Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 12 January 2023
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health: Chemosensory Signals, Nutrition, and Eating Behavior View all 6 articles

Chronic stress is associated with reward and emotion-related eating behaviors in college students

  • 1Faulty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 2MOE Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Chongqing, China

Introduction: Stress is related to altered olfactory perception and eating behaviors. The current study investigated the association between chronic stress, food reward and perception of food and non-food odors among college students.

Methods: Sixty-one participants completed the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) and the Brief Daily Stressors Screening Tool (BDSST). The detective threshold and suprathreshold perception (pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity) of two food (chocolate, strawberry) odors and a non-food (rose) odor were measured. Food reward and macronutrient preference were measured using the computerized Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire and the Macronutrient and Taste Preference Ranking task, respectively. Reward-related eating, emotional eating and eating-related inhibitory control were measured by the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) and the Reward-Based Eating Drive Scale (RED) scales.

Results: Neither the perceived stress or the severity of daily life stressor exposure was related to odor sensitivity, however, the PSS score was significantly correlated with pleasantness for strawberry odor (r = 0.329, p = 0.013). Chronic stress (PSS and BDSST scores) was significantly correlated with the DEBQ emotional eating and reward-related eating measured by RED (all ps < 0.01). Moreover, the BDSST score was negatively correlated with subjective liking for low-calorie sweet foods (r = −0.46, p < 0.001).

Discussion: Together, our preliminary results suggest disassociated effect of chronic stress on odor perception and eating behaviors.

Introduction

Olfaction plays an important role in eating. Food odors are potent stimuli with high ecological relevance and more affect-laden (e.g., hedonism or motivation) in the context of food appraisal (1). Chronic stress is characterized as an oppressive, unremitting prolonged aversive state that accumulate and lead to poor psychological and physical health (2). Exposure to chronic stress is associated with long-lasting effects on olfactory functions (3, 4). Specifically, mice exposed to chronic variable stress demonstrated decreased sensitivity toward food (lemon and strawberry) and non-food odors (5, 6), as well as reduced odor-induced electrophysiological responses at the olfactory mucosa (7). In humans, chronic stress exposure is associated with structural and functional alterations of several key brain areas that involved in olfactory processing, such as the amygdala and the hippocampus (8). However, the potential relationship between chronic stress levels and odor perceptions in humans had not been directly explored. Besides, food odors can reflect nutritional information such as the caloric density and main macronutrients content of food (9). For example, by smelling of food odors, people can distinguish and can classify food items with the “taste” (for example: sweet, non-sweet) or energy density (e.g., high or low energy-dense) (10, 11). It is also relevant to investigate the relationship between chronic stress with food odors related to high or low-calorie density.

With large cohort of population survey, several recent studies have shown an association between high chronic stress levels and unhealthy dietary patterns, such as increased consumption of foods with high fat or sugar contents, and decreased preference and consumption of fruits or vegetables (1214). One possible mechanism is that stress increases the reward processing of food via cortisol activation (15) and reduced dietary restraint (16). For example, people with high chronic stress demonstrated enhanced brain responses to high-calorie food cues (17). In addition, chronic stress was positively correlated with other abnormal eating behavior, such as emotional eating (18), loss of control of eating (19).

The aim of the current study was twofold. First, the association between chronic stress and perception of food (chocolate and strawberry) and non-food (rose-like) odors were investigated. Second, food reward and macronutrient preferences were measured with validated behavioral tests, and the relationship between chronic stress with food reward and preference were explored. Moreover, the stress responses (subjective perceived stress) and stressor exposure of participants were assessed using the perceived stress scale (PSS) (20) and the brief daily stressors screening tool (BDSST) (21).

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from college students of the Southwest University China. Intended participants with known olfactory dysfunction, stuffy nose or rhinitis, neurological or mental diseases, smoking habit, or taking drugs that affecting appetite or olfaction were excluded via online survey. Six-one college students (Age Mean = 20.7, SD = 2.0; BMI Mean = 20.0, SD = 2.2; 33 females) were recruited and participated in the study. All participants reported to have normal olfactory function, and none was tested to be COVID-19 positive. The experiment was carried out in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki on biomedical research involving human subjects. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee at the Faculty of Psychology Southwest University. Participants signed consent form prior to participation.

