- 1INSERM UMR1093-CAPS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Dijon, France
- 2Centre d'Expertise de la Performance, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Dijon, France
- 3Nexira, Rouen, France
Background: Salvia (sage) supplementation has been shown to improve the cognition function in healthy individuals or patients (e.g., attention, memory). To date, no study has explored its relevancy in the context of sporting performance. The aim of this study was to explore the acute effects of a combination of Salvia officinalis and Salvia lavandulaefolia on cognitive function in athletes performing a fatiguing cycling task.
Methods: Twenty-six volunteers were included in this cross-over, randomized, double-bind vs. placebo trial. Two hours before the two experimental sessions (here called SAGE and PLACEBO), volunteers randomly received a supplementation of sage or placebo. During each experimental session, participants were tested at four occasions while cycling during a warm-up, in the middle and at the end of a fatiguing task and after a short 5-min recovery. Tests included a Stroop task, a simple reaction time task, and a backward digit span memory task. Heart rate and rating of perceived exertion (RPE) were also measured at the beginning of the four test sessions.
Results: Heart rate was significantly greater during the fatiguing exercise than during warm-up and recovery (P < 0.001) without any supplementation effect. RPE was greater during the fatiguing exercise than during warm-up and recovery (P < 0.001). Moreover, RPE was significantly lower during the SAGE session as compared to PLACEBO (P = 0.002). Reaction time was not altered during the exercise but was significantly shorter with SAGE as compared to PLACEBO (P = 0.023). The Stroop task only revealed significantly longer reaction time during warm-up as compared to recovery (P = 0.02) independently of the supplementation. The digit span memory test revealed a significant greater span score with SAGE as compared to PLACEBO (P = 0.044).
Conclusion: The combination of Salvia improved the cognitive functions (perceived exertion, working memory, and reaction time). The positive effects were obtained in fresh condition and were maintained with fatigue.
Introduction
Multiple nutritional strategies could be used to improve performance of competitive athletes. Long-term (chronic) diets may be beneficial for multiple physiological adaptations such as gains of muscle mass using proteins (1). Acute supplementations may also enhance numerous aspects of performance (2). Supplementation may act at different physiological levels, including the central nervous system. For instance, supplementation could reduce serotonin synthesis or increase the concentration of neurotransmitters and therefore lower mental fatigue or increase the cognitive function (i.e., attention or vigilance) (2, 3). Confirming this assumption, the authors have observed that the negative effects of fatigue during a long duration offshore sailing race (feeling of fatigue and decrease in memory) were lowered with protein feeding (4). Nutrition supplementation could therefore be beneficial during numerous sporting situations for physical performance (i.e., improve performance and delay fatigue) and/or for neural and cognitive performance (e.g., improve attention and vigilance for fast decision makings or short reaction times).
Numerous nutrients have been proposed to act at these different levels. Among the wide possibilities, Salvia species are promising candidates according to their various active constituents such as caffeic acid, salvianolic acid, sagecoumarin, and sagerinic acid (5). Among the possible effects (5) of Salvia have been demonstrated effective for acetylcholinesterase inhibition (6) that would likely originate from the monoterpenoid constituents (7). Concomitant improved mood and cognitive performance were observed (6, 7). Terpenoids (e.g., eucalyptol, camphor, α-, β-pinene, and others), particularly present in Salvia essential oil, have also been shown to include antioxidant effects by inhibiting reactive oxygen species production and increasing endogenous antioxidant compounds (8). Anti-inflammatory effects are also observed as a result of phenolic diterpenes present in some species of Salvia (e.g., Salvia officinalis) that reduce, for instance, cytokines or interleukins (5). Different trials have shown positive effects for attention, memory, speed of memory, alertness, or calmness after single intake in healthy individuals (9–11). Salvia also includes numerous polyphenols (e.g., rosmarinic acid, apigenin glucosides, luteolin glucosides, and others) components with a well-known antioxidant effect (5, 12). Polyphenols are of particular interest for endurance-type performance. Indeed, in a preceding study, we have observed the positive effects of acute apple and grape polyphenols intake to increase cycling time to exhaustion and delay the maximal perceived exertion (13).
Salvia plants include numerous species with considerable constituent variations and resultant potential effects (5). To date, most clinical studies have confirmed the acute and chronic effects of a given particular species. In contrast, a recent study demonstrated that a combination of Salvia was more efficient than a single constituent (14). This study observed larger improvements in memory and learning (Y-maze and Morris water maze tests) on in vivo rodents using a combination of S. officinalis (leaf extract mostly characterized for polyphenol content) and Salvia lavandulaefolia (encapsuled essential oil with predominant terpenoids) (14). This specific combination of Salvia has also been demonstrated efficient for working memory in healthy humans using various tasks (9). However, to date and to the best of our knowledge, no study has explored the effects of Salvia, in sports or physical activity settings. The aim of this study was to explore the acute effects of a supplementation of S. officinalis and S. lavandulaefolia on cognitive function in athletes performing a fatiguing cycling task. According to the positive effects of Salvia on cognitive function, we hypothesized that Salvia will improve cognition during exercise and that these positive effects were exacerbated in a fatigue state.
