Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Nutr., 20 December 2022
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health: Chemosensory Signals, Nutrition, and Eating Behavior View all 6 articles

Neural correlates of food labels on brand, nature, and nutrition: An fMRI meta-analysis

  • Oral and Maxillofacial Radiology, Applied Oral Sciences and Community Dental Care, Faculty of Dentistry, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Eating is an essential act of our everyday life, and it involves complicated cognitive appraisal and gustatory evaluation. This study meta-analyzed the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies about food labels on brand, nature and nutrition. Web of Science Core Collection (WoS), Scopus, and PubMed were queried to identify human fMRI studies written in English and published in peer-reviewed journals and used taste or food related labels. Studies were excluded if they reported no results from taste/food related stimuli versus control, no task-based fMRI results, or no results from whole-brain analysis. Nineteen studies entered the analysis. Results for the meta-analysis on food nutrition revealed that the precuneus on the right hemisphere was significantly activated, a brain region related to internal mentation of self-consciousness and nutritional evaluation. Results for the overall analysis on all 19 studies, the analysis on food brand, and the analysis on food nature revealed no significant brain regions. Food nutrition labels were generally processed by brain regions related to internal mentation of self-consciousness and nutritional evaluation. However, the neural correlates of labels of food brand and food nature were inconsistent across studies. More future studies are needed to better understand the cognitive processing of different kinds of food labels in our brain.

Introduction

Eating is an essential act of our everyday life, and it involves complicated cognitive appraisal and gustatory evaluation. Multiple meta-analyses have attempted to pool data across studies to identify brain regions responsible for primary tasting (13); for processing visual, taste, and odor food stimuli (4), food commercials (5), affect, intensity, and quality of food stimuli (6), tastants delivered with and without swallowing (7); for differential processing between hungry and satiated state (8), male and female (9), obese and normal weight (10); and regions targeted by anti-appetite medications (11). (For a more complete list of meta-analyses on taste and food, please refer to (12)). Meanwhile, different kinds of food label, such as food brand logo, nutritional label, or label about food nature should modulate the cerebral processing of food information. An obvious analogy is graphic cigarette warning label. Some smokers and potential smokers may refrain from taking the cigarettes upon seeing such warning label printed on a pack of cigarettes. Compared to control warnings, it was reported that such graphic warning labels could significantly improve subjects’ motivation to quit smoking, and elicited stronger activation in numerous brain regions (13). The information provided by a nutritional label may similarly influence consumer behavior. For example, consumers were likely to pay 11% more for a box of cookies with a nutritional label compared to its counterpart without a label (14). There could be more modulating factors such as consumers’ nutritional knowledge and trust toward the nutrition and health claims (15, 16). On the other hand, food brand could also affect how much a consumer liked the product and his/her subsequent purchasing behavior (17). Understanding the neurobiology underlying such food choice and consumption behavior is obviously beneficial for marketing, nutritional advice, and pharmaceuticals. For instance, the cerebral regions highly related to gustatory processing, such as the insula and orbitofrontal cortex, were activated in response to visual cues of shower gel and dish liquid with beverage-imitating packages (18). This might explain why some people would unintentionally drink shampoo or other poisonous home products. In fact, anti-appetite medications often targeted the gustatory processing at the insula (11). Besides, it was found that discrete regions of the prefrontal cortex, responsible for decision making and self-control, were activated in response to seeing popular brand logos of foods such as French fries versus corresponding organic brand logos (19). It is important to know more about the neural correlates of various types of food labels, so that the designs of the food labels can be tested and optimized by neuroimaging evaluations.

