- 1College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Bedford Park, SA, Australia
- 2School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition (IPAN), Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia
This review uses person-centered research and data analysis strategies to discuss the conceptualization and measurement of appetite self-regulation (ASR) phenotypes and trajectories in childhood (from infancy to about ages 6 or 7 years). Research that is person-centered provides strategies that increase the possibilities for investigating ASR phenotypes. We first examine the utility of examining underlying phenotypes using latent profile/class analysis drawing on cross-sectional data. The use of trajectory analysis to investigate developmental change is then discussed, with attention to phenotypes using trajectories of individual behaviors as well as phenotypes based on multi-trajectory modeling. Data analysis strategies and measurement approaches from recent examples of these person-centered approaches to the conceptualization and investigation of appetite self-regulation and its development in childhood are examined. Where relevant, examples from older children as well as developmental, clinical and educational psychology are drawn on to discuss when and how person-centered approaches can be used. We argue that there is scope to incorporate recent advances in biological and psychoneurological knowledge about appetite self-regulation as well as fundamental processes in the development of general self-regulation to enhance the examination of phenotypes and their trajectories across childhood (and beyond). The discussion and conclusion suggest directions for future research and highlight the potential of person-centered approaches to progress knowledge about the development of appetite self-regulation in childhood.
Introduction
In current food environments, where there is an abundance of palatable but unhealthy foods, it is important that children are able to self-regulate appetite. Appetite self-regulation (ASR) means that children are better able to resist tempting but unhealthy foods, better balance energy intakes with expenditure and select and consume healthier diets. The conceptualization and measurement of children's ASR is an emerging field and new approaches are needed. The recent application of person-centered approaches to research on children's eating and ASR provide new perspectives and insights and is the focus of the present review.
There has been some vagueness and uncertainty about definitions of ASR in childhood, including its components and fundamental processes. A number of theoretical models together with potentially relevant constructs are evident in the literature. There is a general acceptance that ASR has to do with responding to hunger cues as well as to cues of satiety and satiation (1, 2). Satiation and satiety have been conceived as separate but overlapping processes. Satiation leads to the termination of eating while satiety is a post-consumption process that leads to the inhibition of further eating and is an ingredient in the inter-meal interval (3–6). Beyond that, it has been variously treated as a multidimensional construct that includes traits (e.g., food responsiveness or satiety responsiveness), processes [e.g., as in the Satiety Cascade (4, 7)] and individual skills or strategies (e.g., delay of gratification) (8). Many of the traits have been associated with the subscales of the Children's Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ). This scale includes food approach behaviors such as food responsiveness (e.g., child is attracted to food and eating) and food avoidance behaviors such as satiety responsiveness (e.g., child gets full easily and leaves food on his/her plate) (9). The Satiety Cascade is a model that involves pre-consumption processes (such as hunger cues and food responsiveness), processes during consumptions such as satiety and habituation, and post-consumptions processes such as satiety cues.
The conceptualization of ASR has been assisted by the application of overall models such as the bottom-up, top-down model (10–12) or dual processing model (13) and the satiety cascade (4, 7). These models incorporate aspects of traits, processes, and skills. In the bottom-up, top-down model there is a recursive interplay between bottom-up reactive, often emotion driven and automatic, approach or avoidance behaviors (such as hedonic responses to hunger or food cues) and top-down regulatory control processes including inhibitory control. In the dual processing model, self-regulation is conceived as involving an interaction between regulatory processes such as inhibitory control (top-down) and approach-avoidance behaviors (bottom-up). Here, self-regulation is considered to involve an interplay between impulse generating and impulse controlling systems. It is possible to group many of the ASR-related constructs that have been described and investigated in the literature under the main headings of bottom-up (approach), such as food responsiveness, bottom-up (avoidance) such as food fussiness, and top down such as inhibitory control (12). The bottom-up, top-down model for conceptualizing ASR in childhood is helpful in the investigation of ASR phenotypes as it provides a framework for the interpretation of patterns of characteristics and behaviors.
Most of the existing research on the development of ASR has taken a variable-centered approach [c.f. the studies reviewed in (8)]. Similarly, the conceptualization of fundamental processes in ASR such as in the models described above has been substantially based on variable-centered research. To broaden knowledge about ASR in childhood, there is a need to give greater recognition to the nature and extent of individual differences in behaviors, processes, traits, skills and trajectories in the development of ASR. There is limited research and theory about ASR that focuses separately on traits, processes, and skills. Instead, consistent with the multidimensional treatment of ASR in infancy and childhood, research designs typically include measures of one or more of traits, processes, and skills although with a heavy emphasis on traits. In the present review, the focus is on possible advances in conceptualization that could be gained from a person-centered perspective to research and theory. We discuss ASR phenotypes, trajectories of individual ASR indicators, and trajectories of underlying ASR phenotypes. We argue that the exploration of ASR phenotypes and trajectories has implications for the conceptualization and measurement of ASR in childhood. If it can be assumed that scholarship is at a relatively early stage of developing an integrated model of ASR in childhood, person-centered strategies could provide insights into how traits, processes and skills might be organized and interrelated.
The initial scoping of the review was based on keyword and abstract literature searches in the main databases: PubMed, PsycINFO, SCOPUS, Web of Science, and Google Scholar. The searches covered theoretical articles (e.g., person-centered vs. variable centered), data analysis (e.g., latent class analysis, trajectory analysis), and research evidence (e.g., eating behavior phenotypes, latent class, and profile analyses of eating behaviors in childhood). We sourced articles under these headings from the literature on children's eating and ASR, as well as other areas of scholarship (such as clinical psychology, developmental psychology, and educational psychology). In contrast to the structured approach of a systematic review consistent with our purpose we chose articles for their illustrative significance in relation to the purpose of discussing the nature, role, and contribution of person-centered strategies in the investigation of ASR phenotypes and trajectories in childhood.
We begin with a discussion of person-centered vs. variable-centered strategies before considering the prospect for investigations of phenotypes to examine ASR components and processes in childhood. The main body of the review is then about the measurement of trajectories and the potential of this approach to contribute to knowledge about the development of ASR.
Person-Centered vs. Variable-Centered Approaches
Person-centered and variable-centered approaches answer different research questions, and can provide complementary information about a research field, including about child development (14–21). Variable-centered approaches (e.g., regression analysis, factor analysis, structural equation modeling) examine associations between variables in a population and are suited to questions about normative development and the effect of one variable on another, especially in terms of the contributions of predictor variables to outcome variables (16, 22–26). In this case, data are aggregated and individual differences are treated as “noise” or “errors” which provides more parsimony (19) but less specificity (16).
In contrast, individual differences, or an assumption that the sample is not uniform and that behavior and psychological processes are unique to the individual or differ from one group to another (15, 27), are the basis of person-centered approaches such as cluster analysis and finite mixture models (14, 15, 20, 27–31). In this case the emphasis is on individual and sub-group differences within the sample, including sub-group differences in development. Person-centered approaches are helpful for understanding possible developmental mechanisms (20) and allow investigation of inter-individual (between-person) differences in intraindividual (within-person) profiles of behavior or change (32, 33). Person-centered approaches categorize participants into subgroups, or phenotypes, based on a set of shared characteristics or attributes. The resulting phenotypes may be based on complex patterns of substantive variables to provide a more detailed and nuanced picture of development. For these reasons, person-centered approaches have the potential to advance knowledge about ASR and its development in childhood.
