansen W, Raat H. Children's sugar-sweetened beverages consumption: associations with family and home-related factors, differences within ethnic groups explored. BMC Public Health. (2017) 17:195. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4095-0
26. Verzeletti C, Maes L, Santinello M, Vereecken CA. Soft drink consumption in adolescence: associations with food-related lifestyles and family rules in Belgium Flanders and the Veneto Region of Italy. Eur J Public Health. (2010) 20:312–7. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp150
27. Cohen JF, Gorski MT, Gruber SA, Kurdziel LB, Rimm EB. The effect of healthy dietary consumption on executive cognitive functioning in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Br J Nutr. (2016) 116:989–1000. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516002877
28. Chen Y. Association between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and executive function in children. Nutrients. (2021) 13:4563. doi: 10.3390/nu13124563
29. Cohen JFW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Young J, Oken E. Associations of prenatal and child sugar intake with child cognition. Am J Prev Med. (2018) 54:727–35. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.02.020
30. Crichton GE, Elias MF, Torres RV. Sugar-sweetened soft drinks are associated with poorer cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes: the Maine-Syracuse Longitudinal Study. Br J Nutr. (2016) 115:1397–405. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516000325
31. Chong CP, Shahar S, Haron H, Din NC. Habitual sugar intake and cognitive impairment among multi-ethnic Malaysian older adults. Clin Interv Aging. (2019) 14:1331–42. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S211534
32. Hatta T. Associations between handedness and executive function in upper-middle-aged people. Laterality. (2018) 23:274–89. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2017.1358273
33. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
34. Association CNSSCH. Report on the 2014th National Survey on Students' Constitution and Health. Beijing: China College & University Press (2016).
35. Dong Y, Chen M, Chen L, Wen B, Yang Y, Wang Z. Individual-, family-, and school-level ecological correlates with physical fitness among chinese school-aged children and adolescents: a National Cross-Sectional Survey in 2014. Front Nutr. (2021) 8:684286. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.684286
36. Yang Z, Li Y, Hu P, Ma J, Song Y. Prevalence of anemia and its associated factors among Chinese 9-, 12-, and 14-year-old children: results from 2014 Chinese National Survey on Students Constitution and Health. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1474. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051474
37. Chen A, Jiang R, Ji X, Tao B, Zhu F, Yan J. Effects of 8-week moderate fancy rope skipping training on executive function in preadolescent deaf children: a school-based experimental study. J Sports Sci. (2015) 36:105–9. doi: 10.13598/j.issn1004-4590.2015.04.017
38. Xiong X, Zhu LN, Dong XX, Wang W, Yan J, Chen AG.. Aerobic exercise intervention alters executive function and white matter integrity in deaf children: a randomized controlled study. Neural Plast. (2018) 2018:3735208. doi: 10.1155/2018/3735208
39. Zhu L, Yu Q, Herold F, Cheval B, Dong X, Cui L. Brain structure, cardiorespiratory fitness, and executive control changes after a 9-week exercise intervention in young adults: a randomized controlled trial. Life. (2021) 11:292. doi: 10.3390/life11040292
40. Zhu H, Zhu L, Xiong X, Dong X, Chen D, Wang J. Influence of aerobic fitness on white matter integrity and inhibitory control in early adulthood: a 9-week exercise intervention. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:1080. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11081080
41. Si J, Zhang H, Zhu L, Chen A. The relationship between overweight/obesity and executive control in college students: the mediating effect of BDNF and 5-HT. Life. (2021) 11:313. doi: 10.3390/life11040313
42. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys. (1974) 16:143–9. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
43. Hillman CH, Kramer AF, Belopolsky AV, Smith DP. A cross-sectional examination of age and physical activity on performance and event-related brain potentials in a task switching paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol. (2006) 59:30–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.04.009
44. Salthouse TA, Fristoe N, McGuthry KE, Hambrick DZ. Relation of task switching to speed, age, and fluid intelligence. Psychol Aging. (1998) 13:445–61. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.3.445
45. Chen A-G, Yan J, Yin H-C, Pan C-Y, Chang Y-K. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on multiple aspects of executive function in preadolescent children. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2014) 15:627–636. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.06.004