Procedure

Participants were instructed not to wear perfumes and to avoid eating anything (water excepted) 2 h before they come to the laboratory. Upon arrival, participants rated their hunger level on a 100-mm visual analog scale (from 0 = not hungry at all to 100 = very hungry). Participants were asked to fill out the questionnaires, then olfactory tests in the order of evaluation task, threshold test and discrimination tasks were performed. After olfactory tests, participants performed the food reward and macronutrients preference tasks. The whole experiment lasts for about 1 h.

Questionnaires

Perceived stress and daily stressor

The Chinese version of the PSS (22) was used to measure participants’ long-term stress level. The 10-item scale is used to assess how out of control, unpredictable or overloaded an individual’s life has felt in the past month on a five-point scale (0 = never, 4 = always) (20). The ratings of all items were summed to create a score range from 0 to 40, with higher score indicating a higher level of chronic stress. The PSS offers a good internal reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.88).

The BDSST is a 10-item questionnaire assessing the experience of general daily stressors in eight distinct life domains such as housing or employment or study over the past 12 months (21). It measures subjective degree of stress on a five-point scale (0 = not at all, 4 = very much). Due to the cultural difference, the first item “difficulty in social obligation” was deleted, so the questionnaire had nine items in total. The internal reliability of the Chinese version of the BDSST was high, and the Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0.78.

Chronic stress related manifestation

As suggested by Schmidt et al. (23), chronic stress is characterized as a multidimensional including fatigue, depressiveness and anxiety. Relevant questionnaires were included to assess those aspects. The Chalder Fatigue Scale (CFS) measures participants’ physical and mental fatigue in the past month (24). The CFS consists of 11 items with each question is rated on a four-point scale (0 = less than usual, 1 = no more than usual, 2 = more than usual, 3 = much more than usual). The higher the score of each dimension, the higher the fatigue degree. The internal reliability of CFS was also good (Cronbach’s α = 0.81). The Chinese version (25) of Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) was used to measure participants’ depressive tendencies (26). The scale consists of 13 questions, each of which has 4 short sentences, representing 4 possible answers. The Chinese version of the BDI offers a good internal reliability with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.88. The Chinese version (27) of Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T) was used to assess subjects’ relatively stable anxiety tendencies, including the general states of calmness, confidence and security (28). The scale consists of 20 items, and each item is rated on a four-point scale (1 = almost none, 2 = some, 3 = often, 4 = almost always). The higher the total score, the higher the trait anxiety level. The internal reliability of the Trait Anxiety Inventory was 0.89.

Eating behaviors

The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) was used to measure individuals’ problematic eating tendencies (29). The questionnaire consists of 33 questions and measures three dimensions: restrained eating, emotional eating, and external eating. Each question is rated on a five-point scale (1 = never, 2 = occasionally, 3 = sometimes, 4 = always, 5 = very often). The higher the score of each dimension of the questionnaire is, the higher the tendency of restricted eating, emotional eating and external eating. The internal reliability of the three subscales was high (Cronbach’s α ranged from 0.73 to 0.91).

The 13-item reward-based Eating Drive Scale (RED-13) measures reward-related eating (30). The scale assesses three dimensions: loss of control over eating, lack of satiety, and pre-occupation with food. Participants answered each question on a five-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The higher the score of each dimension of the questionnaire, the higher the tendency to overeat. The internal reliability of the three subscales was good (Cronbach’s α ranged from 0.77 to 0.88).

Olfactory measurement

A high-calorie food odor (chocolate, Taste Master Pty Ltd, Australia, Product code ITM40033), a low-calorie food odor (strawberry, Taste Master Pty Ltd, Australia, Product code ITM20177) and a non-food odor (rose, Taste Master Pty Ltd, Australia, Product code IFP10765/B) were selected as olfactory stimuli. The edibility rating of the rose odor was significantly lower than that of the chocolate or the strawberry odors (p < 0.001).