Materials and Methods
Study Design
This study was a cross-over, randomized, double-blind, and placebo-controlled trial. All volunteers came to the laboratory on four separate occasions (familiarization, initial testing, and two experimental sessions) with 7 days between each session. All experimental sessions were performed the same day of the week, at the same hour of the day (during the morning, 2 h after a standardized breakfast). During the total duration of the study, volunteers were instructed to refrain from intensive exercise.
The familiarization session aimed to (i) explain the experimental procedure, (ii) determine anthropometrics (age, height, body mass, and percentage of fat mass), and (iii) familiarize volunteers with the different tests at rest and while cycling at a light power output (ranging from 60 to 80 W with a minimum of 60 rpm, revolutions per minute). Body mass and percentage of fat mass were measured using a Tanita BC420 (Tanita, Tokyo, Japan). Cycling was performed on an air- and magnet-braked cycle ergometer Wattbike Pro (Wattbike Ltd., Nottingham, United Kingdom), with saddle and handlebar individually adjusted (settings were noted and used during the other sessions). Then, the initial testing session aimed to (i) determine the maximal aerobic power (MAP) using an incremental cycling test and (ii) re-familiarize volunteers with the different tests at rest and while cycling at a light power output (ranging from 60 to 80 W with a minimum of 60 rpm).
During the two other experimental sessions, volunteers were tested at different time points during a fatiguing cycling procedure performed 2 h after a supplementation of Salvia or placebo (here called SAGE or PLACEBO condition, respectively). SAGE or PLACEBO order was randomly presented by blocks of two using www.randomizer.org website. The fatiguing cycling procedure (Figure 1) consisted in different stages: (i) 5 min of warm-up at 50% of MAP, (ii) first test series (T1), (iii) 10 min at 80% of MAP, (iv) second test series (T2), (v) 10 min at 80% of MAP, (vi) third tests series (T3), (vii) 5-min recovery while pedaling at a very light power output (ranging from 60 to 80 W), and (viii) a final test (T4). Tests (T1–T4) were always performed while cycling for 5 min at 60% of MAP. While cycling, volunteers were requested to remain seated with a pedaling rate ranging between 80 and 90 rpm (except during the 5-min recovery period at very low power output). During the two periods at 80%, volunteers were encouraged at constant time points using standardized sentences. Moreover, care was taken to control power output during the whole procedure in order to produce the same mean power during the two experimental sessions. Three different tests were performed, namely, a Stroop task, a simple reaction time task, and a backward digit span memory task. These tests were selected because they are commonly used to investigate the cognitive function, for instance, in nutrients supplementation studies (15). Moreover, they are often linked to sport-related performance. Stroop and reaction time were selected to investigate attention and vigilance (16). The backward digit span memory task was selected for working memory due to its high cognitive demand (17). Because tests were performed while cycling, a 17-inch computer monitor faced the ergocycle and a keyboard was secured in the front part of the handlebar (Figure 2). Tests were randomly presented but with exactly the same order for a given volunteer. Moreover, heart rate was continuously registered using a Polar heart rate monitor (Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland), and rating of perceived exertion (RPE, 10-point scale) was done at the very beginning of the four test sessions.
Figure 1. Study flowchart. After a familiarization session (not shown), volunteers performed an initial test session followed by two randomized experimental sessions with SAGE or PLACEBO with 1 week between each session. The experimental sessions were performed on an ergocycle, and cognitive tests were conducted while cycling during the warm-up, during and at the end of the fatiguing exercise and after a short recovery.
Figure 2. Experimental setup. The experimental procedure was conducted on a Wattbike ergocycle. Cognitive tests were performed with a keyboard positioned on the handlebar and a computer screen facing the ergocycle. During cognitive tests, hands were first positioned on the handlebar. Volunteers then had to press the keyboard as quickly as possible with the index finger of the right hand. For the Stroop test, four colored keys were identified in the middle of the keyboard. For the reaction time, volunteers had to press the red key. For the backward digit span task, volunteers had to write the digits on the numerical keypad.
Participants
Twenty-six volunteers (18 men and 8 women) were included in this study. Their mean age ± SD, height, and body mass were 26.1 ± 6.4 years, 173.6 ± 8.4 cm, and 72.3 ± 13.3 kg, respectively. All were physically active with 5.4 ± 1.9 h training per week. Sports were fitness/strength (n = 12), running/cycling/swimming (n = 12), and team sports (n = 4). None reported lower limb injuries within the past 3 months. Prior to participation, they were fully informed about the type of product, expected effects on the cognitive function, purpose of the study, and experimental procedure. However, participants were blinded from our a priori hypothesis. All signed an informed consent form. This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was obtained from the University of Burgundy Ethics Committee, and the trial was registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (NCT04804657). Based on a previous study (18), the sample size was calculated a priori using G*Power (version 3.1.9.6, free software available at https://www.psychologie.hhu.de/arbeitsgruppen/allgemeine-psychologie-und-arbeitspsychologie/gpower.html) with a level of significance set at 0.05 and power of 0.9 to detect a large effect (partial-eta-squared: partial η2 > 0.14). A sample size of 14 individuals was indicated. The backward digit span memory task was considered as the primary criterion.
