Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 10 May 2022
Sec. Nutritional Epidemiology

Association of Daily Eating Duration and Day-To-Day Variability in the Timing of Eating With Fatal Cancer Risk in Older Men

\nElisa M. S. Meth
Elisa M. S. Meth1*Lieve T. van EgmondLieve T. van Egmond1Thiago C. MoulinThiago C. Moulin2Jonathan Cedernaes,Jonathan Cedernaes3,4Fredrik RosqvistFredrik Rosqvist5Christian BenedictChristian Benedict6
  • 1Department of Surgical Sciences, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 2Department of Experimental Medical Science, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 3Department of Medical Sciences, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 4Department of Medical Cell Biology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 5Department of Public Health and Caring Sciences, Clinical Nutrition and Metabolism, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 6Sleep Science Laboratory, Department of Pharmaceutical Biosciences, Molecular Neuropharmacology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden

Meal timing has significant effects on health. However, whether meal timing is associated with the risk of developing and dying of cancer is not well-researched in humans. In the present study, we used data from 941 community-dwelling men aged 71 years who participated in the Uppsala Longitudinal Study of Adult Men to examine the association of meal timing with cancer morbidity and fatal cancer. The following meal timing variables were derived from 7-day food diaries: (i) daily eating duration, i.e., the time between the first and last eating episode of an arbitrary day; (ii) the calorically weighted midpoint of the daily eating interval, a proxy of when the eating window typically occurs during an arbitrary day; and (iii) the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating. We also assessed the reported daily energy intake reliability using the Goldberg method. During a mean observational period of 13.4 years, 277 men (29.4%) were diagnosed with cancer. Furthermore, 191 men (20%) died from cancer during 14.7 years of follow-up. As shown by Cox regression adjusted for potential confounders (e.g., smoking status and daily energy intake), men with reliable dietary reports whose daily eating intervals were on average 13 h long had a 2.3-fold greater fatal cancer risk than men whose daily eating windows were on average about 11 h long. We also found that men with an average day-to-day variability in the timing of eating of 48 to 74 min had a 2- to 2.2-fold higher fatal cancer risk than those with the lowest average day-to-day variability in the timing of eating (i.e., 23 min). No clear associations were found in men with inadequate dietary reports, emphasizing the need to consider the reliability of dietary records in nutritional epidemiology. To fully unlock its potential, studies are needed to test whether recommendations to time-restrict the 24-h eating interval and reduce day-to-day variability in the timing of eating can meaningfully alter the risk of death due to cancer.

Introduction

Dietary habits have been recognized as a modifiable risk factor for many types of cancer. For example, a meta-analysis including more than three million participants demonstrated that high adherence to the Mediterranean diet was related to a lower risk of all-cause cancer mortality (1). Furthermore, a separate study including about 800,000 participants revealed that those who best complied with a dietary approach to stop hypertension (characterized by, e.g., low intake of saturated fat) had a 19% lower relative risk of colorectal cancer (2). Finally, in a prospective study from Sweden following about 1,000 community-dwelling older men over a median period of 13 years, dietary habits characterized by low carbohydrate and high protein intake were associated with a ~50% reduced risk to be diagnosed with prostate cancer (3). However, the association between diet and prostate cancer in this Swedish study was only seen among men with accurate reports of energy intake (3). Food intake reports of poor validity can distort the relationships between nutrient intake and health (4).

In addition to the type of diet, the 24-h temporal eating pattern may affect cancer risk in humans. For example, in a study among 2,413 women with early-stage breast cancer, a habitual short nightly fast of <13 h was associated with a 36% higher risk for breast cancer recurrence (5). Furthermore, in a case-control study, habitual fasting for more than 11 h overnight was associated with 23% lower odds of prostate cancer (6). In addition, a prospective French study involving 41,389 day-working men and women found that regular late eating, defined as an eating episode after 21:30 h, was associated with an increased breast and prostate cancer risk (+48 and +120%, respectively) (7). Finally, a population-based case-control study in Spain observed that those who habitually fasted at least 2 h before their nighttime rest had a 20% lower risk for breast and prostate cancer (8).

The cancer incidence is greater in older adults (9). However, it has not been systematically studied among seniors whether meal timing characteristics affect the risk of developing and dying from cancer. Here, we examined in 70-year-old men if the daily eating interval, the timing of the daily eating interval, and the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating are associated with the risk of being diagnosed with and dying of cancer. Furthermore, since unreliable food logs represent a source of bias in nutritional epidemiology (10), we paid particular attention to those with reliable dietary reports.