Here, three types of food labels were examined: food brand logo, nutritional label, or label about food nature. Food brand logos might trigger a consumer’s memory about the past consumption experience and involve social facets such as brand loyalty. In 1985, the Coca Cola company introduced the so-called “New Coke” into the market, which was rated more superiorly than the original product in blinded taste tests. However, the word “new” printed on the can was largely rejected by consumers as they preferred the original image of the can and thus the original “package” of Coca Cola including the drink filled into the can (20). Similarly, it was found that human brain had differential gustatory processing of Coca Cola relative to Pepsi in multiple regions such as dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, implying differential drink preference (21). Meanwhile, nutritional label provides nutritional information of the food product that may influence liking, perception of healthiness, and ultimately food choice. There are three main groups of nutritional labels, traffic light, guideline daily amounts (GDA), and health logos/ratings (22). Consumers were more willing to pay for foods tagged with healthy nutritional labels than foods with unhealthy labels (23). In particular, seeing foods tagged with red (unhealthy) traffic light nutritional label would activate the inferior frontal gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, regions implicated in self-control in food choice (23). Finally, the remaining type of label is about food nature. Food nature has diverse context. One fundamental aspect is food versus non-food. For instance, isovaleric acid is a chemical with a strong odor. It is present in foods (e.g., cheese and apple juice) and also produced by human body, attributing to sweaty smell and foot odor (24). The smell of isovaleric acid was rated much more unpleasant when it was labeled as body odor compared to labeled as cheddar cheese, with differential activations at the anterior cingulate, medial orbitofrontal cortex, and the amygdala (25). Apart from food versus non-food, the social/environment meanings of the food can also be expressed by the labels, such as Halal versus non-Halal for Muslims (26) was differentiated in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, organic versus conventional (27) was differentiated in the ventral striatum, and fair trade versus conventional (28) was differentiated in the ventral striatum, cingulate cortex, and superior frontal gyrus.

Therefore, this meta-analytic study pooled data across functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. The activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analytic approach was selected, which calculated the consistency of significantly activated brain coordinates reported from published studies by means of probability distribution modeling (29, 30). Based on the existing literature, it was hypothesized that the neural correlates of food brand logo, nutritional label, and label about food nature would involve the gustatory network, such as the insula and the prefrontal cortex.

Materials and methods

Literature search and study selection

In May 2022, three databases, Web of Science Core Collection (WoS), Scopus, and PubMed were queried with the following search string: (taste* OR flavo* OR food*) AND (fMRI OR neuroimaging OR “functional MRI” OR “functional magnetic”) AND (brand* OR label* OR logo*). For the former two databases, the title, abstract, and keyword fields were searched. Since PubMed does not index keywords, the title and abstract fields were searched. The search yielded 100 papers from WoS, 149 from Scopus, and 64 from PubMed. After excluding duplicates, 178 papers remained. The titles and abstracts of these 178 papers were reviewed to determine their relevance. Papers were eligible if they were (i) original studies written in English and published in peer-reviewed journals; (ii) human fMRI studies; and (iii) using taste or food related labels. Studies were excluded if they reported (i) no results from taste/food related stimuli versus control; (ii) no task-based fMRI results; or (iii) no results from whole-brain analysis. Finally, 19 studies remained (19, 21, 23, 25, 27, 28, 3143) (Figure 1 and Table 1). This study was not pre-registered in PROSPERO or other databases.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A flow chart that illustrates the literature search.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Details of the 19 analyzed studies.

ALE meta-analysis

Brain coordinates of reported activations were collected from the included studies. They were subjected to probability distribution modeling to unveil brain regions consistently activated across studies (29). Some studies mentioned that their coordinates were reported in Talairach space, while other studies reported their coordinates in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. For those coordinates reported in Talairach space, the Lancaster transformation (44) was applied to convert them into MNI space before the analysis. The software GingerALE (version 3.0.2) (45) was used for such conversion and to conduct the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. The Turkeltaub non-additive ALE method was selected (46), and subject-based full-width half-maximum values were applied as default (29). For statistical threshold, a cluster-level inference was used. In other words, a cluster of voxels was considered significantly activated across the studies if it survived a cluster-level P < 0.05, familywise error (FWE) rate-corrected for multiple comparisons, with a cluster-defining threshold of uncorrected voxel-level P < 0.001. This is the standard statistical threshold recommended for ALE meta-analysis (47). A meta-analysis was performed independently for: (1) all 19 included studies, (2) 8 studies that involved food brand, (3) 9 studies that involved food nature, and (4) 5 studies that involved food nutrition. The thresholded ALE maps were overlaid onto the standard Colin T1 brain template in MNI space (48) and visualized with Mango 4.0 (49). The cluster size of brain regions was outputted by GingerALE from the ALE map. Figure 2 shows a flow chat that illustrated the process of this ALE meta-analysis.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow chart that illustrates the process of activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis.