Incremental MAP Test
Participants started cycling at 80 W and 100 W for women and men, respectively. Every 2 min, power was increased using 20 W (women) and 25 W (men) increments. The test was interrupted when participants were unable to maintain the requested cycling rate (80–90 rpm). The last power value maintained at least 60 s was considered as the MAP. During this test, participants were instructed to remain seated all the time and to keep a constant pedaling rate. This power value was used as the reference during the two other experimental testing sessions. Heart rate was continuously measured using a Polar heart rate monitor. The maximal heart rate value was retained.
Cognitive Tests
An incongruent attentional Stroop task was used (19). It consisted of color words (yellow, blue, red, green) written in a different ink color (either yellow, blue, red, green). Words were presented on a computer screen and appeared centrally on a black background. Volunteers were requested to read the word and to press one of the four colored buttons on a computer keyboard depending on the color of the ink. Volunteers were instructed to press the keyboard as quickly and accurately as they can with the index finger of the right hand. The hand was positioned in a standardized position on the handlebar between trials. Words and ink colors were randomly presented. The test consisted in 32 trials with 1 s between trials. The Stroop accuracy (percentage of correct responses) and averaged Stroop reaction time were measured to monitor cognitive attentional performance. The test was conducted using OpenSesame software (ver 3.3.6 Lentiform Loewenfeld, downloadable for free from http://www.cogsci.nl/) (20). Preliminary tests revealed good day-by-day reliability with intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs), with values being 0.83 and 0.89 for Stroop accuracy and reaction time, respectively.
The simple reaction time test was performed using OpenSesame software. Volunteers were instructed to press a given key on a computer keyboard as quickly as they can with the index finger of the right hand following the visual stimulus. The right hand of the volunteers was positioned on the ergocycle handlebar (standardized position) between each trial. The background color was white, and the stimulus was a draw representing a smiling face. The test consisted in 20 trials with a random period ranging from 1 to 3 s between trials. The averaged reaction time was retained for analyses. Preliminary tests revealed good reliability (ICC = 0.87).
Working memory was evaluated by using the backward digit span memory task [e.g., (3)]. Volunteers had to read a sequence of digits (presented one by one) and then had to write it backward on a computer keyboard. The digit span was presented in an ascending order from three to seven digits. Volunteers had two different and successive trials for each span. A total of 10 spans corresponding to 50 digits were therefore presented. Two scores were measured: digit memory score and span memory score. The digit memory score was the percentage of correct digits (as a function of the total number of digits). The span memory score was the percentage of correct spans (with reference to the 10 spans). This test was conducted using a freely downloadable software (Digit-span-tester.exe ver 2.1.3 available from https://sourceforge.net/projects/digitspantester/). Preliminary tests revealed good reliability with ICC = 0.85 and 0.78 for the digit memory and span memory scores, respectively.
Perception of effort was measured at the very beginning of each test session using a 10-point visual RPE scale (21). Volunteers were asked to rate the conscious sensation of how hard the preceding cycling period was. One corresponded to “very light” exercise and 10 to “very hard” exercise. Simultaneously, the heart rate value was registered in beats per minute (bpm).
Supplementation
Depending on the randomization, either capsules of 600 mg of Salvia extracts or capsules of 600 mg of placebo (maltodextrin) were ingested 2 h before the two experimental sessions. Capsules were of similar appearance. Unfortunately, the taste was impossible to be masked. Some volunteers (n = 2) were able to identify the product ingested because of possible reflux. Both the participants and experimenters were blinded from the randomization. Participants were asked to have exactly the same food intake the preceding day. Also, volunteers were asked to have a standardized breakfast 2 h before the experimental sessions. As previously used (13), breakfast was composed of 125 mL orange juice, 80 g wholemeal bread, 20 g of butter, and 20 g of jelly.
The SAGE supplementation was made of CogniviaTM (Nexira, Rouen, France): a mix of S. officinalis and S. lavandulaefolia (9, 14). It contains 400 mg of aqueous extract from S. officinalis leaves characterized for its content in polyphenols (rosmarinic acid, apigenin glucosides, luteolin glucosides, and others) and 200 mg, which contains 50 μL of S. lavandulaefolia essential oil characterized for its content in terpenoids (as eucalyptol, camphor, α-, β-pinene, and others) encapsulated with acacia gum. The posology of both active substances has been selected in accordance with descriptions of the most effective dosages described in clinical acute studies introduced above (i.e., 50 μL of S. lavandulaefolia essential oil and extract of S. officinalis with a ratio equivalent to 2.25 g of dried leaves). Encapsulated powder of essential oil with acacia gum has been proposed to facilitate posology and observance compared to liquid form, and to protect terpenoids from evaporation and oxidation. Moreover, acacia gum has been shown not to compromise the nootropic activities of S. lavandulaefolia essential oil (14).