The Contribution of Phenotypes to the Conceptualization of ASR
Technically, a phenotype is a set of behaviors and characteristics arising from the interaction of the genotype with the environment (34). The phenotype concept is useful as a way of describing combinations of individual traits and behavior, especially as they apply to sub-groups of the population. The concept has been applied to children's eating behaviors as we discuss here, but also to parent feeding practices (35, 36), in developmental psychology (37–39), and in other areas of psychology such as educational psychology (40) and clinical psychology (41–43).
The examination of ASR phenotypes can contribute to the understanding of components and processes in ASR. The investigation of possible phenotypes enables ASR to be conceptualized in new ways by suggesting different combinations of measures of traits, processes, skills, behaviors and other ASR-related measures (e.g., neurobehavioral indicators) and how these combinations interact for separate sub-groups. For example, ASR difficulties could be associated with increased food approach tendencies in some groups of children, difficulties in responding to satiety and satiation cues in other groups, and increased impulsiveness and reduced inhibitory control in other children, or different combinations and patterns of change of these characteristics. The so-called patterns of change could vary from consistency across ages, to increases, decreases or other variations such as consistency followed by an increase or a decrease. Possible insights from the examination of phenotypes have been illustrated in an adult sample where four phenotypes of obesity-related behaviors and characteristics were derived from behavioral and questionnaire measures (44). One phenotype, labeled “hungry brain,” was described as having abnormal satiation, and another phenotype, labeled “emotional eating,” was high on hedonic eating. The other phenotypes were “hungry gut,” involving abnormal satiety, and “slow burn” described as involving decreased metabolic rate. The authors assumed that the phenotypes revealed something about fundamental processes in weight gain and obesity and described them as “actionable” because they were helpful to target weight-loss treatments. The examination of phenotypes has been enhanced by the application of person-centered data analysis strategies. In the following section we describe the analytical strategies available for examining phenotypes and illustrate their use in ASR/eating behavior research. These include strategies for analyzing both cross-sectional and longitudinal data.
Person-Centered Approaches and Their Application in ASR Phenotype Research
Earlier person-centered approaches for evaluating the underlying psychological attributes of weight gain in cross-sectional studies included cluster analyses using hierarchical (e.g., Ward's) or partitioning (e.g., k-means) methods (45, 46). However, finite mixture models (FMM), also known as model-based clustering or latent variable mixture models (LVMM), are newer techniques that have become a popular alternative for understanding population heterogeneity (47). Examples of such techniques include latent class analysis (LCA), latent transition analysis (LTA), and growth mixture models (GMM) (47). FMM draw on a structural equation modeling (SEM) approach and encompass a “collection of statistical approaches” for analyzing cross-sectional and longitudinal data [(41, 48), p. 175]. They take one or more observed input variables to model the probability of participants belonging to latent (i.e., unobserved or underlying) subgroups and classify participants to the subgroup with the highest probability of their belonging (41, 48). The resultant subgroups may be referred to as classes, profiles, typologies, or phenotypes, with the latter more commonly used in the field of psychology (32). Interested readers are referred to Berlin and colleagues (18, 48) for an introduction and non-technical account of cross-sectional and longitudinal FMM/LVMM approaches, covering assumptions and a “how to” description of their use.
Cross Sectional Studies: Latent Class and Latent Profile Analysis
LCA for categorical data and latent profile analysis (LPA) for continuous data are foundation person-centered strategies used in many fields including medical, biological, physical nutritional and social sciences (18, 31, 40, 48, 49). LCA and LPA are examples of FMM/LVMM used in cross-sectional studies (31) and identify latent subgroups based on specific combinations of observed variables (18, 48). The goal of the analysis is to determine the optimal number of latent subgroups that summarize the unique, and often complex, patterns of the observed variables within individuals (32).
In cross-sectional studies, distinct eating behavior subgroups, or phenotypes, have been identified in children using LCA or LPA. For example, Boutelle et al. (50) conducted LPA from multiple measures of eating behaviors in a sample of 8 to 12 year-old children with overweight or obesity. Three latent profiles were identified, labeled as (i) high satiety responsiveness, (ii) high food responsiveness and (iii) moderate satiety and food responsiveness. Although each phenotype was associated with overeating and overweight, the phenotypes involved combinations of different levels on the individual variables, such as eating in the absence of hunger (EAH), satiety responsiveness, food responsiveness, negative affect eating, loss of control eating and external eating.
In a sample of 4-year-old children, Tharner et al. (51) conducted LPA on Children's Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) scores and identified six eating behavior profiles. Most children were in the “moderate eaters” (44.6%) or “avoidant eaters” (33.2%) profiles. The authors were mainly interested in the “fussy eater” profile (5.6%) which was characterized by high scores on the subscales of satiety responsiveness, food fussiness and slowness in eating combined with low scores on the enjoyment of eating subscale. This subscale was associated with dietary, weight and parental factors, demonstrating the utility of examining a number of eating behavior subscales as profiles rather than individual variables.
Longitudinal Studies of Subgroups: Latent Transition Analysis
LTA is a longitudinal extension of LCA and LPA that identifies unique latent subgroups based on combinations of observed variables, and an individual's transition, or movement, between these latent subgroups over time (40, 52–55). LTA has mostly been applied to two time points, but three may be used (16, 56, 57). In longitudinal research, LTA may be used to investigate different developmental paths or transitions from one phenotype to another over time (16, 47, 53). It also enables the examination of whether phenotypes might be age-specific or if they are established early and then are maintained across childhood. Further, it could assist in the examination of outcomes or changes associated with interventions (e.g., whether participants transition to a different phenotype following intervention).
Pitt et al. (58) illustrate the use of LTA to examine developmental change. The authors calculated phenotypes (using LCA) for dietary patterns at age 3 and 5 years and then used LTA to investigate changes in the subgroups over time. Similarly, development change was explored by Swanson et al. (59) who measured eating disorder symptoms in girls in five age groups from preadolescence, early adolescence, late adolescence and two young adulthood periods and calculated transition probabilities from the latent classes at age 9–12 to classes at age 19–22. Latent transition probabilities following LCA has also been used to investigate learning outcomes (55) in a way that could provide a parallel for the examination of eating-based intervention outcomes. These approaches examine transitions between groups, however changes over time are also usefully explored with modeling of trajectories.
Trajectories Modeling
Nagin (60) argued that charting and understanding trajectories in longitudinal research is fundamental to knowledge about development, including the possibility that sub-groups might follow distinct trajectories. Trajectory analyses in longitudinal research might inform the investigation of multifinality (a common starting point, but then divergence of trajectories), equifinality (different starting points, but convergence on a common end point, possibly via different routes or paths), and fanning (increasing interindividual differences in trajectories over time) (17, 61).
There are two kinds of analytical strategies for examining trajectories of ASR-related eating behaviors and the associated outcomes of weight gain, adiposity or BMI. The first involves the investigation of trajectories for individual variables. The second approach has been to examine trajectories that include multiple variables in the one analysis.
Longitudinal Studies of Trajectories of Single Variables: Latent Growth Mixture Modeling
GMM is a statistical approach for modeling the average rate of individual change, or trajectories, across three or more time points. Whilst LTA involves the identification of latent subgroups and then the calculation of transitions, GMM models include the trajectories when calculating the latent subgroups (16). GMM is an extension of the traditional latent growth curve model (62) and includes latent class growth analysis (LCGA), a simplified GMM, and group-based trajectory modeling (GBTM), a special case of LCGA that assumes error variances are the same for all latent subgroups (18, 21, 32, 33, 60, 63–68). Unlike conventional latent growth curve modeling which assumes the same pattern of growth corresponds to the whole population, GMM takes into account population heterogeneity and identifies latent subgroups of individual growth patterns, or developmental trajectories (62).