Subjects and Methods

Setting and Participants

The present study is based on data from the Uppsala Longitudinal Study of Adult Men (ULSAM; http://www.pubcare.uu.se/ulsam). This prospective cohort study was initiated in 1970 with the primary aim to identify cardiovascular disease and diabetes risk factors in middle-aged men. All men born between 1920 and 1924 and living in the Uppsala municipality were invited to participate in the study; 82% (n = 2,322) agreed to participate. The men were reinvestigated repeatedly until December 2015 (see http://www.pubcare.uu.se/ulsam). Between August 1991 and May 1995, 53% of the initial sample (n = 1,221) participated in the age 70 investigation, of whom 93% filled out a 7-day dietary record (n = 1,138). Dietary habits were not recorded at any other age timepoint in the ULSAM cohort study. The age-70 investigation therefore served as the baseline. Following exclusions (e.g., a cancer diagnosis before baseline), 941 men remained for analysis. No imputation for missing values was performed (please see Supplementary Figure 1 for a summary of all exclusions). The regional ethics review board at Uppsala University approved the study, including all data reported herein (ethical diary numbers: 251/90 and 97/329). All participants gave informed consent before the study inclusion.

Dietary Logs and Meal Timing Variables

After being instructed by a dietician, participants documented the content and time of each eating episode for one entire week. As previously suggested, we defined an eating episode as a calorie intake of at least 50 kcal (11). Based on the 7-day food logs, the three following meal timing variables of interest were computed:

i) The daily eating interval was calculated as the time between the first (after midnight) and last (before or at midnight) entry of an arbitrary day in the food diary (e.g., first entry at 08:00 and last entry at 18:00 = 10-h eating interval). The resulting seven daily eating intervals were then averaged.

ii) The midpoint of the daily eating interval was defined as half the time between the first and last eating episode of an arbitrary day and represents a proxy of when the eating window occurs during the 24-h day. The time of each eating episode was weighted for calorie content before calculating daily eating interval midpoints. For example, assume a person had breakfast at 06:00 (600 kcal), lunch at noon (600 kcal), and dinner at 18:00 (800 kcal) on an arbitrary day. In this case, the unweighted midpoint is at 12:00 ([06:00 + 12:00 + 18:00]/3), whereas the calorically weighted midpoint is at 12:36 (06:00*600 kcal/2,000 kcal + 12:00*600 kcal/2,000 kcal + 18:00*800 kcal/2,000 kcal). The one-week average for the calorically weighted midpoint was computed for the analysis.

iii) To determine the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating, we calculated the standard deviation of the seven daily calorically weighted midpoints. Thus, the lower the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating, the greater the eating midpoint regularity across the 7 days.

A mismatch between the timing of the eating window on working and free days, also called eating jetlag, has been associated with higher BMI (12). In the present study, we did not examine the association of eating jetlag with cancer outcomes, as the included men all passed the retirement age at the time of the investigation. Finally, as more than 90% of the participants had their last eating episode at 18:00, we did not examine the association of timing of the last meal with cancer outcomes.

Adults misreport their dietary intake on self-administered tools, most often in the direction of underreporting energy intake. Hence, we used the Goldberg approach modified by Black to identify men with inaccurate reports of energy intake (4). This method classifies participants as underreporters, accurate reporters, or overreporters using the ratio of reported energy intake to total energy expenditure. We calculated the total energy expenditure of each participant from the product of physical activity level (four categories as described below, physical activity factor: 1.4–1.7) and basal metabolic rate [using the age-adjusted Schofields formula, ref. (13)]. Then, a 95% confidence interval was created about the log of the ratio, and individuals who fell outside of the confidence interval were classified as participants inaccurately reporting daily energy intake.

Cancer Outcomes

We used entries from the Swedish National Cancer and Swedish Cause of Death Registries to calculate the time at risk for primary cancer morbidity and fatal cancer (date of retrieval: December 31, 2013). The time at risk was calculated from the exact age at the baseline investigation (scheduled between August 1991 and May 1995) to the exact age of primary cancer diagnosis (for the primary cancer morbidity analysis), death due to cancer (for the fatal cancer analysis), actual age at last follow-up (December 31, 2013), or date of death due to something else than cancer, whichever came first.