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were conducted using JASP (version 0.14, JASP Team 2020, University of Amsterdam, available free at https://jasp-stats.org/download/). The normality and sphericity of the data were tested and confirmed by the Shapiro–Wilk and Mauchly's tests. Gender differences for the characteristics of the volunteers and power output during the two experimental sessions were investigated using Student's t-test. Then, a two-way (supplementation × time) ANOVA with repeated measures was performed. The supplementation factor corresponded to PLACEBO vs. SAGE. The time factor corresponded to the four test sessions (T1 vs. T2 vs. T3 vs. T4). Post hoc tests with Bonferroni corrections were conducted if significant main effects or interactions were present. Partial-eta-squared (partial η2) was calculated from ANOVA results, with values of 0.01, 0.06, and above 0.14 representing small, medium, and large differences, respectively (22). Subsequently, qualitative descriptors of standardized effects were used for pairwise comparisons with Cohen's d <0.5, 0.5–1.2, and >1.2 representing small, medium, and large magnitudes of change, respectively (22). P < 0.05 was taken as the level of statistical significance for all comparisons. Absolute values are expressed as mean ± SD or mean difference with 95% confidence intervals (95%CI).
Results
Seventeen volunteers were considered for analyses (Table 1). Nine volunteers were lost due to the COVID-19 sanitary crisis (Figure 3). The mean power output during the two experimental procedures (power at 80% MAP) was similar between the two sessions (178.3 ± 50.6 W vs. 179.7 ± 48.8 W during SAGE and PLACEBO, respectively, P = 0.196). Results from the two-way ANOVA during the two experimental sessions are presented in Table 2. Briefly, a significant supplementation effect was observed for RPE, reaction time, and responses of the memory test. A significant time effect was obtained for heart rate, RPE, reaction time, and Stroop reaction time. Significant interactions were obtained for RPE and the accuracy of the Stroop test.
Post hoc analyses revealed that heart rate was significantly greater during T2 and T3 (during the fatiguing exercise) as compared to T1 and T4 (warm-up and recovery, respectively) (Figure 4, P < 0.001). No difference was observed between T2 and T3 (mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 4.9 ± 11.7 bpm (−2.9; 12.7), d = 0.419, small, P = 0.545). Heart rate remained significantly greater at T4 than at T1 (mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 11.5 ± 11.7 bpm (3.7; 19.4), d = 0.985, medium, P = 0.001). The post hoc analysis conducted on the interaction for RPE revealed various effects. With SAGE and PLACEBO, RPE was significantly lower at T1 as compared to T2, T3, and T4 (Figure 4, P < 0.001). For both SAGE and PLACEBO, RPE was significantly greater at T2 and T3 than at T4 (P < 0.001). However, RPE was significantly greater with PLACEBO as compared to SAGE at T3 [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 0.6 ± 0.7 (0.1; 1.2), d = 0.901, medium, P = 0.005] and T4 [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 0.7 ± 0.7 (0.1; 1.3), d = 0.705, medium, P = 0.002]. The main supplementation effect revealed lower RPE, all time points merged, with SAGE as compared to PLACEBO [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 0.3 ± 0.3 (0.1; 0.5), d = 0.868, medium, P = 0.002].
Figure 4. Heart rate (A), RPE [rate of perceived exertion, (B)], and reaction time (C) with SAGE and PLACEBO supplementations during the different tests. T1, T2, T3, and T4 were tests following warm-up, first and second fatiguing stages, and recovery, respectively. *** significant difference between tests whatever the supplementation (P < 0.001). $ significant difference between the supplementation for a given time point (RPE) or main effect (reaction time) (P < 0.05).
Reaction time was not altered during the exercise. However, reaction time (Figure 4) was significantly longer with PLACEBO as compared to SAGE [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 33.7 ± 55.4 ms (5.2; 62.2), d = 0.608, medium, P = 0.023]. During the Stroop task, reaction time (Figure 5) was significantly longer at T1 as compared to T4 [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): −86.4 ± 92.4 ms (−24.7;148.1), d = 0.935, medium, P = 0.02]. The post hoc analysis did not reveal any differences for Stroop accuracy. During the digit span memory test, the results revealed a significant supplementation effect (Figure 5). A significant greater span score was measured with SAGE as compared to PLACEBO [mean difference ± SD (95%CI): 6.0% ± 11.4 (0.2; 11.9), d = 0.53, medium, P = 0.044].
Figure 5. Stroop reaction time (A), Stroop accuracy (B), digit memory score (C), and span memory score (D) with SAGE and PLACEBO supplementations during the different tests. T1, T2, T3, and T4 were tests following warm-up, first and second fatiguing stages, and recovery, respectively. ** significant difference between tests whatever the supplementation (P < 0.01). $ main significant effect between the supplementation (P < 0.05).
Discussion
This study aimed to explore the acute effects of a supplementation of S. officinalis and S. lavandulaefolia on cognitive functions in athletes performing a fatiguing cycling task. The main findings are as follows: (1) The combination of Salvia improved the cognitive function (as witnessed by perceived exertion, working memory, and reaction time) throughout the experimental procedure and (2) the positive effects were maintained with fatigue. The present findings partly confirmed our initial hypothesis. We can conclude that an acute supplementation with a combination of S. officinalis and S. lavandulaefolia is beneficial on some cognitive function outcomes in the context of an endurance cycling exercise.