A number of authors compare the assumptions and use of the main approaches to GMM (18, 32, 43, 69). Nagin and Odgers (43) argue that while there are technical differences between these approaches (i.e., they make different assumptions about the distribution of trajectories in the population), they are all designed to assign individuals into trajectory groups. For example, LCGA is a restricted version of GMM that constrains the variations (i.e., variances and covariances) within each class to zero and as a consequence reduces the number of parameters and simplifies model selection (70). LCGA assumes that all individual growth trajectories within classes are homogeneous (71) and is often recommended as a first step in the exploration of possible latent classes. GMM relaxes the assumption that all individuals in a class are from a single homogenous population (67) and estimates all growth factors (e.g., means, variances and covariances). However, such increases in model complexity may lead to estimation difficulties, including non-convergence or non-optimal latent subgroup solutions; the model chosen should be one that best fits the data and leads to meaningful subgroup solutions based on substantive prior theory (62). Berlin et al. discuss when and how a researcher might use LCGA and GMM, including a step-by-step account of processes in the identification of latent trajectory subgroups (18, 48). After conducting Monte Carlo simulations of synthetic data, Den Teuling et al. (69) concluded that GMM provided the “best overall performance.”
A common example of phenotypes defined in terms of trajectories of ASR-related constructs with a single variable is trajectories for weight gain or BMI. Norris et al. (72), for instance, examined weight trajectories from 0 to 60 months of age using GMM to identify five groups of individuals with different average trajectories. The subgroups included “average,” “high-decreasing,” and “stable-high” BMI trajectories. They then examined maternal (e.g., maternal BMI and education), family and birth characteristics of children associated with the different trajectories. Becnal and Williams (64) and Ventura et al. (73) followed a similar approach, using GMM to identify several weight trajectories. Risk factors were included as covariates and health outcomes associated with the trajectories were examined. In a birth cohort, van Rossem et al. (74) used LCGA to investigate trajectories for BMI until age 11 years. Trajectories of persistent overweight and overweight reduction were subsequently found to be related to early-life and parent factors including parent overweight.
Trajectories of individual ASR-related eating behaviors have also been explored. Derks et al. (75) used LCGA to examine trajectories for individual CEBQ scales assessed at ages 4 and 10 years. They found three patterns for emotional overeating and five patterns for food responsiveness, but no subgroups for enjoyment of food and satiety responsiveness. Follow-up regression analyses enabled them to explore early life predictors of each of the trajectories. Herle et al. (70) also used LCGA to investigate trajectories of child eating behaviors in the first 10 years of life from parent reports. They reported a number of trajectories for each of the single variables of overeating, undereating and fussy eating. The eating trajectories were associated with later zBMI in meaningful ways and were also found to be predictive of later eating disorder diagnosis.
These examples illustrate the potential contribution of person-centered trajectory analyses to the identification of developmental patterns for weight gain and ASR-related eating behaviors. They also show how predictors or risk factors can be related to trajectories as well as relationships between trajectories and outcomes.
Longitudinal Studies of Multiple Trajectories: Multi-Trajectory Modeling
Rather than using a single trajectory variable to assign individuals to a phenotype, multi-trajectory modeling, or multivariate GBTM, approach defines trajectory phenotypes using multiple and distinct trajectories variables (76). It is a variation of univariate GBTM. It defines a trajectory group in terms of trajectories for multiple indicators and takes account of the interrelationships among the indicators in a multivariate design. Nagin et al. (76) provide two illustrative examples and argue there is a need to sharpen guidelines for model selection and evaluation. Their first illustrative example was from male subjects in the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study with measures at different ages from 3 to 38 years. There were five trajectory groups, defined by the patterns of trajectories on three physiological outcome variables. The second illustrative example was from the Montreal-based longitudinal study of 1,037 males with measures from ages 6 to 17 years. The analysis yielded five trajectory groups based on the pattern of trajectories of four individual variables.
We illustrate this multivariate approach to the investigation of latent trajectory phenotypes for ASR-related eating behaviors with two studies. First, Epstein et al. (77) measured trajectories of food habituation to salty, sweet and savory foods in a sample of 8–12 year-old children. GBTM was used to identify individual trajectory phenotypes for the three foods and multivariate GBTM was used to determine trajectories for the combination of foods. The habituation phenotypes (such as “rapidly decelerating habituation” vs. “slower to initiate the decelerating rate of responding”) were related to a measure of the reinforcing value of each of the foods. This approach is helpful in the examination of ASR, as it demonstrated that the children who habituated slower also found food more reinforcing than children with a rapid habituation phenotype, thereby providing new insights into possible processes in ASR in childhood.
Boutelle et al. (78) provide a second example of multi-trajectory modeling. They assessed four child (mean age 10.4 years) appetitive traits at 3, 6, 12, and 24 months after baseline. Multivariate GBTM yielded three “trait trajectories of appetitive subgroups.” In a subgroup they labeled “high satiety responsiveness” there was an increasing pattern in satiety responsiveness, a decreasing pattern in food responsiveness and a low stable pattern in emotional eating and negative affect eating. The phenotypes, therefore, were characterized by a different pattern of trajectories for the four eating behaviors. Again, these phenotypes show the potential of multi-trajectory analyses for the investigation of ASR development because they show different combinations of eating behaviors in subgroups of children. The finding that only the high satiety responsiveness subgroup-maintained weight loss following a family-based treatment for children with overweight or obesity provides further support for separating appetite trajectory phenotype subgroups.
Some Strengths and Limitations of Using Person-Centered Approaches to Understand ASR
The literature includes extensive discussion of assumptions, processes and strategies in model selection, the inclusion of covariates and the limitations of these approaches to person-centered analyses (18, 32, 33, 42, 43, 48, 52, 79, 80). In all FMM, selecting the optimal number of latent subgroups requires the investigation of multiple model fit criteria (62). These may include information criteria statistics [e.g., Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), Consistent Akaike's Information Criteria (CAIC), and Approximate Weight of Evidence Criterion (AWE)], entropy values, the log-likelihood sample size, and likelihood ratio tests comparing the k-class and k-1 subgroup model [e.g., Bootstrap Likelihood Ration Test (BLRT), Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT)]. Bray and Dziak (52) argue that sometimes the use of FMM/LVMM is something of an art as well as a science in model selection. This is particularly so when the sample size is small relative to the model complexity, or the quality of the measurement model is poor. They suggest that both statistical fit and theoretical interpretability should be considered and at times it is better to select a model with a few interpretable classes even if the statistical fit is not optimal. Herle et al. (32) also comment on “non-science” aspects of model selection. This includes taking account of the size and interpretability of classes (70). Lubke and Luningham (42) in a related way refer to the considerable uncertainty around model selection. Several practical guides to the selection and reporting of latent subgroups are available (81–83). Whilst there is currently there is no “rule-of-thumb” regarding a minimal sample size for FMMs, the impact of sample size on determining the optimal number of latent subgroups in FMM have been investigated in several Monte Carlo simulation studies (81, 84).