46. Pearce AL, Leonhardt CA, Vaidya CJ. Executive and reward-related function in pediatric obesity: a meta-analysis. Child Obes. (2018) 14:265–79. doi: 10.1089/chi.2017.0351
47. Gardener H, Caunca M, Dong C, Cheung YK, Rundek T, Elkind MSV. Obesity measures in relation to cognition in the Northern Manhattan Study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2020) 78:1653–60. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201071
48. B Bugge A, Möller S, Westfall DR, Tarp J, Gejl AK, Wedderkopp N, et al. Associations between waist circumference, metabolic risk and executive function in adolescents: a cross-sectional mediation analysis. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0199281. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199281
49. Egbert AH, Creber C, Loren DM, Bohnert AM. Executive function and dietary intake in youth: A systematic review of the literature. Appetite. (2019) 139:197–212. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.04.013
50. Li L, Zhang J, Cao M, Hu W, Zhou T, Huang T. The effects of chronic physical activity interventions on executive functions in children aged 3-7 years: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. (2020) 23:949–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.03.007
51. Holmen TL, Egeland J, Andersen E, Bigseth TT, Engh JA. The association between cardio-respiratory fitness and cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2018) 193:418–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.015
52. Zhan Z, Ai J, Ren F, Li L, Chu CH, Chang YK. Cardiorespiratory fitness, age, and multiple aspects of executive function among preadolescent children. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01198
53. Zhang F, Yin X, Bi C, Li Y, Sun Y, Zhang T. Normative reference values and international comparisons for the 20-metre shuttle run test: analysis of 69,960 test results among Chinese Children and Youth. J Sports Sci Med. (2020) 19:478–88. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84634-7
54. Rosinger A, Herrick K, Gahche J, Park S. Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption among U.S. Adults, 2011-2014. NCHS Data Brief . (2017) 271:1–8.
55. Jacob L, Stubbs B, Koyanagi A. Consumption of carbonated soft drinks and suicide attempts among 105, 061 adolescents aged 12-15 years from 6 high-income, 22 middle-income, and 4 low-income countries. Clin Nut. (2020) 39:886–92. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.03.028
56. Micha R, Khatibzadeh S, Shi P, Andrews KG, Engell RE, Mozaffarian D. Global, regional, and national consumption levels of dietary fats and oils in 1990 and 2010: a systematic analysis including 266 country-specific nutrition surveys. BMJ. (2015) 350:g2272. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g2272
57. Vereecken CA. Influence of mother's educational level on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite. (2004) 43:93–103. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(04)00043-1
58. French SA, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D, Fulkerson JA, Hannan P. Fast food restaurant use among adolescents: associations with nutrient intake, food choices and behavioral and psychosocial variables. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2001) 25:1823–33. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801820
59. Miller SA, Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Gillman MW. Association between television viewing and poor diet quality in young children. Int J Pediatr Obes. (2011) 3:168–76. doi: 10.1080/17477160801915935
60. Grace FM, Hayes LD, Nicholas S. Letter to the Editor: RE: excessive sugar consumption may be a difficult habit to break: a view from the brain and body. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2015) 100:56–7. doi: 10.1210/jc.2015-2223
61. Hall PA. Executive-control processes in high-calorie food consumption. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2016) 25:91–8. doi: 10.1177/0963721415625049
Keywords: Chinese Tibetan adolescents, sugar-sweetened beverages, inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility
Citation: Zhang F, Yin X, Liu Y, Li M, Gui X and Bi C (2022) Association Between Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Consumption and Executive Function Among Chinese Tibetan Adolescents at High Altitude. Front. Nutr. 9:939256. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.939256
Received: 09 May 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 27 June 2022.
Edited by:
Yajun Chen, Sun Yat-sen University, ChinaReviewed by:
Muqing Cao, Sun Yat-sen University, ChinaGemma Bridge, Independent researcher, Leeds, United Kingdom
Jiao Jiao, Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong SAR, China
Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Yin, Liu, Li, Gui and Bi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Xiaojian Yin, eGp5aW4xOTY1JiN4MDAwNDA7MTYzLmNvbQ==