Participants first evaluated the three odors at suprathreshold concentrations (chocolate 0.128%; strawberry 0.128% and rose 25.6%). Participants held the brown bottle and sniffed it for three to five seconds and rated for the odor pleasantness, intensity, or familiarity on 100-mm visual analogue scales from 0 (very unpleasant/no smell at all/very unfamiliar) to 100 (very unpleasant/very intense/very familiar). After completion of the suprathreshold odor evaluation tests, participants underwent the odor sensitivity test. Odors were diluted using propylene glycol into 12 concentrations in a geometric dilution series (1:2). The assessment of participants’ olfactory sensitivity followed the single-staircase, 3-alternative forced choice procedure, in which participants were presented with 3 randomly arranged bottles, 2 of which contained pure diluent (the propylene glycol) and the third the target odor stimuli. Participants have to decide which smells differently. The test follows the procedure of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” odor threshold test (31). In brief, two successive correct identifications or one incorrect identification triggered a reversal of the staircase, i.e., the next higher or the next lower concentration step was presented, respectively. Seven reversals had to be obtained (including the starting point), and the sensitivity was defined as the mean of the last four staircase reversals. A higher score indicates higher odor sensitivity. The order of olfactory test for three odors was balanced among participants.

After completion of the odor threshold tests, participants evaluated the three odors at suprathreshold concentrations (chocolate 0.128%; strawberry 0.128% and rose 25.6%). Participants held the brown bottle and sniffed it for three to 5 s and rated for the odor pleasantness, intensity, or familiarity on 100-mm visual analog scales from 0 (very unpleasant/no smell at all/very unfamiliar) to 100 (very unpleasant/very intense/very familiar).

Food reward

Food liking and wanting were measured by the computer-based Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire (32). Twenty food pictures were selected based on their taste (sweet or savory) and fat content (high or low) resulted in four categories (high-fat sweet (HFSW); high-fat savory (HFSA); low-fat sweet (LFSW), and low-fat savory (LFSA)] with five foods in each category (Supplementary Table 1). The food pictures were selected based on International Food Picture Database and modified for local Chinese dietary culture.

The task consisted of two parts, presented in randomized order. For explicit liking, participants rated for each food item on a 100-mm visual analogue scale (VAS). For wanting part, there are 190 groups of 2 food pictures in each group for the current desired choice. Two food pictures in each group were presented on the left and right positions of the same page in a random order. Subjects were required to press “c” and “m” keys on the keyboard for their key response. Finally, the cumulative bonus points of the degree of wanting of each food picture are accumulated to calculate the degree of food wanting of each food.

Macronutrients and taste preference

The Macronutrient and Taste preference ranking task (MTPRT) was developed for measuring individuals’ preference for food tastes and macronutrients (33). A modified version (10) of the MTPRT includes 32 food images from four macronutrient categories: high-carbohydrate, high-fat, high-protein and low-energy. Each category contained 8 food items, of which four were sweet and four were savory. Because no products met all requirements to be included as high-protein sweet, the high-protein category formed an exception and consisted of 8 savory products. Therefore, the seven categories are as follows: high-carbohydrate sweet (HCSW), high-carbohydrate savory (HCSA); high-fat sweet (HFSW); high-fat savory (HFSA); high-protein savory (HPSA); low-energy sweet (LESW); low-energy savory (LESA) and savory (LESA) (Supplementary Table 2). There were three parts in the test: practicing, liking, and ranking. The liking part was designed to introduce participants to each product by name and picture. Liking was assessed by presenting pictures of all 32 food items with the question: “How much do you like [product name]?” which was rated on a 9-point scale ranging from 1 (do not like at all) to 9 (like extremely). The ranking part consisted of two sections, one focused on macronutrients and the other on taste, i.e., sweet and savory. Participants were asked to rank four products based on how much they preferred to eat the different foods in their daily life. The task outcomes were: macronutrient liking score (range from 1 to 9), macronutrient preference score (range from 1 to 4), and taste preference score (range from 1.5 to 3.5). These rankings were used to assess the relative preferences for specific macronutrient or flavors (sweet or savory). The task was executed in E-Prime 2.0 professional (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA).