Label-based meta-analysis

In addition, a “label-based meta-analysis” [method based on (50)] was performed to tabulate/summarize the activated brain regions commonly reported across the studies. The extracted activation foci were re-surveyed by entering them into Mango 4.0, which provided a label to each focus (coordinate) to the nearest gray matter in a standardized way (50). This qualitative analysis would provide additional information to the readers in case the quantitative ALE analysis yielded insignificant results.

Results

The details of the 19 fMRI studies on food labels are listed in Table 1. Each study recruited 9–50 subjects, with two studies recruiting men only and one study women only. Regarding subject handedness, 10 studies recruited right-handed subjects only, whereas 4 studies recruited mostly right-handed subjects and 5 studies did not report details. Regarding subject age, most of the studies recruited young and middle-aged adults, whereas 3 studies focused on children and adolescents below the age of 18, and no study involved the elderly above the age of 65. For body mass index (BMI) of the subjects, 8 studies provided the mean data that ranged from 17.7 to 23.6. Meanwhile, 7 studies reported the number of hours the subjects fasted prior to the fMRI scan (1–6 h) and 1 study reported that the subjects did not fast. For the dimensions investigated, 5 studies investigated food brand, 6 studies investigated food nature, 5 studies investigated food nutrition, and 3 studies investigated food brand/nature.

For ALE meta-analysis, the overall analysis on all 19 studies, the analysis on food brand, and the analysis on food nature revealed no significant brain regions. Results remained insignificant even when a more liberal threshold of P < 0.05 (false-discovery rate corrected) with cluster size > 200 mm3 was applied.

The ALE meta-analysis on food nutrition revealed that the precuneus on the right hemisphere was significantly activated (Figure 3). The peak voxel of the activated cluster was at [6, −68, 40], with ALE value of 1.75 × 10–2 and cluster size of 520 mm3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The right precuneus was significantly activated across studies that investigated food nutrition labels. The peak voxel of the activated cluster was at [6, –68, 40], with ALE value of 1.75 × 10–2 and cluster size of 520 mm3.

The additional label-based meta-analysis showed that the activated brain regions for food brand processing seemed to be more diverse/inconsistent compared to food nature and food nutrition (Tables 24). Figure 4 shows the unthresholded ALE brain map for food brand. It illustrated that multiple brain regions were reported from the included studies, though none of them reached statistical significance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Qualitative label-based meta-analysis of the eight studies concerning food brand.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Qualitative label-based meta-analysis of the nine studies concerning food nature.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Qualitative label-based meta-analysis of the five studies concerning food nutrition.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Unthresholded ALE brain map resulted from food brand studies showing the diverse brain regions involved. It is overlaid on a glass brain for visualization. None of the clusters reached statistical significance.

Discussion

This is the first meta-analysis on fMRI studies regarding brain responses to food labels. Surprisingly, results did not yield significant activations in the hypothesized brain regions of the insula and the prefrontal cortex. Results have shown that the cerebral processing of food labels on nutrition significantly activated the right precuneus. The processing of food labels on food brand and food nature did not yield significant results.