The results of this study confirmed previously published observations that revealed beneficial effects of Salvia on the cognitive function (9–11). These positive results were previously obtained with chronic supplementation in humans suffering from different diseases such as Alzheimer's disease or in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (23, 24). Acute Salvia supplementation was also beneficial in healthy individuals for mood, memory, or attention (7, 9–11, 25). More specifically, in healthy individuals, the two species of Salvia under investigation in the present study (S. officinalis and S. lavandulaefolia) have, separately, demonstrated improvements for immediate or delayed word recall, speed of memory, and Stroop test (7, 10, 26). These positive effects have been observed within the first 4 h post-ingestion (6, 26). The combination of these two species has been explored more recently (9). Similar positive effects, for instance in working memory, have been obtained in healthy individuals just 2 h after supplementation but also after a chronic administration (9, 14).
The nootropics properties of Salvia are partly described and begin to be explained (5). Inhibition of the acetylcholinesterase is often proposed as a mechanism for memory retention (27, 28). Interestingly, acetylcholinesterase inhibition is demonstrated for the two species of Salvia tested here whether it is an aqueous extract from leaves or essential oil. In addition, terpenes particularly identified in S. lavandulaefolia essential oil are able to attenuate oxidative stress by inhibiting reactive oxygen species production (8). Other effects originating from the phenolic constituents of Salvia include antioxidant, anti-inflammatory activities in neurons as well as increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (5). A preclinical trial also demonstrated that chronic ingestion of S. officinalis combined with S. lavandulaefolia increased the expression of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (14). This enzyme has large impact for improving working memory and cognitive processes since it acts in numerous neuronal functions, including neurotransmitter metabolism, neuronal signal transduction, or synaptic plasticity [see (29, 30)]. However, further studies are clearly needed to verify these hypotheses after a single intake and elucidate the Salvia-induced mechanisms.
To date and to the best of our knowledge, no study has explored the acute effects of Salvia in the context of sport, physical activity, or exercise-induced fatigue. In humans, fatigue has previously been considered in those complaining of chronic fatigue (23) with chronic supplementation of Salvia miltiorrhiza combined with Astragali. In healthy individuals, fatigue from exercise was explored with chronic supplementation in a single study performing a downhill running with S. miltiorrhiza combined with panax ginseng (31). A faster recovery of the joint range of motion and a lower increase in creatine kinase were obtained with supplementation after this eccentric-type exercise (31). Moreover, interleukin-6 increased with the placebo group, while it remained unchanged in the supplementation condition (31). Two other studies used animals performing a forced swimming test with chronic supplementation of Tanshinone IIA, a constituent from S. miltiorrhiza (32), or S. miltiorrhiza combined with Astragali (33). These two studies revealed an anti-fatigue effect of Salvia with reductions in serum glucose and lactate levels and by attenuating oxidative and inflammatory response (32, 33). Taken together, these studies with chronic supplementation confirmed that Salvia could be beneficial during exercise.
This study was the first to explore the acute effects of Salvia supplementation during exercise. Despite positive findings in some outcomes, our results are partly in contradiction with our initial hypothesis. Indeed, we hypothesized that the positive effects of the supplementation would be exacerbated with fatigue. Considering cognitive tests, no additional supplementation effect was obtained with fatigue. It signified that the positive effects of the supplementation were obtained in fresh conditions and were maintained during the exercise. In contrast, RPE was significantly altered during the intensive cycling exercise and during recovery (lower values with SAGE as compared to PLACEBO). At first sight, this outcome could appear trivial in sporting situations, but it is of paramount importance. Indeed, the central nervous system is well-known to play a very important role in the fatigue process (34, 35). For instance, numerous studies have demonstrated the negative impact of mental fatigue in sport-specific situations such as decision making in team sports or reaction time (35–38). After a demanding cognitive task, RPE was one of the most altered outcomes (36, 38, 39). The authors concluded that mental fatigue would limit exercise tolerance through higher perception of effort (39) and often consider RPE as a limiting factor for endurance performance instead of a simple marker of exercise intensity (40). Considering this psychobiological approach, the lower RPE observed with Salvia clearly suggested that supplementation could positively impact performance during fatiguing exercises. Such hypothesis should be further investigated since the present procedure was not conducted until complete exhaustion.
The lower RPE obtained with Salvia supplementation is not surprising. Salvia is a well-described supplement with numerous active constituents, including polyphenols with antioxidant effects (5, 12). Phenolic constituents are rosmarinic acid, caffeic acid, salvianolic acid, sagecoumarin, and sagerinic acid (5, 9). Polyphenols have been demonstrated to positively impact endurance performance in recreational and elite athletes (13, 41) concomitantly with a delay in maximal perceived exertion. Interestingly, one species of Salvia considered here (S. officinalis) has the highest antioxidant effects (42). Furthermore, the monoterpenes of the co-administered S. lavandulaefolia have been demonstrated to inhibit reactive oxygen species production (8). One should acknowledge that other studies failed to identify positive effects of polyphenols on endurance performance (43, 44). These conflicting findings could be related to the training status. Elite athletes (43) or well-training cyclists (44) were included, while physically active individuals were tested here.