Data analysis

First, we checked whether the variables for approximately normally distribution, by use of Shapiro–Wilk’s tests. The gender, age, BMI and hunger level of the subjects was included as control variables for the following analysis. However, PSS and BDSST were used as predictor variables to investigate the associations between stress and odor perception. Correlation analyses were performed between the chronic stress measures with olfactory measures, eating behaviors and food reward. Notably, the significance was established for α = 0.05, and the obtained p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons (e.g., α = 0.017 = 0.05/3 for odor sensitivity). For odor pleasantness, odor familiarity was included as control variables, as these two are positively linked (34). All analyses were performed by means of the SPSS (version 26.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and GraphPad Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, Inc. La Jolla, CA, USA) software. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Participant characteristics

Table 1 illustrated the descriptive statistics of major variables. The high level of perceived stress is characterized by higher level of fatigue, depressive symptoms, and trait anxiety. As suggested the perceived stress is typically multifaceted and includes several psychological components of the stress response, such as feeling of fatigue or anxiety (2, 23). The following analyses will focus on the two variables that assess chronic stress.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlations, means, standard deviations of scale variables (N = 61).

Odor perception

A weak significant correlation was observed between the BDSST and threshold for rose odor (r = 0.270, p = 0.038). However, the correlation no longer significant after corrected for multiple tests (p > 0.017). No significant correlation was found between the PSS, or the BDSST and odor threshold for chocolate or strawberry odors. There was neither significant correlation between stress measures and odor discrimination ability.

Regarding the supra-threshold odor perception, there was no significant correlation between PSS or BDSST and pleasantness or intensity ratings for chocolate or rose odors, after controlling for odor familiarity. For the strawberry odor, PSS was significantly correlated with pleasantness ratings (r = 0.312, p = 0.017), but not for intensity ratings (r = −0.08, p = 0.57). The significance level was preserved after odor familiarity and intensity ratings were both controlled (r = 0.329, p = 0.013). In other words, a higher perceived stress is related with higher pleasantness for strawberry odor.

Questionnaire-based eating behavior

The DEBQ emotional eating was significantly correlated to the PSS (r = 0.327, p = 0.012; Figure 1A) and BDSST (r = 0.352, p = 0.006; Figure 1B). No significant correlation was found between DEBQ restraint or external eating scores and PSS or BDSST (ps > 0.1). Regarding reward-related eating, the RED total score was significantly correlated to PSS (r = 0.39, p = 0.002; Figure 1C) and BDSST (r = 0.48, p < 0.001; Figure 1D). Specifically, the PSS was significantly correlated to the loss of control eating (r = 0.350, p = 0.007) and the lack of satiety (r = 0.392, p = 0.002) of the RED scale, but not the food preoccupation (r = 0.19, p > 0.1). The BDSST score was significantly correlated to all the subscales of the RED scale, namely loss of control (r = 0.447, p < 0.001), food preoccupation (r = 0.427, p = 0.001), and the lack of satiety (r = 0.487, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatter plot (N = 61) showing significant correlations between the DEBQ emotional and PSS (A) or BDSST (B); and the significant correlations between the RED total score and PSS (C) or BDSST (D).

Food liking, wanting, and macronutrients preference

There was a negative correlation between the BDSST and liking for low-calorie sweet food (r = −0.46, p < 0.001, Figure 2). That is to say, the higher BDSST score, the lower liking for low-calorie sweet foods. No other significant correlation was found between chronic stress and food evaluation task measures.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatter plot (N = 61) showing the significant correlation between daily exposed stressors [measured using the brief daily stressors screening tool (BDSST)] and the liking ratings for low-calorie sweet (LCSW) foods (r = –0.46, p < 0.001).