The insula is known to be the primary taste cortex that processes neural signals relayed from taste receptors in the oral cavity (51). The meta-analytic results here did not find significant activation of it, which might be partly explained by the fact that the analyzed studies did not involve actual tasting or stimulation of the taste receptors. Instead, the stimuli involved visual and cognitive aspects of food processing. Meanwhile, the prefrontal cortex is responsible for top-down affective modulation (51) and should be activated. The insignificant results might be attributed to heterogeneous designs of the studies.

The precuneus was reported to consume much more glucose than other parts of the cerebral cortex and thus sensitive to and easily affected by malnutrition (52). Among healthy subjects, precuneus was found to activate more in response to personalized nutritional messages compared to untailored nutritional messages (53). Meanwhile, its cortical thickness correlated with nutritional state and cognitive functions in patients with anorexia nervosa (54). Hence, the results of current meta-analysis on food nutrition labels were consistent to the existing literature that the precuneus seems to play an important role in nutritional evaluation.

At first glance, the insignificant result from food brand was unexpected. However, a prior fMRI study that presented visual cues of car brands showed that the cerebral processing of brands indeed involved diverse brain regions, e.g., familiar brands preferentially activated superior frontal gyrus, hippocampus and posterior cingulate; sports and luxury brands activated medial prefrontal cortex and precuneus; whereas brands deemed value products activated superior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate (55). Therefore, the heterogeneity of subject background and the exact instructions given to them during fMRI scan might have attributed to the inconsistency across the analyzed studies. As a matter of fact, one of the analyzed studies on food brand did investigated Coca Cola and Pepsi separately, and compared brand-cued drink delivery to light-cued (colored circle) delivery (21). Significant results were only found for Coca Cola but not Pepsi. Another study similarly found that Coca Cola elicited greater activation than Pepsi in the right amygdala, whereas the opposite comparison resulted in no significant activation (32). This illustrated that the cerebral processing of food brand(s) could be complicated and not easily elicited in a consistent manner.

The heterogeneous situation was more obvious for food nature studies. Three studies investigated food versus non-food labels, followed by two investigated organic versus conventional food. Other comparisons were each investigated in one study, such as fair-trade label versus no label, food name label versus no label, eating alone label versus eating together label, and a correct match between flavor name and color on potato chip packaging versus a wrong match. It could be reasonably argued that different brain regions could be utilized for the cognitive processing of such distinct group of labels about food nature, rendering the results insignificant. There were even other labels on food nature being investigated. For instance, it was reported that there was a significant difference in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity level between viewing meat images labeled with Halal logo and those labeled with non-Halal logo, though this study was not included in the present analysis due to its lack of description on whether whole-brain analysis was performed (and also no brain coordinates provided) (26).

This meta-analysis has several limitations. First, the participants involved in the analyzed studies were mainly young and middle-aged adults with some children and adolescents. The results therefore might not be well applied to the elderly population, especially that aging was shown to associate with decreasing activation in the precuneus in response to food stimuli in a previous study (56). In addition, the analyses on the food brand and food nature subsets did not reveal significant results, so that potential follow-up analyses to compare between food brand, food nature, and food nutrition could not be conducted. Moreover, the majority of the analyzed studies exclusively recruited right-handed subjects or did not report subject handedness, similar to the scenario revealed by a prior survey on nutritional neuroscience literature (57). It was established that handedness would affect the lateralization of cerebral processing of tasting (58, 59). However, the methodology to gauge and determine subject handedness lacked a consensus and remained disputable (60). Regardless, due to the lack of results from exclusively left-handed subjects, it was not possible to test and determine if handedness would affect cerebral processing of food labels. Readers should be aware of these limitations during results interpretation. Finally, ALE meta-analysis can only compute the consistency of brain coordinates activated across studies but it cannot compute the effect size (30). Subsequently, future studies should include left-handed subjects in their sample. Moreover, the elderly people should be recruited so that the results could be more representative of the whole population. Researchers should share the statistical brain maps resulted from future original studies or report their effect size, so that the effect size could also be meta-analyzed.