Confounders

Potential confounders measured during the age-70 investigation were selected based on previous literature (2, 1418). Diabetes was treated as a binary variable and was confirmed when 2-h blood glucose readings during an oral glucose tolerance test were ≥11.1 mmol/l. The body mass index (BMI; kg/m2) was measured during the physical examination and treated as a continuous variable. Self-reported leisure-time physical activity was divided into four categories: mainly sedentary behavior (level 1); walking or cycling for pleasure (level 2), recreational sports or heavy gardening at least 3 h every week (level 4); and regularly engage in hard physical training (level 4). Participants' exact age (continuous), current smoking status (binary variable), and family history of cancer (any cancer diagnosis among parents or siblings) at the time of the baseline investigation were assessed via questionnaires or during an onsite interview. We used the 7-day food logs to estimate the Healthy Diet Indicator (HDI) score, which is based on the dietary guidelines from the World Health Organization (19). As described elsewhere (20), the HDI score ranged from −1 (minimum adherence level) to 8 points (maximum adherence level). Based on the 7-day food logs, each participant's daily energy intake was assessed to calculate the average across the 7 days of food registration. To determine participants' alcohol intake, for each day, the energy amount of alcohol was calculated and expressed as a percentage from the daily energy intake. For the analysis, the average across the 7 days was used (continuous variable). Finally, the season of the dietary recordings was assessed.

Statistical Analysis

We performed all analyses with SPSS 26.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). For the analysis, the meal timing variables were divided into quartiles (i.e., ≤25th [quartile 1]; >25th and ≤50th [quartile 2]; >50th and ≤75th [quartile 3]; and >75th percentile [quartile 4]; see Table 1 for descriptive statistics). To account for the reliability of the meal logs, we calculated quartiles separately for the subgroup with accurate and the subgroup with inaccurate reports of daily energy intake.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the ULSAM cohort at the age 70 investigation.

The association between the meal timing variables and cancer was examined by using Cox proportional hazard regression models with attained age (in days) as the underlying timescale to estimate hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Outcomes besides that being investigated in the analysis at present were handled as censoring event (for the primary cancer diagnosis, deaths were censored and for the fatal cancer analysis, deaths due to something else than cancer were censored). The crude Cox model included the daily eating interval, the calorically weighted eating midpoint, and the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating as independent variables. To test the robustness of potential associations with cancer outcomes, we added participants' age, BMI, family history of cancer status, smoking status, self-reported physical activity level, alcohol consumption, diabetes status, adherence to the healthy diet recommendations by the WHO, mean energy intake, and season of the dietary records to the fully adjusted Cox regression model. Since unreliable food logs represent a source of bias in nutritional epidemiology (10), we report results from the Cox regression analyses for men with and without reliable dietary reports separately. Proportional hazards assumptions were confirmed using graphical evaluation.

Results

Cohort Characteristics

At baseline, the cohort was on average 71 years old, 10.8% of the men had a BMI ≥30kg/m2, and every fifth participant smoked. More characteristics, including a visualization of the distribution of the three meal timing variables of interest, can be found in Table 1 and Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Histograms for meal timing variables split by reliability of the dietary records.

Meal Timing Characteristics and Cancer Risk

During the mean observational period of 13.4 ± 5.8 years, 277 men (29.4% of the entire cohort) developed cancer (a list of primary cancer sites can be found in Supplementary Table 1). In addition, about 20% (n = 191) of the cohort died from cancer during the 14.7 ± 5.2 years follow-up (a list of cancer sites leading to death is shown in Supplementary Table 2).

Among men with reliable dietary reports, the fully adjusted Cox regression analysis revealed that men in the fourth quartile of the daily eating interval (meaning that their daily eating interval was about 13 h long) had a 2.33-fold higher risk of dying from cancer than those in the lowest quartile (i.e., those eating on average 11 h and 11 min; Table 2). We also found that men with an average a day-to-day variability in the timing of eating of 48–74 min (i.e., those assigned to quartiles 3 and 4) exhibited a 1.95- to 2.19-fold higher fatal cancer risk than those with the lowest the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating (i.e., those assigned to quartile 1; Table 2). In contrast, no significant associations between the quartiles of the meal timing exposures and cancer morbidity were observed (Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Association of meal timing variables with cancer mortality among men with and without reliable dietary reports.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association of meal timing variables with primary cancer morbidity among men with and without reliable dietary reports.

The associations of the meal timing parameters with cancer outcomes among men with unreliable dietary reports are summarized in Tables 2, 3.