Taken together, our results demonstrated the positive effects of Salvia during fatiguing exercises with lower RPE and maintained cognitive performance. Such finding has large impact for performance but also for injury prevention. Indeed, a recent study pointed out altered kinematics of landing that could increase injury risks in people conducting a perceptual-cognitive task combined with fatigue (45) such as those regularly occurring in numerous sports. Increasing awareness with, for example, cognitive training has been suggested to be efficient as prevention programs for a better neuromuscular control (46). Combining these preventive programs with supplementation could be hypothesized to emphasize prevention. Therefore, investigating the effects of supplementation in specific technical tasks would be of interest to confirm a potential positive effect for injury prevention.
Several limitations could be acknowledged. First, like in other studies, although the present study was double-blind, some volunteers were able to identify the product ingested. It is attributed to the herbal taste during some reflux at the end of the fatiguing exercise. Nevertheless, statistical analyses were similar when including or excluding these volunteers (n = 2). In addition, we decided to evaluate the cognitive function while cycling. This double task, often observed during sporting events, could be a limitation. Nevertheless, care was taken to familiarize individuals during two separate sessions. The fatiguing exercise consisted in two 10-min high-intensity cycling (with RPE > 8). This exercise was designed to be achievable by physically active individuals; it could be interesting to repeat such experimental design with a greater exercise difficulty, for instance, as often performed, until exhaustion (13). Finally, cognitive function was investigated using some specific tasks. It should be acknowledged that the Stroop task was not sensitive to fatigue with values very close to the maximal performance (100% of accuracy). Additional measurements for the Stroop test could be suggested to increase its sensitivity to fatigue or to the supplementation. Additional cognitive tasks could be applied in order to have a more exhaustive view of Salvia acute effects. Moreover, additional cognitive tasks, specific to some real-world sporting situations (for example, reaction time while playing tennis), should be tested in order to conclude that the observed effects could be translated to sporting events. In line with this perspective, the exercise under investigation should also be more specific (for example, while modeling a sporting event).
Conclusions
In conclusion, this study is consistent with previous experiments conducted in healthy individuals after acute Salvia supplementation. As compared to placebo, it revealed improved cognitive function (perceived exertion, reaction time, and working memory) after an acute Salvia intake (2 h after ingestion). Most effects were observed independently of the time point (before, during, or after a fatiguing cycling exercise). It is therefore suggested that Salvia effects are not negatively or positively affected by the exercise performed. Salvia demonstrated universal effects in fresh or fatigued conditions. It has a potential impact in the context of sport performance and more generally in the context of physical activity but requires more real-world exercises and cognitive tasks.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Burgundy Institutional Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
NB conceived and designed the experiment, analyzed the data, performed statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the paper. AN, NA, and CC performed the experiments. AN and CC performed data collection. NB and DG interpreted the data. All authors reviewed the manuscript and approved the final version of the paper.
Funding
This study was supported by the Centre d'Expertise de la Performance from the Université of Bourgogne. The product under investigation was provided by Nexira. The authors declare that this study received funding from Nexira to cover the costs of publication. The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.
Conflict of Interest
DG was employed by Nexira.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all the participants for their participation and commitment to this study.
Abbreviations
95%CI, 95% confidence intervals; ANOVA, analysis of variance; bpm, beats per minute; MAP, maximal aerobic power; partial η2, partial-eta-squared; RPE, rating of perceived exertion; rpm, revolutions per minute; SD, standard deviation; T1, T2, T3, and T4, tests during warm-up, during the fatiguing exercise, at the end of the fatiguing exercise, and after recovery, respectively.
References
1. Babault N, Deley GG, Le Ruyet P, Morgan FF, Allaert FA. Effects of soluble milk protein or casein supplementation on muscle fatigue following resistance training program: a randomized, double-blind, and placebo-controlled study. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. (2014) 11:36. doi: 10.1186/1550-2783-11-36
2. Guest NS, VanDusseldorp TA, Nelson MT, Grgic J, Schoenfeld BJ, Jenkins NDM, et al. International society of sports nutrition position stand: caffeine and exercise performance. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. (2021) 18:1. doi: 10.1186/s12970-020-00383-4
3. Davis JM, Alderson NL, Welsh RS. Serotonin and central nervous system fatigue: nutritional considerations. Am J Clin Nutr. (2000) 72:573S−8S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/72.2.573S
4. Portier H, Chatard JC, Filaire E, Jaunet-Devienne MF, Robert A, Guezennec CY. Effects of branched-chain amino acids supplementation on physiological and psychological performance during an offshore sailing race. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2008) 104:787–94. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0832-5