In conclusion, the cerebral processing of food labels on nutrition significantly activated the right precuneus, a brain region related to nutritional evaluation. The peak voxel of the activated cluster was at [6, −68, 40], with ALE value of 1.75 × 10–2 and cluster size of 520mm3. The processing of food labels on food brand and food nature did not yield significant results. More future studies are needed to better understand the cognitive processing of different kinds of food labels in our brain. They should include left-handed subjects as well as the elderly people, so that results could be more generalizable.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the departmental funds only.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB. A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:519–34. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Veldhuizen MG, Albrecht J, Zelano C, Boesveldt S, Breslin P, Lundström JN. Identification of human gustatory cortex by activation likelihood estimation. Hum Brain Mapp. (2011) 32:2256–66. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Yeung AWK, Goto TK, Leung WK. Basic taste processing recruits bilateral anteroventral and middle dorsal insulae: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Brain Behav. (2017) 7:e00655. doi: 10.1002/brb3.655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Huerta CI, Sarkar PR, Duong TQ, Laird AR, Fox PT. Neural bases of food perception: coordinate-based meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies in multiple modalities. Obesity. (2014) 22:1439–46. doi: 10.1002/oby.20659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Yeung AWK. Brain responses to watching food commercials compared with nonfood commercials: a meta-analysis on neuroimaging studies. Public Health Nutr. (2021) 24:2153–60. doi: 10.1017/S1368980020003122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Yeung AWK, Goto TK, Leung WK. Affective value, intensity and quality of liquid tastants/food discernment in the human brain: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neuroimage. (2018) 169:189–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Yeung AWK. Differences in brain responses to food or tastants delivered with and without swallowing: a meta-analysis on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. Chemosens Percept. (2022). doi: 10.1007/s12078-022-09299-6 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. van der Laan LN, De Ridder DT, Viergever MA, Smeets PA. The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage. (2011) 55:296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Yeung AWK. Sex differences in brain responses to food stimuli: a meta-analysis on neuroimaging studies. Obes Rev. (2018) 19:1110–5. doi: 10.1111/obr.12697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kennedy J, Dimitropoulos A. Influence of feeding state on neurofunctional differences between individuals who are obese and normal weight: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Appetite. (2014) 75:103–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yeung AWK. Neural correlates of anti-appetite medications: an fMRI meta-analysis. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2021) 19:2049–56. doi: 10.2174/1570159X19666210914142227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Yeung AWK, Wong NSM, Lau H, Eickhoff SB. Human brain responses to gustatory and food stimuli: a meta-evaluation of neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neuroimage. (2019) 202:116111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Green AE, Mays D, Falk EB, Vallone D, Gallagher N, Richardson A, et al. Young adult smokers’ neural response to graphic cigarette warning labels. Addict Behav Rep. (2016) 3:28–32. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Loureiro ML, Gracia A, Nayga RM Jr. Do consumers value nutritional labels? Eur Rev Agric Econ. (2006) 33:249–68. doi: 10.1093/erae/jbl005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Drichoutis AC, Lazaridis P, Nayga RM. Nutrition knowledge and consumer use of nutritional food labels. Eur Rev Agric Econ. (2005) 32:93–118. doi: 10.1093/erae/jbi003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Drichoutis AC, Lazaridis P, Nayga RM, Kapsokefalou M, Chryssochoidis G. A theoretical and empirical investigation of nutritional label use. Eur J Health Econ. (2008) 9:293–304. doi: 10.1007/s10198-007-0077-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kühn S, Strelow E, Gallinat J. Multiple “buy buttons” in the brain: forecasting chocolate sales at point-of-sale based on functional brain activation using fMRI. Neuroimage. (2016) 136:122–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Basso F, Robert-Demontrond P, Hayek M, Anton J-L, Nazarian B, Roth M, et al. Why people drink shampoo? Food imitating products are fooling brains and endangering consumers for marketing purposes. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e100368. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fehse K, Simmank F, Gutyrchik E, Sztrókay-Gaul A. Organic or popular brands—food perception engages distinct functional pathways. an fMRI study. Cogent Psychol. (2017) 4:1284392. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1284392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Imram N. The role of visual cues in consumer perception and acceptance of a food product. Nutr Food Sci. (1999) 5:224–8. doi: 10.1108/00346659910277650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. McClure SM, Li J, Tomlin D, Cypert KS, Montague LM, Montague PR. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron. (2004) 44:379–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Grunert KG, Wills JM. A review of European research on consumer response to nutrition information on food labels. J Public Health. (2007) 15:385–99. doi: 10.1007/s10389-007-0101-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Enax L, Hu Y, Trautner P, Weber B. Nutrition labels influence value computation of food products in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Obesity. (2015) 23:786–92. doi: 10.1002/oby.21027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Liddell K. Smell as a diagnostic marker. Postgrad Med J. (1976) 52:136–8. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.52.605.136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. De Araujo IE, Rolls ET, Velazco MI, Margot C, Cayeux I. Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron. (2005) 46:671–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Al-Kwifi OS, Gelaidan HMH, Fetais AHM. Identifying the influence of the halal logo on muslim consumers’ attitudes using fMRI technology. J Islam Mark. (2020) 12:1159–79. doi: 10.1108/JIMA-01-2020-0026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Linder NS, Uhl G, Fliessbach K, Trautner P, Elger CE, Weber B. Organic labeling influences food valuation and choice. Neuroimage. (2010) 53:215–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Enax L, Krapp V, Piehl A, Weber B. Effects of social sustainability signaling on neural valuation signals and taste-experience of food products. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9:247. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, Wang LE, Zilles K, Fox PT. Coordinate-based ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30: 2907–26. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR, Kurth F, Fox PT. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage. (2012) 59:2349–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schaefer M, Knuth M, Rumpel F. Striatal response to favorite brands as a function of neuroticism and extraversion. Brain Res. (2011) 1425:83–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kühn S, Gallinat J. Does taste matter? How anticipation of cola brands influences gustatory processing in the brain. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e61569. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bruce AS, Bruce JM, Black WR, Lepping RJ, Henry JM, Cherry JBC, et al. Branding and a child’s brain: an fMRI study of neural responses to logos. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2014) 9:118–22. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Burger KS, Stice E. Neural responsivity during soft drink intake, anticipation, and advertisement exposure in habitually consuming youth. Obesity. (2014) 22:441–50. doi: 10.1002/oby.20563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Wolfe K, Jo W, Olds D, Asperin A, DeSanto J, Liu W-C. An fMRI study of the effects of food familiarity and labeling on brain activation. J Culin Sci Technol. (2016) 14:332–46. doi: 10.1080/15428052.2016.1138917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Burger KS. Frontostriatal and behavioral adaptations to daily sugar-sweetened beverage intake: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. (2017) 105:555–63. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.140145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Courtney AL, PeConga EK, Wagner DD, Rapuano KM. Calorie information and dieting status modulate reward and control activation during the evaluation of food images. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0204744. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Prevost M, Hot P, Muller L, Ruffieux B, Cousin E, Pichat C, et al. Neural correlates of the healthiness evaluation processes of food labels. Nutr Neurosci. (2018) 21:467–77. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2017.1309820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wegman J, van Loon I, Smeets PA, Cools R, Aarts E. Top-down expectation effects of food labels on motivation. Neuroimage. (2018) 173:13–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Huang J, Wang F, Sui J, Wan X. Functional and structural basis of the color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search. Neuropsychologia. (2019) 127:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Masterson TD, Stein WM, Beidler E, Bermudez M, English LK, Keller KL. Brain response to food brands correlates with increased intake from branded meals in children: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2019) 13:1035–48. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9919-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zhang X, Liu Y, Gu Y, Wang S, Chen H. Red for “stop”:“traffic-light” nutrition labels decrease unhealthy food choices by increasing activity and connectivity in the frontal lobe. Nutrients. (2020) 12:128. doi: 10.3390/nu12010128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Huang J, Wang C, Wan X. Influence of eating together on brain activation and hedonic evaluation in response to foods. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2022). [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.3758/s13415-021-00982-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, et al. Bias between MNI and talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp. (2007) 28:1194–205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Research Imaging Institute. GingerALE version 2.3.6. (2016). Available online at: http://www.brainmap.org/ale/ (accessed May 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

46. Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox M, Wiener M, Fox P. Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum Brain Mapp. (2012) 33:1–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Laird AR, Fox PT, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, et al. Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 84:151–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Holmes CJ, Hoge R, Collins L, Woods R, Toga AW, Evans AC. Enhancement of MR images using registration for signal averaging. J Comput Assist Tomogr. (1998) 22:324–33. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199803000-00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Research Imaging Institute, UTHSCSA. Mango for the desktop. (2016). Available online at: http://rii.uthscsa.edu/mango/mango.html (accessed May 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

50. Lin C-S, Niddam D, Hsu M-L. Meta-analysis on brain representation of experimental dental pain. J Dent Res. (2014) 93:126–33. doi: 10.1177/0022034513512654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Rolls ET. Taste, olfactory, and food reward value processing in the brain. Prog Neurobiol. (2015) 127:64–90. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2001) 2:685–94. doi: 10.1038/35094500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Casado-Aranda L-A, van der Laan N, Sánchez-Fernández J. Neural activity in self-related brain regions in response to tailored nutritional messages predicts dietary change. Appetite. (2022) 170:105861. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. de la Cruz F, Schumann A, Suttkus S, Helbing N, Zopf R, Bär K-J. Cortical thinning and associated connectivity changes in patients with anorexia nervosa. Transl Psychiatry. (2021) 11:95. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01237-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Schaefer M, Rotte M. Thinking on luxury or pragmatic brand products: brain responses to different categories of culturally based brands. Brain Res. (2007) 1165:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.06.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cheah Y, Lee S, Ashoor G, Nathan Y, Reed L, Zelaya F, et al. Ageing diminishes the modulation of human brain responses to visual food cues by meal ingestion. Int J Obes. (2014) 38:1186–92. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Yeung AWK, Wong NSM, Eickhoff SB. Empirical assessment of changing sample-characteristics in task-fMRI over two decades: an example from gustatory and food studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2020) 41:2460–73. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cerf B, Lebihan D, Van de Moortele P, Mac Leod P, Faurion A. Functional lateralization of human gustatory cortex related to handedness disclosed by fMRI study. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1998) 855:575–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10627.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Faurion A, Cerf B, Van De Moortele P-F, Lobel E, Mac Leod P, Le Bihan D. Human taste cortical areas studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging: evidence of functional lateralization related to handedness. Neurosci Lett. (1999) 277:189–92. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00881-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Yeung AWK, Wong NSM. The non-transparent usage and reporting of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory in functional magnetic resonance imaging literature: a survey of studies published since 2013. Laterality. (2021) 27:221–31. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2021.1984497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: food, nutrition, meta-analysis, gustation, taste, CBMA food, CBMA

Citation: Yeung AWK (2022) Neural correlates of food labels on brand, nature, and nutrition: An fMRI meta-analysis. Front. Nutr. 9:1056692. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.1056692

Received: 29 September 2022; Accepted: 29 November 2022;
Published: 20 December 2022.

Edited by:

Pengfei Han, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by:

Shao-Wei Xue, The Affiliated Hospital of Hangzhou Normal University, China
Yanming Wang, University of Science and Technology of China, China

Copyright © 2022 Yeung. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andy Wai Kan Yeung, ndyeung@hku.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.