Discussion

Several studies have shown that short regular daily eating intervals (definitions range between ≤11 and <13 h) reduce the risk of cancer and prolong the cancer remission period among cancer survivors (5, 6). However, whether meal timing characteristics, such as the length and timing of the daily eating interval and regularity of daily eating patterns, are associated with the risk of developing and dying from cancer in men of advanced age is largely unknown. In the present prospective cohort of community-dwelling older men with a maximum follow-up of over 22 years, we show that those eating on average 13 h per day and exhibiting an average day-to-day variability in the timing of eating of about 50 min or more have an increased risk for fatal cancer. These associations were seen despite adjustment for well-known cancer risk factors, e.g., smoking, dietary habits, and daily energy intake. If confirmed in future studies with larger sample size that involve assessment at a younger age and both sexes, our results suggest that restricting the daily eating interval and reducing the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating may have some potential in the prevention of fatal cancer among seniors.

The observation that the timing of the daily eating interval, measured by its midpoint, was not associated with cancer contrasts with some but not all previous studies. For example, a shorter daytime eating window has been shown to reduce the risk of prostate cancer in those starting eating at 08:30 or before. However, no such association was seen for those having a later onset of the daily eating interval (6), suggesting that not only the length but also the timing of the eating interval may impact the future cancer risk. However, a study among breast cancer survivors showed that fasting at least 13 h per day reduced the risk of breast cancer recurrence, irrespective of the timing of the offset of the eating interval (5).

Although our study cannot establish causality, several possible mechanisms could explain the association of the length of the daily eating duration and the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating with the risk of fatal cancer. For example, even during a shorter fasting period that can occur within a single day, healthy cells can switch from an anabolic state toward maintenance and repair state. This may limit the growth potential and mutagenic process of pre-malignant or cancerous cells (21). In contrast, proliferating cancer cells fail to adapt to scarce nutrient conditions because of a relative increase in their metabolic activity (22). Furthermore, fasting decreases blood levels of insulin-growth factor I (23, 24), a hormone that activates cellular oncogenes involved in malignant cell proliferation (25, 26). These anticancer properties may also explain why intermittent fasting has attracted significant interest as a potential lifestyle strategy in the prevention and treatment of cancer (27, 28). However, it must be kept in mind that results from intermittent fasting studies in rodents are controversial and suggest potential detrimental effects in certain oncological conditions (29). Finally, a greater day-to-day variability in the timing of eating may concur with a greater risk that food intake coincides with metabolically inappropriate endogenous circadian times. Notably, a temporal mismatch between behavioral and endogenous metabolic rhythms, e.g., as occurs in jetlag, has been associated with cancer in animal models (30).

Several strengths apply to our study, including the prospective study design and the extended follow-up, and that the observed associations remained significant following adjustment for well-known cancer risk factors. However, limitations of our study include the observational design and that residual confounding cannot be excluded. Furthermore, no potential site-specific associations between cancer outcomes and meal timing characteristics were examined. Additionally, dietary habits were only monitored at the age-70 investigation. The present study involved only men. Thus, it remains unclear whether meal timing characteristics affect cancer outcomes in women, although preliminary evidence is in favor of this hypothesis. For example, a daily eating interval shorter than 13 h may prolong cancer remission, as suggested by an investigation of female breast cancer patients (5). Finally, 29% of the cohort developed cancer during the observational period, and 20% died with cancer as the primary cause. These numbers are higher than those usually reported by, e.g., the U.S. National Cancer Institute (9), and could be explained by selection bias. With all these limitations in mind, other population-based studies, including women and repeated assessments of dietary characteristics, are needed to evaluate the generalizability of our findings further.

According to the widely applied Goldberg cut-off criterion (4), ~47% of the participants in the present study provided inaccurate reports of energy intake. Compared to men with accurate reports of daily energy intake, those with unreliable food logs had a two-point greater BMI and an about 31% lower daily energy intake. The magnitude of misreporting of energy intake by older men in the present study was greater than typically seen for younger men. For example, the percentage of underreporters in studies using estimated food records ranged from 14.3 to 42% for men (31). The underlying reasons for the relatively high proportion of older men with inaccurate energy intake reports in the present study are unclear. Still, they may include respondent memory lapses and misrepresentation of portion size consumed. Thus, screening for unreliable dietary reports is necessary when studying the association of nutritional patterns, including meal timing, with health outcomes, especially when including seniors. If ignored, inaccurate dietary reports can otherwise contribute to the lack of replicability and inconsistency across studies in the field of nutritional research, including nutritional epidemiology (10, 32). Future studies should also compare the efficacy of various methods to measure food intake among seniors reliably.