Discussion

Animal research showed that chronic stress is related to olfactory impairment, especially early step of olfactory processing, such as odor detection (6, 7). However, our primary analyses indicated no significant association between chronic stress and olfactory sensitivity to chocolate, strawberry or rose odors. The olfactory habituation/cross-habituation test was used in the animal study which is different from the detection threshold test used for human study. In addition, the well-controlled chronic stress induction in animal experiments led to severe depressive like status. Thus, direct comparison of the current results with findings from animal studies can be difficult. Emotions have been linked to changed odor perception. Chronic stress can induce mood changes and promote the onset of anxio-depressive symptoms in human with loss of pleasure seeking (anhedonia) (35). For examples, associations between state anxiety and decreased sensitivity to rose odor (36), while another study found people with high anxiety sensitivity demonstrated enhanced sensitivity to guaiacol–a smoke-like odor (37). Future study would include measures of chronic stress associated negative emotions of the participants and investigate the effect of specific emotional feelings on odor perception (3). There were evidences from animal research that stress-related decreased olfactory perception is modulated by the glucocorticoid hormone (5, 38). In contrary, early human research showed that increased cortisol was associated with improved odor detection abilities (39). Future research with cortisol measurement included could better characterize the impact of chronic stress on olfaction.

In terms of eating behavior, participants with a higher level of perceived stress demonstrated higher tendency for reward-based eating, with higher score on loss of control eating and lack of satiety, which is in accordance with previous research findings (4042). It had been shown that participants with higher reported chronic stress demonstrated increased activation of the reward brain regions and decreased activations of the prefrontal regions in response to high palatable food stimuli (17). However, the current study did not show significant results regarding the food reward processing assessed with the food reward and preference tasks. One study has found that the level of chronic stress during examination period had no effect on behaviors related to palatable food purchase and intake, but participants reported having less control over their food choices during the exam period (43). Moreover, a significant association was found between the BDSST and decreased liking for low-calorie foods. This is in consistent with previous study showing a decreased hedonic ratings and liking for low-calorie chip flavors among individuals with high chronic stress levels (44). However, the results also showed positively correlation between the perceived stress score and pleasantness rating for the strawberry odor at suprathreshold level.

There are limitations to the current study. First, limited types of odor were used in the olfactory perception tests, which is insufficient to draw any generalized conclusions. Second, compared to other studies using PSS, the participants within the higher range of PSS score may be moderate stress. Other potential moderating factors (e.g., gender) was not explored with the small sample size in the current study. In addition, the computerized tasks may be not optimal do not capture the real liking perception of food flavors (45). Last but not least, some questionnaires (e.g., RED) lack of local validation. The results from the current study could not be generalized to wider population (e.g., different ages, or geographical backgrounds).