In relation to latent transition analysis, there is debate and guidance in the literature about procedures and decisions in the conduct of latent transition analyses including sample size and selection of classes [e.g., (52)]. There are challenges associated with causal analysis using these approaches, as well as the possibility for these analyses to compare different theoretical models and for linking latent classes to predictors and outcomes. Berlin et al. (18) alert researchers to issues in using FMM arising from sample size. They argue that insufficient sample sizes can lead to convergence problems, improper solutions and a limited ability to identify meaningful subgroups. They also point out difficulties in determining adequate sample size, such in relation to reliability and the distribution of variables.
A key advantage of all FMM is the ability to directly incorporate predictors or antecedents, covariates, including time-varying covariates, and outcomes in the model (27, 32, 43, 52, 85, 86). Many techniques have been suggested for the examination of covariates [e.g., (87)]. One possibility is based on a regression model where class membership is predicted by the covariates, or classes are used to predict outcome variables such as zBMI (70). Or, where the covariates are partitioned into the latent classes (32). Here, Herle et al. included a discussion of two approaches to the investigation of covariates and their use. Marsh et al. (88) provided a discussion of the inclusion of correlates: when, why and what it assumes. They argued that their inclusion should not qualitatively change the classes but should make them more accurate and that covariates should be antecedent not concurrent or outcome. The inclusion of covariates in models is complex but a useful discussion is provided by Lubke and Luningham (42) on the theoretical bases of FMM/LVMM and the inclusion of covariates.
Finally, scholarship associated with analytic approaches following a person-centered perspective is an active and expanding field. Authors have discussed additional analysis options or extensions of LCA, LPA, LTA (52), and GMM (60, 62). Bray and Dziak indicate that the FMM/LVMM framework is flexible and permits the specification of different types of mixture models that include path models, factor models, survival models, growth curve models, and that structural equation models can be specified for multiple subgroups.
In summary, and overall, research on ASR-related phenotypes, including trajectory phenotypes (whether using individual constructs or a multitrajectory approach) is contributing to an understanding ASR and its development in childhood. The extensive conceptual and technical literature on person-centered strategies in the investigation of subgroup differences also shows the potential of this approach to contribute to knowledge about ASR and its development in childhood. At the same time, researchers should be cognizant of the assumptions and limitations of this approach, as well as the art vs. science aspects of subgroup identification. An important consideration is also that the phenotypes identified are clearly a product of the number and type of individual variables that have been measured. Further, the inclusion of covariates and outcomes provided opportunities to better understand the predictors or possible origins of ASR, influences on its development and effects on important development outcomes such as BMI and weight gain. Below, we comment further on the importance of phenotype research for advancing knowledge about ASR and its development. We argue for the need to expand measures into new domains of ASR-related constructs.
Discussion
Knowledge and understanding of ASR can be advanced in many ways: through conceptual and theoretical developments, improvements in research design and methods (especially measurement) and progress in approaches to data analysis and statistics. In this review we have taken a slice through some of these matters via the distinction between person-centered and variable-centered approaches to research, and then a focus on person-centered strategies.
In the discussion we first explore insights from extant person-centered approaches for the conceptualization and measurement of ASR in childhood together with suggestions for future research. Second, we discuss possibilities for combining person-centered and variable-centered approaches in scholarship about ASR and children's eating behaviors in childhood. Third, we argue that person-centered approaches could assist in the design of intervention strategies and in the measurement of intervention outcomes.
Insights From a Person-Centered Approach for Conceptualizing and Measuring ASR
Research on phenotypes from cross-sectional and longitudinal data has the potential to examine underlying processes and dimensions of ASR in childhood. This is apparent from the labels applied to the latent subgroups [e.g., “high satiety responsiveness” (78), “rapidly decelerating habituation” (77)] and the particular measures used to characterize them in the research reviewed here. This research can also contribute to knowledge about (a) antecedents, precursors, or correlates of ASR-related phenotypes, and (b) associations between phenotypes and outcomes (either developmental outcomes or outcomes from interventions).
Latent class/profile and trajectory analyses have been described as data driven and exploratory [e.g., (43, 89)]. Consistent with this description, findings from person-centered analyses should not be used to infer causality, but rather to generate hypotheses for future testing. Also in line with this description, Bergman and Trost (20) emphasize the vagueness of guiding theories in the case of person-centered approaches, especially about how the studied system operates, for example, about how different measures might interact and coordinate to form distinct phenotypes or how and why there could be different trajectories of ASR and its components. For example, in the Derks et al. (75) study although many patterns were found for each of the measured appetitive traits and these were linked to early life predictors, due to the absence of a clear theoretical foundation for the development of appetitive traits, it is unclear why, for instance, three patterns of emotional eating and 5 patterns for food responsiveness may arise. While recognizing the potential of person-centered approaches for advancing knowledge about ASR and its development, it is important to keep in mind that it needs to be built on sound theory as well as efforts to integrate it with variable-centered approaches. As we mention below, recent data analysis strategies that enable a more confirmatory approach and assessment of predictions are also helpful.
Because person-centered strategies in relation to ASR have been mainly exploratory, an important role of these strategies could be in the development and clarification of conceptualization and measurement. Central to a person-centered approach is the assumption that the individual is an integrated totality over time, with behaviors interwoven and interacting (20). This approach is suited to the exploration of possible developmental mechanisms and to inductive theory building (16). A complexity here is that while ASR could be changing over time, other developmental processes and changes are also occurring. This means that person-centered strategies to investigate the development of ASR will need to take account of wider developmental changes and processes.
Morin et al. (27) suggest that some areas of research might not be sufficiently advanced in theory and with a substantial enough body of results to generate clear hypotheses about the expected nature of profiles. They argue that when this is the case, construct validation is important, through showing that the profiles have heuristic and theoretical values and are meaningfully related to key correlates or outcomes. In this case, confirmatory approaches may be applied. Schmiege et al. (89) discuss approaches to confirmatory latent class analysis, including a dual sample approach and confirmatory testing of a latent class structure. As person-centered evidence on ASR expands, it is possible that these more stringent theoretical tests will become more important.
Fundamentally, phenotypes are suggestive of central components and processes in ASR. But the latent subgroups are limited and constrained by, and entirely reflect, the individual measures used. Person-centered strategies with a focus on ASR-related phenotypes will be better placed to contribute to conceptualization and measurement with the continued addition of individual measures of behaviors, characteristics, traits or processes based on emerging evidence about processes and individual differences in ASR. This includes evidence from biologically based research such as genetic susceptibility, psychoneurological measures, as well as measures about general self-regulation from psychology, neuroscience and from areas such as the effects of highly processed food and the rewarding value of food. When covariates and outcomes are included in the research design, person-centered strategies could progress knowledge about the possible origins of ASR, influences on the development of ASR, and developmental outcome associated with ASR.
Indeed research on ASR-related behaviors, characteristics, traits and processes has expanded considerably in recent years and there is now a growing set of possibilities for inclusion in research about ASR phenotypes in childhood including: temperament (such as impulsivity and effortful control) (90), Executive function (such as inhibitory control), genetic susceptibility, reward sensitivity, hedonic responses to food, cognitive function (91), cognitive control and negative affect (92), state and/or trait food cue reactivity (93), brain reward sensitivity to food cues (94), dietary measures, such as dietary fat or carbohydrates (95, 96), fructose consumption (97), intake of processed food (98), sensory sensitivity (99), neuroimaging functional connectivity (100), metabolomics and analysis of the gut microbiome (101, 102), measures of the social facilitation of eating (103), susceptibility to modeling (104), effects of portion size cues (105) and attachment security (106), behavioral and neural measures of appetitive traits such as through neuroimaging measures (107, 108). A helpful broadening of work on ASR phenotypes is also suggested by attention to endophenotypes where genetic predisposition and neural substrates as well as behavioral measures are included (107, 109–112).