5. Lopresti AL. Salvia (Sage): a review of its potential cognitive-enhancing and protective effects. Drugs R D. (2017) 17:53–64. doi: 10.1007/s40268-016-0157-5
6. Kennedy DO, Pace S, Haskell C, Okello EJ, Milne A, Scholey AB. Effects of cholinesterase inhibiting sage (Salvia officinalis) on mood, anxiety and performance on a psychological stressor battery. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2006) 31:845–52. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300907
7. Kennedy DO, Dodd FL, Robertson BC, Okello EJ, Reay JL, Scholey AB, et al. Monoterpenoid extract of sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia) with cholinesterase inhibiting properties improves cognitive performance and mood in healthy adults. J Psychopharmacol. (2011) 25:1088–100. doi: 10.1177/0269881110385594
8. Porres-Martínez M, González-Burgos E, Carretero ME, Gómez-Serranillos MP. Major selected monoterpenes -pinene and 1,8-cineole found in Salvia lavandulifolia (Spanish sage) essential oil as regulators of cellular redox balance. Pharm Biol. (2015) 53:921–9. doi: 10.3109/13880209.2014.950672
9. Wightman EL, Jackson PA, Spittlehouse B, Heffernan T, Guillemet D, Kennedy DO. The acute and chronic cognitive effects of a sage extract: a randomized, placebo controlled study in healthy humans. Nutrients. (2021) 13:1–21. doi: 10.3390/nu13010218
10. Tildesley NTJ, Kennedy DO, Perry EK, Ballard CG, Wesnes KA, Scholey AB. Positive modulation of mood and cognitive performance following administration of acute doses of Salvia lavandulaefolia essential oil to healthy young volunteers. Physiol Behav. (2005) 83:699–709. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.09.010
11. Scholey AB, Tildesley NTJ, Ballard CG, Wesnes KA, Tasker A, Perry EK, et al. An extract of Salvia (sage) with anticholinesterase properties improves memory and attention in healthy older volunteers. Psychopharmacology. (2008) 198:127–39. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1101-3
12. Lu Y, Yeap Foo L. Antioxidant activities of polyphenols from sage (Salvia officinalis). Food Chem. (2001) 75:197–202. doi: 10.1016/S0308-8146(01)00198-4
13. Deley G, Guillemet D, Allaert F-AF-A, Babault N. An acute dose of specific grape and apple polyphenols improves endurance performance: a randomized, crossover, double-blind versus placebo controlled study. Nutrients. (2017) 9:917. doi: 10.3390/nu9080917
14. Dinel AL, Lucas C, Guillemet D, Layé S, Pallet V, Joffre C. Chronic supplementation with a mix of Salvia officinalis and Salvia lavandulaefolia improves Morris water maze learning in normal adult C57BL/6J mice. Nutrients. (2020) 12:1–18. doi: 10.3390/nu12061777
15. McLellan TM, Caldwell JA, Lieberman HR. A review of caffeine's effects on cognitive, physical and occupational performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:294–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.001
16. Periáñez JA, Lubrini G, García-Gutiérrez A, Ríos-Lago M. Construct validity of the Stroop color-word test: influence of speed of visual search, verbal fluency, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and conflict monitoring. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2021) 36:99–111. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa034
17. Kessels RPC, Van Den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AMA. The backward span of the corsi block-tapping task and its association with the WAIS-III digit span. Assessment. (2008) 15:426–34. doi: 10.1177/1073191108315611
18. Rogerson M, Gladwell VF, Gallagher DJ, Barton JL. Influences of green outdoors versus indoors environmental settings on psychological and social outcomes of controlled exercise. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2016) 13:363. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13040363
19. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. (1935) 18:643–62. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
20. Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2012) 44:314–24. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
21. Borg G. Physiological studies of effort and exertion: some historical, theorical and empirical aspects. In: Borg G, Ottoson D, editors. The Perception of Exertion in Physical Work. London: Macmillan (1986). p. 3–12.
22. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. (2013). Available online at: https://journals.google.fr/journals?hl=fr&lr=&id=rEe0BQAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PP1&dq=Statistical+power+analysis+for+the+behavioral+sciences+&ots=suXSHtOUud&sig=mv1c3vJTBpzRlrhdoR6yqVPJ5PU (accessed September 11, 2015).
23. Joung JY, Lee JS, Cho JH, Lee DS, Ahn YC, Son CG. The efficacy and safety of myelophil, an ethanol extract mixture of Astragali Radix and Salviae radix, for chronic fatigue syndrome: a randomized clinical trial. Front Pharmacol. (2019) 10:991. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00991
24. Miroddi M, Navarra M, Quattropani MC, Calapai F, Gangemi S, Calapai G. Systematic review of clinical trials assessing pharmacological properties of salvia species on memory, cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2014) 20:485–95. doi: 10.1111/cns.12270
25. Moss M, Smith E, Milner M, McCready J. Acute ingestion of rosemary water: evidence of cognitive and cerebrovascular effects in healthy adults. J Psychopharmacol. (2018) 32:1319–29. doi: 10.1177/0269881118798339
26. Tildesley NTJ, Kennedy DO, Perry EK, Ballard CG, Savelev S, Wesnes KA, et al. Salvia lavandulaefolia (Spanish Sage) enhances memory in healthy young volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2003) 75:669–74. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(03)00122-9
27. Smach MA, Hafsa J, Charfeddine B, Dridi H, Limem K. Effects of sage extract on memory performance in mice and acetylcholinesterase activity. Ann Pharm Fr. (2015) 73:281–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pharma.2015.03.005