In conclusion, the current preliminary study provided little evidence for a correlation between chronic stress levels and odor perception. Higher chronic stress was associated with reward-related and emotional eating behaviors and decreased liking for low-calorie sweet foods. Future research on characterizing stress-related eating favor a multidimensional and more objective measurement of chronic stress and also a combination of self-reported eating behavior with actual food choices and consumption (46, 47).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee at the Faculty of Psychology Southwest University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MT and PH contributed to conception and design of the study. MT performed the experiment and the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. PH guided the conduct of experiments, data analysis, and revision of the manuscript. Both authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by research grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32100881), the High-End Foreign Expert Project (G202168001L), and the Program for the Top Young Talents Chongqing awarded to PH. This study was also supported by the Innovation Research 2035 Pilot Plan of Southwest University (SWUPilotPlan006).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.1025953/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Chan K, Stevenson RJ, Francis H. The nose is hungrier than the eyes. Psychon Bull Rev. (2020) 28:657–64. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01843-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Epel ES, Crosswell AD, Mayer SE, Prather AA, Slavich GM, Puterman E, et al. More than a feeling: a unified view of stress measurement for population science. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2018) 49:146–69. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bombail V. Perception and emotions: on the relationships between stress and olfaction. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2019) 212:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.12.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Yuan TF, Hou G, Arias-Carrion O. Chronic stress impacts on olfactory system. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. (2015) 14:486–91. doi: 10.2174/1871527314666150429111356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Meunier N, Raynaud A, Le Bourhis M, Grebert D, Dewaele A, Acquistapace A, et al. The olfactory mucosa, first actor of olfactory detection, is sensitive to glucocorticoid hormone. Eur J Neurosci. (2020) 51:1403–18. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Vaz RP, Cardoso A, Serrao P, Pereira PA, Madeira MD. Chronic stress leads to long-lasting deficits in olfactory-guided behaviors, and to neuroplastic changes in the nucleus of the lateral olfactory tract. Horm Behav. (2018) 98:130–44. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Raynaud A, Meunier N, Acquistapace A, Bombail V. Chronic variable stress exposure in male Wistar rats affects the first step of olfactory detection. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 291:36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lupien SJ, Juster RP, Raymond C, Marin MF. The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: from neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2018) 49:91–105. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Denzer-Lippmann MY, Beauchamp J, Freiherr J, Thuerauf N, Kornhuber J, Buettner A. Development and validation of a food-associated olfactory test (FAOT). Chem Senses. (2017) 42:47–57. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjw099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Han P, Mohebbi M, Seo HS, Hummel T. Sensitivity to sweetness correlates to elevated reward brain responses to sweet and high-fat food odors in young healthy volunteers. Neuroimage. (2020) 208:116413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Morquecho-Campos P, Bikker FJ, Nazmi K, de Graaf K, Laine ML, Boesveldt S. Impact of food odors signaling specific taste qualities and macronutrient content on saliva secretion and composition. Appetite. (2019) 143:104399. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hill D, Conner M, Clancy F, Moss R, Wilding S, Bristow M, et al. Stress and eating behaviours in healthy adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. (2021) 16:280–304. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2021.1923406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Khaled K, Tsofliou F, Hundley V, Helmreich R, Almilaji O. Perceived stress and diet quality in women of reproductive age: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr J. (2020) 19:92. doi: 10.1186/s12937-020-00609-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Long Z, Huang L, Lyu J, Xia Y, Chen Y, Li R, et al. The associations of perceived stress with food preferences and activity preferences: a cross-sectional study among Chinese adults. J Affect Disord. (2022) 297:430–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.10.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Adam TC, Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol Behav. (2007) 91:449–58. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Roberts CJ, Campbell IC, Troop N. Increases in weight during chronic stress are partially associated with a switch in food choice towards increased carbohydrate and saturated fat intake. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2014) 22:77–82. doi: 10.1002/erv.2264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Tryon MS, Carter CS, Decant R, Laugero KD. Chronic stress exposure may affect the brain’s response to high calorie food cues and predispose to obesogenic eating habits. Physiol Behav. (2013) 120:233–42. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ling J, Zahry NR. Relationships among perceived stress, emotional eating, and dietary intake in college students: eating self-regulation as a mediator. Appetite. (2021) 163:105215. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Joseph PV, Davidson HR, Boulineaux CM, Fourie NH, Franks AT, Abey SK, et al. Eating behavior, stress, and adiposity: discordance between perception and physiology. Biol Res Nurs. (2018) 20:531–40. doi: 10.1177/1099800418779460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. (1983) 24:385. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Scholten S, Lavallee K, Velten J, Zhang XC, Margraf J. The brief daily stressors screening tool: an introduction and evaluation. Stress Health. (2020) 36:686–92. doi: 10.1002/smi.2965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Lu W, Bian Q, Wang W, Wu X, Wang Z, Zhao M. Chinese version of the perceived stress scale-10: a psychometric study in Chinese university students. PLoS One. (2017) 12:e0189543. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schmidt K, Enge S, Miller R. Reconsidering the construct validity of self-reported chronic stress: a multidimensional item response theory approach. Psychol Assess. (2020) 32:997–1014. doi: 10.1037/pas0000829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Cella M, Chalder T. Measuring fatigue in clinical and community settings. J Psychosom Res. (2010) 69:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chan C-M, Tsoi MM. The BDI stimulus determinants of cognitive-related depression among Chinese college students. Cogn Ther Res. (1984) 8:501–7. doi: 10.1007/BF01173287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbauch J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lai H-L, Liao K-W, Huang C-Y, Chen P-W, Peng T-C. Effects of music on immunity and physiological responses in healthcare workers: a randomized controlled trial. Stress Health. (2013) 29:91–8. doi: 10.1002/smi.2429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Spielberger CD. State-trait anxiety inventory for adults. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden, Inc (1983). doi: 10.1037/t06496-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Van Strien T, Frijters JE, Bergers GP, Defares PB. The dutch eating behavior questionnaire (debq) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int J Eat Disord. (1986) 5:295–315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Epel ES, Tomiyama AJ, Mason AE, Laraia BA, Hartman W, Ready K, et al. The reward-based eating drive scale: a self-report index of reward-based eating. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e101350. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. ‘Sniffin’ sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses. (1997) 22:39–52. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.1.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Finlayson G, King N, Blundell JE. Liking vs. wanting food: importance for human appetite control and weight regulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2007) 31:987–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. de Bruijn SEM, de Vries YC, de Graaf C, Boesveldt S, Jager G. The reliability and validity of the macronutrient and taste preference ranking task: a new method to measure food preferences. Food Qual Prefer. (2017) 57:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Broge EHDL, Wendin K, Rasmussen MA, Bredie WLP. Familiarity and identification of everyday food odors in older adults and their influence on hedonic liking. Food Qual Prefer. (2023) 103:104715. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Anacker C, Zunszain PA, Carvalho LA, Pariante CM. The glucocorticoid receptor: pivot of depression and of antidepressant treatment? Psychoneuroendocrinology (2011) 36:415–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Takahashi T, Itoh H, Nishikawa Y, Higuchi Y, Nakamura M, Sasabayashi D, et al. Possible relation between olfaction and anxiety in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2015) 69:431–8. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Houghton DC, Howard SL, Uhde TW, Paquet C, Schlosser RJ, Cortese BM. Odor sensitivity impairment: a behavioral marker of psychological distress? CNS Spectr. (2019) 24:404–12. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Siopi E, Denizet M, Gabellec MM, de Chaumont F, Olivo-Marin JC, Guilloux JP, et al. Anxiety- and depression-like states lead to pronounced olfactory deficits and impaired adult neurogenesis in mice. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:518–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2817-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pause BM, Sojka B, Krauel K, Fehm-Wolfsdorf G, Ferstl R. Olfactory information processing during the course of the menstrual cycle. Biol Psychol. (1996) 44:31–54. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(96)05207-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Groesz LM, McCoy S, Carl J, Saslow L, Stewart J, Adler N, et al. What is eating you? Stress and the drive to eat. Appetite. (2012) 58:717–21. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sims R, Gordon S, Garcia W, Clark E, Monye D, Callender C, et al. Perceived stress and eating behaviors in a community-based sample of African Americans. Eat Behav. (2008) 9:137–42. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Tomiyama AJ, Dallman MF, Epel ES. Comfort food is comforting to those most stressed: evidence of the chronic stress response network in high stress women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2011) 36:1513–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Schmidt JB, Bertolt CJ, Sjodin A, Ackermann F, Schmedes AV, Thomsen HY, et al. Does stress affect food preferences? - A randomized controlled trial investigating the effect of examination stress on measures of food preferences and obesogenic behavior. Stress. (2018) 21:556–63. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2018.1494149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Luckett CR, Oswald CG, Wilson MKM, Pinto de Carvalho Alves M, Sullivan LB, Ferreira Floriano G, et al. Chronic stress decreases liking and satisfaction of low-calorie chips. Food Res Int. (2015) 76:277–82. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.01.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Pool E, Sennwald V, Delplanque S, Brosch T, Sander D. Measuring wanting and liking from animals to humans: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 63:124–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Dorsey A, Scherer EM, Eckhoff R, Furberg R. Measurement of human stress: a multidimensional approach. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI Press Publication (2022). doi: 10.3768/rtipress.2022.op.0073.2206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stammers L, Wong L, Brown R, Price S, Ekinci E, Sumithran P. Identifying stress-related eating in behavioural research: a review. Horm Behav. (2020) 124:104752. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: chronic stress, food odor perception, emotion-related eating, reward-related eating, food preference

Citation: Tuluhong M and Han P (2023) Chronic stress is associated with reward and emotion-related eating behaviors in college students. Front. Nutr. 9:1025953. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.1025953

Received: 23 August 2022; Accepted: 28 December 2022;
Published: 12 January 2023.

Edited by:

Elsa Lamy, University of Évora, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Jonathan Kershaw, Bowling Green State University, United States
Marie-Christine Broillet, Université de Lausanne, Switzerland

Copyright © 2023 Tuluhong and Han. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pengfei Han, www.frontiersin.org p.han@foxmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.