However, the inclusion of many behaviors, characteristics, and traits to determine ASR phenotypes may be too computationally burdensome for the model-based person-centered approaches discussed in the present review. Future ASR research may need to employ machine learning, or data mining, methods to determine phenotypes from large and complex datasets (113–115).
Combining Person-Centered and Variable-Centered Strategies
Comparisons of person-centered and variable-centered approaches to research have highlighted their differences in assumptions, purpose, sampling, research questions, analytic approach and strengths (16). The two approaches can also be complementary (16, 20, 116). Derks et al. (75) for example, demonstrated how combining the two approaches can provide information about children's ASR. They identified different trajectory patterns of children's eating behaviors using LCGA (person centered) and then investigated the early life or other predictors of those patterns (variable centered). Predictors could include child and family characteristics (51) and BMI-z (50) or socio-demographic and clinical characteristics (117). Another option to combine the two approaches is to first identify latent profiles of ASR and use these as predictors of subsequent outcomes such as BMI or diet [e.g., (118)]. Much of the research on risk factors for obesity or weight gain has examined individual predictor variables. This type of research could be helpfully expanded to include relationships between ASR phenotypes and subsequent measures of weight, thereby gaining greater specificity about potential risks as well as possible processes associated with weight gain and obesity.
Phenotype and other person-centered analyses also enable reflections on theoretical models such as a biopsychosocial approach (119) that incorporate variable and person-centered elements. For instance, these analyses could enable the examination of how biological, psychological and social measures combine in the formation of phenotypes, how they differ from one phenotype to another, and then how the phenotypes relate to model outcomes such as weight gain and adiposity. It would also be possible to incorporate parent and child measures from cross-lagged designs to examine transactional processes via phenotypes and co-variate analyses. Other designs could also provide important insights, such as parallel process latent growth modeling which could investigate ASR trajectories alongside other developmental trajectories, such as BMI or emotion regulation.
In the present review we highlighted the potential of person-centered strategies for the conceptualization and measurement of ASR in childhood and infancy. In contrast to a systematic review, the emphasis of the present review was not on an assessment of the evidence or findings from person-centered strategies. Aligned to the purpose of the review, the literature chosen was supportive rather than critical of these strategies and does not cover nor assess the suitability or quality of all research on person centered approaches to advancing the conceptualization of ASR. We briefly discussed the roles and contributions of person-centered vs. variable centered approaches. There is value in further appraisal of these two approaches to the conceptualization and measurement of ASR in infancy and childhood. Further, as evidence accumulates, there is a need for systematic reviews to appraise and synthesize evidence from the two approaches.
Person-Centered Strategies in Intervention Design and Measurement of Outcomes
There is scope for person-centered approaches to contribute to the design of intervention strategies and in the measurement of outcomes. Person-centered analyses could better inform intervention strategies by a greater focus on the specificity arising from the identification of phenotype subgroups that could be tied to personalized intervention strategies (44, 78, 117). Phenotype and trajectory analyses could be used to examine intervention outcomes such as through changes in phenotypes following an intervention, and transitions from one phenotype to another, such as has been described in the literature on teaching and learning (40, 120). Finally, in contributing to intervention design and the measurement of outcomes, person-centered approaches could contribute to knowledge about possible developmental processes and assist theory development.
Conclusion
Person-centered strategies can make an important contribution to advances in the conceptualization and measurement of ASR in children, including to an understanding of developmental paths and processes. This appears to be especially so for person-centered strategies that explore phenotypes, whether based on cross-sectional data or trajectories. The potential contribution seems to be enhanced when combined with variable centered approaches that include predictors or co-variates and that examine outcomes. Possible gains from person-centered approaches should be strengthened by further evidence about individual skills, traits and behaviors that comprise ASR, as well as increased evidence about ASR processes and developmental change/trajectories. There is also a need for overall theory development, more confirmatory research and a greater integration with variable-centered approaches. Finally, evidence about children's appetitive phenotypes and trajectories could assist in the design and measurement of intervention outcomes.
Author Contributions
The review was initiated by CGR and AR. All authors jointly written, contributed to the article, and approved the submitted version.
Funding
Funding for open access fees were provided by the School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University. RL was supported by a National Health & Medical Research Council Emerging Leader Fellowship (APP1175250). The funding bodies had no role in the writing of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Abbreviations
FMM, Finite mixture modeling; LCA, Latent class analysis; LCGA, Latent class growth analysis; LPA, Latent profile analysis; LTA, Latent transition analysis; GBTM, Group-based trajectory modeling; GMM, Latent growth mixture modeling; LVMM, Latent variable mixture modeling; CEBQ, Children's Eating Behavior Questionnaire; BMI, Body Mass Index; EAH, Eating in the absence of hunger; ASR, Appetite self-regulation.
References
1. Hughes SO, Power TG, O'Connor TM, Orlet Fisher J. Executive functioning, emotion regulation, eating self-regulation, and weight status in low-income preschool children: how do they relate? Appetite. (2015) 89:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.009
2. Saltzman JA, Fiese BH, Bost KK, McBride BA. Development of appetite self-regulation: integrating perspectives from attachment and family systems theory. Child Dev Perspect. (2018) 12:51–7. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12254
3. Bellisle F, Drewnowski A, Anderson GH, Westerterp-Plantenga M, Martin CK. Sweetness, satiation, and satiety. J Nutr. (2012) 142:1149S−54S. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.149583
4. Blundell J, de Graaf C, Hulshof T, Jebb S, Livingstone B, Lluch A, et al. Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev. (2010) 11:251–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00714.x
5. Forde CG. Measuring satiation and satiety. In: Ares G, Varela P, editors. Methods in Consumer Research. Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing (2018). p. 151–82.
6. Gibbons C, Hopkins M, Beaulieu K, Oustric P, Blundell JE. Issues in measuring and interpreting human appetite (satiety/satiation) and its contribution to obesity. Curr Obes Rep. (2019) 8:77–87. doi: 10.1007/s13679-019-00340-6
7. Chambers L. Food texture and the satiety cascade. Nutrition Bulletin. (2016) 41:277–82. doi: 10.1111/nbu.12221
8. Russell A, Russell CG. Appetite self-regulation declines across childhood while general self-regulation improves: a narrative review of the origins and development of appetite self-regulation. Appetite. (2021) 162:105178. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105178
9. Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2001) 42:963–70. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00792
10. Bridgett DJ, Burt NM, Edwards ES, Deater-Deckard K. Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: a multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychol Bull. (2015) 141:602–54. doi: 10.1037/a0038662
11. Nigg JT. Annual Research Review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2017) 58:361–83. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675
12. Russell CG, Russell A. “Food” and “non-food” self-regulation in childhood: a review and reciprocal analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2020) 17:33. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00928-5
13. Kemps E, Goossens L, Petersen J, Verbeken S, Vervoort L, Braet C. Evidence for enhancing childhood obesity treatment from a dual-process perspective: a systematic literature review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2020) 77:101840. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101840
14. Laursen BP, Hoff E. Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill Palmer Quart. (2006) 52:377–89. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0029
15. Eye AV, Bogat GA. Person-oriented and variable-oriented research: concepts, results, and development. Merrill Palmer Quart. (2006) 52:390–420. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0032
16. Howard MC, Hoffman ME. Variable-centered, person-centered, and person-specific approaches: where theory meets the method. Organ Res Methods. (2018) 21:846–76. doi: 10.1177/1094428117744021
17. Curran PJ, Willoughby MT. Implications of latent trajectory models for the study of developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. (2003) 15:581–612. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000300
18. Berlin KS, Parra GR, Williams NA. An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (part 2): longitudinal latent class growth analysis and growth mixture models. J Pediatr Psychol. (2014) 39:188–203. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst085
19. Raufelder D, Jagenow D, Hoferichter F, Drury KM. The person-oriented approach in the field of educational psychology. Problems Psychol 21st Century. (2013) 5:79–88.