28. Sallam A, Mira A, Ashour A, Shimizu K. Acetylcholine esterase inhibitors and melanin synthesis inhibitors from Salvia officinalis. Phytomedicine. (2016) 23:1005–11. doi: 10.1016/j.phymed.2016.06.014
29. Lisman J, Schulman H, Cline H. The molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:175–90. doi: 10.1038/nrn753
30. Zalcman G, Federman N, Romano A. CaMKII isoforms in learning and memory: localization and function. Front Mol Neurosci. (2018) 11:445. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00445
31. Lin HF, Tung K, Chou CC, Lin CC, Lin JG, Tanaka H. Panax ginseng and salvia miltiorrhiza supplementation abolishes eccentric exercise-induced vascular stiffening: a double-blind randomized control trial. BMC Complement Altern Med. (2016) 16:168. doi: 10.1186/s12906-016-1139-4
32. Lin CY, Jhang YS, Lai SC, Chen EL, Lin IH, Chang TW, et al. Antifatigue properties of tanshinone IIA in mice subjected to the forced swimming test. Pharm Biol. (2017) 55:2264–9. doi: 10.1080/13880209.2017.1401648
33. Lee JS, Kim HG, Han JM, Kim YA, Son CG. Anti-fatigue effect of Myelophil in a chronic forced exercise mouse model. Eur J Pharmacol. (2015) 764:100–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2015.06.055
34. Enoka RM, Baudry S, Rudroff T, Farina D, Klass M, Duchateau J. Unraveling the neurophysiology of muscle fatigue. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. (2011) 21:208–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2010.10.006
35. Smith MR, Thompson C, Marcora SM, Skorski S, Meyer T, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue and soccer: current knowledge and future directions. Sport Med. (2018) 48:1525–32. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0908-2
36. Habay J, Van Cutsem J, Verschueren J, De Bock S, Proost M, De Wachter J, et al. Mental fatigue and sport-specific psychomotor performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. (2021) 51:1527–48. doi: 10.1007/s40279-021-01429-6
37. Gantois P, Caputo Ferreira ME, Lima-Junior D de, Nakamura FY, Batista GR, Fonseca FS, et al. Effects of mental fatigue on passing decision-making performance in professional soccer athletes. Eur J Sport Sci. (2020) 20:534–43. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1656781
38. Slimani M, Znazen H, Bragazzi N, Zguira M, Tod D. The effect of mental fatigue on cognitive and aerobic performance in adolescent active endurance athletes: insights from a randomized counterbalanced, cross-over trial. J Clin Med. (2018) 7:510. doi: 10.3390/jcm7120510
39. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol. (2009) 106:857–64. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008
40. Pageaux B, Lepers R. Fatigue induced by physical and mental exertion increases perception of effort and impairs subsequent endurance performance. Front Physiol. (2016) 7:587. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00587
41. Oh JK, Shin YO, Yoon JH, Kim SH, Shin HC, Hwang HJ. Effect of supplementation with Ecklonia cava polyphenol on endurance performance of college students. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. (2010) 20:72–9. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.20.1.72
42. Šulniute V, RagaŽinskiene O, Venskutonis PR. Comprehensive evaluation of antioxidant potential of 10 salvia species using high pressure methods for the isolation of lipophilic and hydrophilic plant fractions. Plant Foods Hum Nutr. (2016) 71:64–71. doi: 10.1007/s11130-015-0526-1
43. Labonté K, Couillard C, Motard-Bélanger A, Paradis M-E, Couture P, Lamarche B. Acute effects of polyphenols from cranberries and grape seeds on endothelial function and performance in elite athletes. Sports. (2013) 1:55–68. doi: 10.3390/sports1030055
44. Decroix L, Tonoli C, Soares DD, Descat A, Drittij-Reijnders MJ, Weseler AR, et al. Acute cocoa Flavanols intake has minimal effects on exercise-induced oxidative stress and nitric oxide production in healthy cyclists: a randomized controlled trial. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. (2017) 14:28. doi: 10.1186/s12970-017-0186-7
45. Mejane J, Faubert J, Romeas T, Labbe DR. The combined impact of a perceptual–cognitive task and neuromuscular fatigue on knee biomechanics during landing. Knee. (2019) 26:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.knee.2018.10.017
46. Alentorn-Geli E, Myer GD, Silvers HJ, Samitier G, Romero D, Lázaro-Haro C, et al. Prevention of non-contact anterior cruciate ligament injuries in soccer players. Part 2: a review of prevention programs aimed to modify risk factors and to reduce injury rates. Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc. (2009) 17:859–79. doi: 10.1007/s00167-009-0823-z
Keywords: memory, attention, fatigue, perceived exertion, supplementation, reaction time
Citation: Babault N, Noureddine A, Amiez N, Guillemet D and Cometti C (2021) Acute Effects of Salvia Supplementation on Cognitive Function in Athletes During a Fatiguing Cycling Exercise: A Randomized Cross-Over, Placebo-Controlled, and Double-Blind Study. Front. Nutr. 8:771518. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.771518
Received: 06 September 2021; Accepted: 25 October 2021;
Published: 30 November 2021.
Edited by:
David Michael Bellar, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, United StatesReviewed by:
Jeroen Van Cutsem, Royal Military Academy, BelgiumSanjoy Deb, University of Westminster, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2021 Babault, Noureddine, Amiez, Guillemet and Cometti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Nicolas Babault, nicolas.babault@u-bourgogne.fr
†ORCID: Nicolas Babault orcid.org/0000-0001-6563-503X
Nicolas Amiez orcid.org/0000-0001-5175-5591
Damien Guillemet orcid.org/0000-0002-6638-0531
Carole Cometti orcid.org/0000-0002-6826-4008