Conclusions

Our study indicates that curtailing the length of the daily eating interval and reducing the day-to-day variability in the timing of eating may be potential behavioral strategies to reduce the fatal cancer risk among older men. However, to fully unlock its potential, studies are needed to test whether recommendations to time-restrict the 24-h eating interval and have regularly timed daily eating intervals can reduce the risk of death due to cancer.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Regional Ethics Review Board at Uppsala University (https://etikprovningsmyndigheten.se/). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

EM and CB came up with the research idea. EM, LvE, TM, and CB analyzed data. EM wrote the paper. CB had primary responsibility for final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The authors' work is funded by the Novo Nordisk Foundation (NNF19OC0056777) and Swedish Brain Research Foundation (FO2020-0044). The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.889926/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Morze J, Danielewicz A, Przybyłowicz K, Zeng H, Hoffmann G, Schwingshackl L. An updated systematic review and meta-analysis on adherence to mediterranean diet and risk of cancer. Eur J Nutr. (2021) 60:1561–86. doi: 10.1007/s00394-020-02346-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Mohseni R, Mohseni F, Alizadeh S, Abbasi S. The Association of Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) Diet with the risk of colorectal cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Nutr Cancer. (2020) 72:778–90. doi: 10.1080/01635581.2019.1651880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ax E, Garmo H, Grundmark B, Bill-Axelson A, Holmberg L, Becker W, et al. Dietary patterns and prostate cancer risk: report from the population based ULSAM cohort study of Swedish men. Nutr Cancer. (2014) 66:77–87. doi: 10.1080/01635581.2014.851712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Black AE. Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake:basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2000) 24:1119–30. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Marinac CR, Nelson SH, Breen CI, Hartman SJ, Natarajan L, Pierce JP, et al. Prolonged nightly fasting and breast cancer prognosis. JAMA Oncol. (2016) 2:1049–55. doi: 10.1001/jamaoncol.2016.0164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Palomar-Cros A, Espinosa A, Straif K, Pérez-Gómez B, Papantoniou K, Gómez-Acebo I, et al. The association of nighttime fasting duration and prostate cancer risk: results from the multicase-control (MCC) study in Spain. Nutrients. (2021) 13:2662. doi: 10.3390/nu13082662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Srour B, Plancoulaine S, Andreeva VA, Fassier P, Julia C, Galan P, et al. Circadian nutritional behaviours and cancer risk: New insights from the NutriNet-santé prospective cohort study: disclaimers. Int J Cancer. (2018) 143:2369–79. doi: 10.1002/ijc.31584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kogevinas M, Espinosa A, Castelló A, Gómez-Acebo I, Guevara M, Martin V, et al. Effect of mistimed eating patterns on breast and prostate cancer risk (MCC-Spain Study). Int J Cancer. (2018) 143:2380–9. doi: 10.1002/ijc.31649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ioannidis JPA. The challenge of reforming nutritional epidemiologic research. JAMA. (2018) 320:969–70. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.11025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Leech RM, Worsley A, Timperio A, McNaughton SA. Characterizing eating patterns: a comparison of eating occasion definitions. Am J Clin Nutr. (2015) 102:1229–37. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.114660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Zerón-Rugerio MF, Hernáez Á, Porras-Loaiza AP, Cambras T, Izquierdo-Pulido M. Eating jet lag: a marker of the variability in meal timing and its association with Body Mass Index. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2980. doi: 10.3390/nu11122980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Schofield WN. Predicting basal metabolic rate, new standards and review of previous work. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr. (1985) 39(Suppl 1):5–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Jha P, Ramasundarahettige C, Landsman V, Rostron B, Thun M, Anderson RN, et al. 21st-century hazards of smoking and benefits of cessation in the United States. N Engl J Med. (2013) 368:341–50. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa1211128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Yuan S, Kar S, Carter P, Vithayathil M, Mason AM, Burgess S, et al. Is type 2 diabetes causally associated with cancer risk? Evidence from a two-sample mendelian randomization study. Diabetes. (2020) 69:1588–96. doi: 10.2337/db20-0084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kyu HH, Bachman VF, Alexander LT, Mumford JE, Afshin A, Estep K, et al. Physical activity and risk of breast cancer, colon cancer, diabetes, ischemic heart disease, and ischemic stroke events: systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. BMJ. (2016) 354:i3857. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i3857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mucci LA, Hjelmborg JB, Harris JR, Czene K, Havelick DJ, Scheike T, et al. Nordic Twin Study of Cancer (NorTwinCan) collaboration. Familial risk and heritability of cancer among twins in Nordic countries JAMA. (2016) 315:68–76. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.17703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Boffetta P, Hashibe M. Alcohol and cancer. Lancet Oncol. (2006) 7:149–56. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(06)70577-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Berentzen NE, Beulens JW, Hoevenaar-Blom MP, Kampman E., Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, Romaguera-Bosch D, et al. Adherence to the WHO's healthy diet indicator and overall cancer risk in the EPIC-NL cohort. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e70535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. van Egmond L, Tan X, Sjögren P, Cederholm T, Benedict C. Association between healthy dietary patterns and self-reported sleep disturbances in older men: the ULSAM study. Nutrients. (2019) 11:1029. doi: 10.3390/nu11051029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lee C, Raffaghello L, Brandhorst S, Safdie FM, Bianchi G, Martin-Montalvo A, et al. Fasting cycles retard growth of tumors and sensitize a range of cancer cell types to chemotherapy. Sci Transl Med. (2012) 4:124ra27. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3003293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Raffaghello L, Lee C, Safdie FM, Wei M, Madia F, Bianchi G, et al. Starvation-dependent differential stress resistance protects normal but not cancer cells against high-dose chemotherapy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2008)105:8215-20. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708100105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Henning PC, Scofield DE, Rarick KR, Pierce JR, Staab JS, Lieberman HR, et al. Effects of acute caloric restriction compared to caloric balance on the temporal response of the IGF-I system. Metabolism. (2013) 62:179–87. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2012.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Beauloye V, Willems B, de Coninck V, Frank SJ, Edery M, Thissen JP. Impairment of liver GH receptor signaling by fasting. Endocrinology. (2002) 143:792–800. doi: 10.1210/endo.143.3.8692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Buono R, Longo VD. Starvation, stress resistance, and cancer. Trends Endocrinol Metab. (2018) 29:271–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2018.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Khandwala HM, McCutcheon IE, Flyvbjerg A, Friend KE. The effects of insulin-like growth factors on tumorigenesis and neoplastic growth. Endocr Rev. (2000) 21:215–44. doi: 10.1210/edrv.21.3.0399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gabel K, Cares K, Varady K, Gadi V, Tussing-Humphreys L. Current evidence and directions for intermittent fasting during cancer chemotherapy. Adv Nutr. (2022) 13:667–80. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmab132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Harvie MN, Howell T. Could intermittent energy restriction and intermittent fasting reduce rates of cancer in obese, overweight, and normal-weight subjects? A summary of evidence. Adv Nutr. (2016) 7:690–705. doi: 10.3945/an.115.011767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Clifton KK, Ma CX, Fontana L, Peterson LL. Intermittent fasting in the prevention and treatment of cancer. CA Cancer J Clin. (2021) 71:527–46. doi: 10.3322/caac.21694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Filipski E, Delaunay F, King VM, Wu MW, Claustrat B, Gréchez-Cassiau A, et al. Effects of chronic jet lag on tumor progression in mice. Cancer Res. (2004) 64:7879–85. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-04-0674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Poslusna K, Ruprich J, de Vries JH, Jakubikova M., van't Veer P. Misreporting of energy and micronutrient intake estimated by food records and 24 hour recalls, control and adjustment methods in practice. Br J Nutr. (2009) 101(Suppl 2):S73–85. doi: 10.1017/S0007114509990602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Willett W. Nutritional epidemiology: issues and challenges. Int J Epidemiol. (1987) 16:312–7. doi: 10.1093/ije/16.2.312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: meal timing, eating duration, eating midpoint, day-to-day variability in the timing of eating, cancer, cohort, hazard ratio

Citation: Meth EMS, van Egmond LT, Moulin TC, Cedernaes J, Rosqvist F and Benedict C (2022) Association of Daily Eating Duration and Day-To-Day Variability in the Timing of Eating With Fatal Cancer Risk in Older Men. Front. Nutr. 9:889926. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.889926

Received: 04 March 2022; Accepted: 07 April 2022;
Published: 10 May 2022.

Edited by:

Stephanie Bonn, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Anna Palomar Cros, Instituto Salud Global Barcelona (ISGlobal), Spain
Daniela Di Giuseppe, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Copyright © 2022 Meth, van Egmond, Moulin, Cedernaes, Rosqvist and Benedict. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elisa M. S. Meth, ZWxpc2EubWV0aCYjeDAwMDQwO25ldXJvLnV1LnNl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.