20. Bergman LR, Trost K. The person-oriented versus the variable-oriented approach: are they complementary, opposites, or exploring different worlds? Merrill Palmer Quart. (2006) 52:601–32. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0023
21. Masyn KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In: Little TD, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology: Vol 2: Statistical Analysis. 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2013). p. 551–611.
22. Galindo L, Power TG, Beck AD, Fisher JO, O'Connor TM, Hughes SO. Predicting preschool children's eating in the absence of hunger from maternal pressure to eat: a longitudinal study of low-income, Latina mothers. Appetite. (2018) 120:281–6. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.007
23. Gearhardt AN, Yokum S, Harris JL, Epstein LH, Lumeng JC. Neural response to fast food commercials in adolescents predicts intake. Am J Clin Nutr. (2020) 111:493–502. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz305
24. Giuliani NR, Kelly NR. Delay of gratification predicts eating in the absence of hunger in preschool-aged children. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:650046. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.650046
25. Miller AL, Gearhardt AN, Retzloff L, Sturza J, Kaciroti N, Lumeng JC. Early childhood stress and child age predict longitudinal increases in obesogenic eating among low-income children. Acad Pediatr. (2018) 18:685–91. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2018.01.007
26. Stifter CA, Moding KJ. Infant temperament and parent use of food to soothe predict change in weight-for-length across infancy: early risk factors for childhood obesity. Int J Obes. (2018) 42:1631–8. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0006-4
27. Morin AJS, Bujacz A, Gagné M. Person-centered methodologies in the organizational sciences. Organ Res Methods. (2018) 21:803–13. doi: 10.1177/1094428118773856
28. D'Souza NJ, Downing K, Abbott G, Orellana L, Lioret S, Campbell KJ, et al. A comparison of children's diet and movement behaviour patterns derived from three unsupervised multivariate methods. PLoS ONE. (2021) 16:e0255203. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255203
29. D'Souza NJ, Kuswara K, Zheng M, Leech R, Downing KL, Lioret S, et al. A systematic review of lifestyle patterns and their association with adiposity in children aged 5-12 years. Obes Rev. (2020) 21:e13029. doi: 10.1111/obr.13029
30. DiStefano C, Kamphaus RW. Investigating subtypes of child development. Educ Psychol Meas. (2016) 66:778–94. doi: 10.1177/0013164405284033
31. McLachlan GJ, Lee SX, Rathnayake SI. Finite mixture models. Ann Rev Stat Appl. (2019) 6:355–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev-statistics-031017-100325
32. Herle M, Micali N, Abdulkadir M, Loos R, Bryant-Waugh R, Hubel C, et al. Identifying typical trajectories in longitudinal data: modelling strategies and interpretations. Eur J Epidemiol. (2020) 35:205–22. doi: 10.1007/s10654-020-00615-6
33. Ram N, Grimm KJ. Growth mixture modeling: a method for identifying differences in longitudinal change among unobserved groups. Int J Behav Dev. (2009) 33:565–76. doi: 10.1177/0165025409343765
34. Kral TVE, Moore RH, Chittams J, Jones E, O'Malley L, Fisher JO. Identifying behavioral phenotypes for childhood obesity. Appetite. (2018) 127:87–96. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.04.021
35. Jennings KM, Loth KA, Tate AD, Miner MH, Berge JM. Application of latent profile analysis to define subgroups of parenting styles and food parenting practices. Appetite. (2019) 139:8–18. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.04.001
36. Burnett AJ, Lacy KE, Russell CG, Spence AC, Worsley A, Lamb KE. Groups of mothers based on feeding practices and their associations with dietary quality of pre-school children: a latent profile analysis. Appetite. (2021) 168:105754. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105754
37. Micalizzi L, Wang M, Saudino KJ. Difficult temperament and negative parenting in early childhood: a genetically informed cross-lagged analysis. Dev Sci. (2017) 20:1–14. doi: 10.1111/desc.12355
38. Blair C, Raver CC. Individual development and evolution: experiential canalization of self-regulation. Dev Psychol. (2012) 48:647–57. doi: 10.1037/a0026472
39. Beam CR, Turkheimer E. Phenotype-environment correlations in longitudinal twin models. Dev Psychopathol. (2013) 25:7–16. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000867
40. Hickendorff M, Edelsbrunner PA, McMullen J, Schneider M, Trezise K. Informative tools for characterizing individual differences in learning: latent class, latent profile, and latent transition analysis. Learn Individ Differ. (2018) 66:4–15. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.001
41. Mori M, Krumholz HM, Allore HG. Using latent class analysis to identify hidden clinical phenotypes. JAMA. (2020) 324:700–1. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.2278
42. Lubke GH, Luningham J. Fitting latent variable mixture models. Behav Res Ther. (2017) 98:91–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.04.003
43. Nagin DS, Odgers CL. Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2010) 6:109–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413
44. Acosta A, Camilleri M, Abu Dayyeh B, Calderon G, Gonzalez D, McRae A, et al. Selection of antiobesity medications based on phenotypes enhances weight loss: a pragmatic trial in an obesity clinic. Obesity. (2021) 29:662–71. doi: 10.1002/oby.23120
45. Hittner JB, Faith MS. Typology of emergent eating patterns in early childhood. Eat Behav. (2011) 12:242–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.06.001
46. Vervoort L, Naets T, Goossens L, Verbeken S, Claes L, Tanghe A, et al. Subtyping youngsters with obesity: a theory-based cluster analysis. Appetite. (2021) 168:105723. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105723
47. Muthen B, Muthen LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res. (2000) 24:882–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x
48. Berlin KS, Williams NA, Parra GR. An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (part 1): overview and cross-sectional latent class and latent profile analyses. J Pediatr Psychol. (2014) 39:174–87. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst084
49. Leech RM, Worsley A, Timperio A, McNaughton SA. Temporal eating patterns: a latent class analysis approach. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2017) 14:3. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0459-6
50. Boutelle KN, Peterson CB, Crosby RD, Rydell SA, Zucker N, Harnack L. Overeating phenotypes in overweight and obese children. Appetite. (2014) 76:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.01.076
51. Tharner A, Jansen PW., Kiefte-de Jong JC, Moll HA, van der Ende J, Jaddoe VW, et al. Toward an operative diagnosis of fussy/picky eating: a latent profile approach in a population-based cohort. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2014) 11:14. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-11-14
52. Bray BC, Dziak JJ. Commentary on latent class, latent profile, and latent transition analysis for characterizing individual differences in learning. Learn Individ Differ. (2018) 66:105–10. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.06.001
53. Ryoo JH, Wang C, Swearer SM, Hull M, Shi D. Longitudinal model building using latent transition analysis: an example using school bullying data. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00675
54. McMullen J, Hickendorff M. Latent variable mixture models in research on learning and individual differences. Learn Individ Differ. (2018) 66:1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.05.008
55. Schulz A, Leuders T. Learning trajectories towards strategy proficiency in multi-digit division – a latent transition analysis of strategy and error profiles. Learn Individ Differ. (2018) 66:54–69. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.04.014
56. Rinne LF, Ye A, Jordan NC. Development of fraction comparison strategies: a latent transition analysis. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53:713–30. doi: 10.1037/dev0000275
58. Pitt E, Cameron CM, Thornton L, Gallegos D, Filus A, Ng SK, et al. Dietary patterns of Australian children at three and five years of age and their changes over time: a latent class and latent transition analysis. Appetite.s (2018) 129:207–16. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.07.008
59. Swanson SA, Horton NJ, Crosby RD, Micali N, Sonneville KR, Eddy K, et al. A latent class analysis to empirically describe eating disorders through developmental stages. Int J Eat Disord. (2014) 47:762–72. doi: 10.1002/eat.22308
60. Nagin DS. Group-based trajectory modeling: an overview. Ann Nutr Metab. (2014) 65:205–10. doi: 10.1159/000360229
61. Hinnant B, Schulenberg J, Jager J. Multifinality, equifinality, and fanning: developmental concepts and statistical implications. Int J Behav Dev. (2021) 1–14. doi: 10.1177/01650254211020402
62. Wickrama K, Lee TK, O'Neal CW, Lorenz F. Higher-Order Growth Curves and Mixture Modeling with Mplus. New York, NY: Routledge. (2016).
63. Gomez R, Skilbeck C, Thomas M, Slatyer M. Growth mixture modeling of depression symptoms following traumatic brain injury. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1320. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01320
64. Becnel JN, Williams AL. Using latent class growth modeling to examine longitudinal patterns of body mass index change from adolescence to adulthood. J Acad Nutr Diet. (2019) 119:1875–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2019.04.025
65. Lai D, Xu H, Koller D, Foroud T, Gao S, A. Multivariate finite mixture latent trajectory model with application to dementia studies. J Appl Stat. (2016) 43:2503–23. doi: 10.1080/02664763.2016.1141181
66. Muthen B. Latent variable analysis: growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In: Kaplan D, editor. The SAGE Handbook of Quantitative Methodology for the Social Sciences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications (2004). p. 345–68.
67. Hawrilenko M, Masyn KE, Cerutti J, Dunn EC. Individual differences in the stability and change of childhood depression: a growth mixture model with structured residuals. Child Dev. (2021) 92:e343–63. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13502
68. van der Nest G, Lima Passos V, Candel MJJM, van Breukelen GJP. An overview of mixture modelling for latent evolutions in longitudinal data: modelling approaches, fit statistics and software. Adv Life Course Res. (2020) 43:100323. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2019.100323
69. Den Teuling NGP, Pauws SC, van den Heuvel ER. A comparison of methods for clustering longitudinal data with slowly changing trends. Commun Stat Simul Comput. (2021) 1–28. doi: 10.1080/03610918.2020.1861464
70. Herle M, Stavola B, Hubel C, Ferreira DLS, Abdulkadir M, Yilmaz Z, et al. Eating behavior trajectories in the first 10 years of life and their relationship with BMI. Int J Obes. (2020) 44:1766–75. doi: 10.1038/s41366-020-0581-z
71. Nagin DS. Analyzing developmental trajectories: a semiparametric, group-based approach. Psychol Methods. (1999) 4:139–57. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.2.139
72. Norris T, Mansukoski L, Gilthorpe MS, Hamer M, Hardy R, Howe LD, et al. Early childhood weight gain: latent patterns and body composition outcomes. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. (2021) 35:557–68. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12754
73. Ventura AK, Loken E, Birch LL. Developmental trajectories of girls' BMI across childhood and adolescence. Obesity. (2009) 17:2067–74. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.123
74. van Rossem L, Wijga AH, Brunekreef B, de Jongste JC, Kerkhof M, Postma DS, et al. Overweight in infancy: which pre- and perinatal factors determine overweight persistence or reduction? A birth cohort followed for 11 years. Ann Nutr Metab. (2014) 65:211–9. doi: 10.1159/000360305
75. Derks IPM, Bolhuis K, Sijbrands EJG, Gaillard R, Hillegers MHJ, Jansen PW. Predictors and patterns of eating behaviors across childhood: results from The Generation R study. Appetite. (2019) 141:104295. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.05.026
76. Nagin DS, Jones BL, Passos VL, Tremblay RE. Group-based multi-trajectory modeling. Stat Methods Med Res. (2018) 27:2015–23. doi: 10.1177/0962280216673085
77. Epstein LH, Carr KA, O'Brien A, Paluch RA, Temple JL. High reinforcing value of food is related to slow habituation to food. Eat Behav. (2020) 38:101414. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2020.101414
78. Boutelle KN, Kang Sim DE, Manzano M, Rhee KE, Crow SJ, Strong DR. Role of appetitive phenotype trajectory groups on child body weight during a family-based treatment for children with overweight or obesity. Int J Obes. (2019) 43:2302–8. doi: 10.1038/s41366-019-0463-4
79. Davies CE, Glonek GF, Giles LC. The impact of covariance misspecification in group-based trajectory models for longitudinal data with non-stationary covariance structure. Stat Methods Med Res. (2017) 26:1982–91. doi: 10.1177/0962280215598806
80. Herle M, Stavola B, Hubel C, Abdulkadir M, Ferreira DS, Loos RJF, et al. A longitudinal study of eating behaviours in childhood and later eating disorder behaviours and diagnoses. Br J Psychiatry. (2020) 216:113–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.174
81. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a monte carlo simulation study. Struct Equat Model Multidiscipl J. (2007) 14:535–69. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396
82. Weller BE, Bowen NK, Faubert SJ. Latent class analysis: a guide to best practice. J Black Psychol. (2020) 46:287–311. doi: 10.1177/0095798420930932
83. van de Schoot R, Sijbrandij M, Winter SD, Depaoli S, Vermunt JK. The GRoLTS-checklist: guidelines for reporting on latent trajectory studies. Struct Equat Model Multidiscipl J. (2016) 24:451–67. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2016.1247646
84. Tein JY, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct Equ Model. (2013) 20:640–57. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2013.824781
85. Twisk J. Is it necessary to classify developmental trajectories over time? A critical note. Ann Nutr Metab. (2014) 65:236–40. doi: 10.1159/000362506
86. Francis LA, Rollins BY, Bryce CI, Granger DA. Biobehavioral dysregulation and its association with obesity and severe obesity trajectories from 2 to 15 years of age: a longitudinal study. Obesity. (2020) 28:830–9. doi: 10.1002/oby.22762
87. Genge E, A. latent class analysis of the public attitude towards the euro adoption in Poland. Adv Data Anal Classif. (2013) 8:427–42. doi: 10.1007/s11634-013-0156-0
88. Marsh HW, Lüdtke O, Trautwein U, Morin AJS. Classical latent profile analysis of academic self-concept dimensions: synergy of person- and variable-centered approaches to theoretical models of self-concept. Struct Equat Model Multidiscipl J. (2009) 16:191–225. doi: 10.1080/10705510902751010
89. Schmiege SJ, Masyn KE, Bryan AD. Confirmatory latent class analysis. Organ Res Methods. (2018) 21:983–1001. doi: 10.1177/1094428117747689
90. Saelens BE, Melhorn SJ, Rowland MG, Scholz K, De Leon MRB, Elfers CT, et al. General and food-specific impulsivity and inhibition related to weight management. Child Obes. (2021) 1–8. doi: 10.1089/chi.2021.0080
91. Smith L, Toussaint L, Micoli A, Lynch B. Obesity, putative biological mediators, and cognitive function in a national sample of children and adolescents. Prev Med. (2021) 150:106659. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106659
92. Vainik U, Garcia-Garcia I, Dagher A. Uncontrolled eating: a unifying heritable trait linked with obesity, overeating, personality and the brain. Eur J Neurosci. (2019) 50:2430–45. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14352
93. Reents J, Pedersen A. Differences in food craving in individuals with obesity with and without binge eating disorder. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:660880. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.660880
94. Han P, Roitzsch C, Horstmann A, Possel M, Hummel T. Increased brain reward responsivity to food-related odors in obesity. Obesity. (2021) 29:1138–45. doi: 10.1002/oby.23170
95. Wallace CW, Fordahl SC. Obesity and dietary fat influence dopamine neurotransmission: exploring the convergence of metabolic state, physiological stress, and inflammation on dopaminergic control of food intake. Nutr Res Rev. (2021) 1–16. doi: 10.1017/S0954422421000196
96. Loper H, Leinen M, Bassoff L, Sample J, Romero-Ortega M, Gustafson KJ, et al. Both high fat and high carbohydrate diets impair vagus nerve signaling of satiety. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:10394. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-89465-0
97. Payant MA, Chee MJ. Neural mechanisms underlying the role of fructose in overfeeding. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:346–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.034
98. Small DM, DiFeliceantonio AG. Processed foods and food reward. Science. (2019) 363:346–7. doi: 10.1126/science.aav0556
99. Zickgraf HF, Elkins A. Sensory sensitivity mediates the relationship between anxiety and picky eating in children/ adolescents ages 8–17, and in college undergraduates: a replication and age-upward extension. Appetite. (2018) 128:333–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.023
100. Voigt K, Razi A, Harding IH, Andrews ZB, Verdejo-Garcia A. Neural network modelling reveals changes in directional connectivity between cortical and hypothalamic regions with increased BMI. Int J Obes. (2021) 45, 2447–54. doi: 10.1101/2020.05.10.087619
101. Gawlik A, Salonen A, Jian C, Yanover C, Antosz A, Shmoish M, et al. Personalized approach to childhood obesity: lessons from gut microbiota and omics studies. Narrative review and insights from the 29th European childhood obesity congress. Pediatr Obes. (2021) 16:e12835. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12835
102. Baranowski T, Motil KJ, Moreno JP. Multi-etiological perspective on child obesity prevention. Curr Nutr Rep. (2019) 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s13668-019-0256-3
103. Ruddock HK, Brunstrom JM, Higgs PS. The social facilitation of eating: why does the mere presence of others cause an increase in energy intake? Physiol Behav. (2021) 240:113539. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113539
104. Blissett J. Effects of modeling on children's eating behavior. In: Lumeng JCFJ, editor. Pediatric Food Preferences and Eating Behaviors. London: Academic Press (2018). p. 53–72.
105. Robertson DA, Lavin C, Lunn PD. Can visual cues to portion size reduce the number of portions of consumed? Two randomized controlled trials. Ann Behav Med. (2021) 55:746–57. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa098
106. Beijers R, Miragall M, van den Berg Y, Konttinen H, van Strien T. Parent–infant attachment insecurity and emotional eating in adolescence: mediation through emotion suppression and alexithymia. Nutrients. (2021) 13:1662. doi: 10.3390/nu13051662
107. Carnell S, Benson L, Pryor K, Driggin E. Appetitive traits from infancy to adolescence: using behavioral and neural measures to investigate obesity risk. Physiol Behav. (2013) 121:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.02.015
108. Thapaliya G, Sadler JR, Jansen E, Carnell S. Obesity and appetite: evidence for a neurobehavioral model of obesity risk and maintenance. In: Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. Second Edition. Elsevier (2021) 347–59. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-819641-0.00142-0
109. Byrne M, Tanofsky-Kraff M. Development of loss of control eating. In: Lumeng JCFJ, editor. Pediatric Food Preferences and Eating Behaviors. London: Academic Press (2018). p. 233–54.
110. Goldsmith HH, Davidson RJ. Disambiguating the components of emotion regulation. Child Dev. (2004) 75:361–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00678.x
111. Kan C, Herle M, Treasure J, Jones A, Rijsdijk F, Llewellyn C. Common etiological architecture underlying reward responsiveness, externally driven eating behaviors, and BMI in childhood: findings from the Gemini twin cohort. Int J Obes. (2020) 44:2064–74. doi: 10.1038/s41366-020-0605-8
112. Michaud A, Vainik U, Garcia-Garcia I, Dagher A. Overlapping neural endophenotypes in addiction and obesity. Front Endocrinol. (2017) 8:127. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00127
113. Farrahi V, Niemela M, Karmeniemi M, Puhakka S, Kangas M, Korpelainen R, et al. Correlates of physical activity behavior in adults: a data mining approach. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2020) 17:94. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00996-7
114. Lin Z, Feng W, Liu Y, Ma C, Arefan D, Zhou D, et al. Machine learning to identify metabolic subtypes of obesity: a multi-center study. Front Endocrinol. (2021) 12:713592. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.713592
115. Niemela M, Kangas M, Farrahi V, Kiviniemi A, Leinonen AM, Ahola R, et al. Intensity and temporal patterns of physical activity and cardiovascular disease risk in midlife. Prev Med. (2019) 124:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.04.023
116. Meeusen C, Meuleman B, Abts K, Bergh R. Comparing a variable-centered and a person-centered approach to the structure of prejudice. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2017) 9:645–55. doi: 10.1177/1948550617720273
117. Clairman H, Dettmer E, Buchholz A, Cordeiro K, Ibrahim Q, Maximova K, et al. Pathways to eating in children and adolescents with obesity. Int J Obes. (2019) 43:1193–201. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0271-2
118. Cao H, Liang Y, Li X, Zhu L, Wu L, Liu H, et al. Childhood maltreatment and affective symptoms and severity of drug addiction among Chinese male drug users: variable-centered and person-centered approaches. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. (2020) 30:1007–27. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2020.1796874
119. Russell CG, Russell A, A. biopsychosocial approach to processes and pathways in the development of overweight and obesity in childhood: insights from developmental theory and research. Obes Rev. (2019) 20:725–49. doi: 10.1111/obr.12838
Keywords: phenotypes, appetite regulation, mixture models, developmental trajectories, latent class analysis, unobserved (or underlying) heterogeneity, eating behavior, child
Citation: Russell A, Leech RM and Russell CG (2021) Conceptualizing and Measuring Appetite Self-Regulation Phenotypes and Trajectories in Childhood: A Review of Person-Centered Strategies. Front. Nutr. 8:799035. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.799035
Received: 21 October 2021; Accepted: 30 November 2021;
Published: 22 December 2021.
Edited by:
Jena Shaw Tronieri, University of Pennsylvania, United StatesReviewed by:
Jeffrey Liew, Texas A&M University, United StatesLori Anne Francis, The Pennsylvania State University (PSU), United States
Copyright © 2021 Russell, Leech and Russell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Catherine G. Russell, Z2VvcmdpZS5ydXNzZWxsJiN4MDAwNDA7ZGVha2luLmVkdS5hdQ==