Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 10 June 2021
Sec. Eating Behavior

Psychosocial Determinants of Vegetable Intake Among Nepalese Young Adults: An Exploratory Survey

\nSujita Pandey
Sujita Pandey1*Mausam BudhathokiMausam Budhathoki2Dipendra Kumar YadavDipendra Kumar Yadav3
  • 1Department of Food Science, University of Copenhagen, Frederiksberg, Denmark
  • 2Department of Management, Xavier International College, Kathmandu, Nepal
  • 3Faculty of Health Sciences, School of Health and Allied Sciences, Pokhara University, Pokhara, Nepal

Background: Adequate intake of vegetables facilitates a healthy lifestyle. However, the majority of Nepalese young adults consume inadequate amount of vegetables per day.

Objectives: We explored psychosocial determinants of daily intake of two or more servings of vegetables among Nepalese young adults using attitude, social influence, and self-efficacy (ASE) as a theoretical framework, extended with measures of habit and self-identity as additional constructs.

Methods/Participants: A cross-sectional study through a web-based questionnaire survey was conducted among 461 Nepalese young adults aged 18–35 years old. Participants were recruited through convenience (snowball) sampling. A factor-based partial least square structural equation modelling was used for analysis.

Results: The findings indicated that attitudes (β = 0.09, p = 0.029), social influence (β = 0.17, p < 0.001), habit (β = 0.24, p < 0.001) and self-identity (β = 0.30, p < 0.001) were significant factors influencing intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. Further, self-efficacy (β = 0.10, p = 0.011), habit (β = 0.08, p = 0.034), diet (β = −0.10, p = 0.014), and place of residence (β = 0.11, p = 0.007) significantly influenced behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. However, self-efficacy (β = 0.07, p = 0.062) did not significantly influence intention and self-identity (β = 0.06, p = 0.083), age (β = −0.02, p = 0.328), gender (β = 0.02, p = 0.350), and body mass index (β = −0.04, p = 0.209) did not significantly influence behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day.

Conclusion: The study shows that attitudes, social influence, habit, and self-identity were significant factors influencing intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. Further, self-efficacy and habit significantly influenced behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. However, self-efficacy did not significantly influence intention and self-identity did not significantly influence behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day.

Introduction

Adequate intake of fruits and vegetables facilitates a healthy lifestyle. Several previous studies have linked consumption of inadequate fruits and vegetables to the growing burden of chronic diseases globally, such as gastrointestinal cancer (1), coronary heart disease (2), obesity (3), diabetes mellitus (4, 5), and stroke (6). The World Health Organisation estimates that ~1.7 million deaths globally are attributable to inadequate consumption of fruits and vegetables. Therefore, the report of a joint World Health Organisation and Food and Agriculture Organisation consultation (7) recommends consuming a minimum of 400 g of fruits and vegetables per day (excluding potatoes and other starchy tubers) to prevent these deaths due to chronic diseases and to mitigate micronutrient deficiencies. Further, consuming a variety of fruits and vegetables helps to ensure an adequate intake of many essential nutrients including, vitamins and minerals, dietary fibres and antioxidants (7). The recommended serving size of fruits and vegetables in Nepal is indicated descriptively, where the Ministry of Health and Population of Nepal recommends eating plenty of fruits and vegetables, especially green leafy vegetables.

In Nepal, the change in food culture in recent decades due to globalisation and urbanisation has resulted in unhealthy food habits among adolescents and young adults (8, 9). A nationwide STEPS survey (2013) results indicated that 99% of the 972 respondents aged 15–29 years old consumed insufficient quantities of fruits and vegetables (9). Recent survey data found that 76% of 1,600 young adults aged 18–29 years residing in the capital city Kathmandu consumed insufficient quantities of fruits and vegetables (10). On average, Nepalese males between the ages of 25 and 34 years old consume 1.45 ± 0.64 servings of vegetable, while females consume an average of 1.47 ± 0.62 servings of vegetables (11). It is important for young adults to consume adequate amount of vegetables because individual dietary patterns developed in young adults often persist into later life, influencing their own health as well as that of their partners and children (12, 13).

To our knowledge, no research has been conducted in developing countries like Nepal to determine psychosocial determinants of vegetable intake among young adults. Existing data have investigated only the sociodemographic and diet-related factors. Thus, the underlying psychosocial determinants remain unclear, and more research is needed to determine the key psychosocial determinants of behaviour regarding vegetable intake and their relationships to daily intake. The main aim of this study is to determine the psychosocial determinants influencing daily intake of two or more servings of vegetables among Nepalese young adults using attitude, social influence and self-efficacy (ASE) as a theoretical framework, extended with measures of habit, and self-identity as additional constructs. The findings from this study may support the development of effective intervention programmes for increasing vegetable intake among young adults in Nepal.

Theoretical Framework

The ASE framework (14, 15) has been used extensively to study dietary behaviours (16, 17), including vegetable intake (1719). According to the ASE model, social cognitive factors, namely attitudes, social influence and self-efficacy expectations are major determinants of health-related behaviour (20, 21). Further, behavioural intention acts as a mediator between the influence of attitudes, social norms and self-efficacy and health-related behaviour (22). The study (23) found that besides intention, Bandura's self-efficacy (24) was the most consistent factor explaining vegetable intake behaviour. Moreover, dietary behaviours are repeatedly and routinely performed and thus may eventually become habitual (25). Lally and Gardner (26) argue that habitual behaviours are automatic responses to environmental cues rather than a conscious decision process as proposed by the ASE framework. Studies have indicated that when habit strength increases, deliberate intentions of behaviour are less predictive (27, 28). The study by Brug et al. (17) found that habit is one of the major psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among adults and could be included as an independent determinant in health behaviour models. Further, previous studies have found that when habit is added as an additional construct to the health behaviour model, the predictive power of the model increased (25, 2931). For instance, the study (31) which predicted fruit consumption showed that the addition of habit in the health behaviour model increased the explained variance up to 9%.

In addition to habit, self-identity—a salient aspect where a person recognises their self-perception and creates ideal decisions to act—is a motivational construct that makes a person more willing to perform health behaviour through influencing the behavioural intention (32, 33). Previous studies have shown that when a person recognises themselves as a healthy eater, the person will have positive effects on intention to eat fruits and vegetables (3436). A meta-analysis by Rise et al. (37) found that self-identity as an additional construct in health behaviour models increased about 6% of variance in behavioural intention.

Based on the findings, following hypotheses were expected (see Figure 1):

Hypotheses 1–5: Attitudes, social influence, self-efficacy, and intention affect behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day.

Hypotheses 6–7: Habit to eat vegetables significantly affects intention as well as behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day.

Hypotheses 8–9: Self-identity significantly affects intentions as well as behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proposed theoretical framework for determining daily intake of two or more servings of vegetables.

Materials and Methods

Questionnaire and Measurement Scale

The study questionnaire consists of standardised and validated questions adopted from previous literatures. Supplementary Table 1 briefly presents the questions measuring the corresponding constructs of the proposed theoretical framework and their source of adoption. A seven-point Likert scale ranging from (−3): Strongly disagree to (+3): Strongly agree was used to evaluate the items measuring ASE constructs as well as the additional constructs of habit and self-identity except for behaviour. Behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day was measured with the following two items: (1) the frequency of vegetable items (cauliflower, lentils, spinach, tomato, etc.) consumed during the last 24 h ranging from 1 = “none” to 4 = “four or more servings” and (2) the number of servings of vegetables consumed last week ranging from 1 = “ <3 per week” to 6 = “more than 3 per day.” To determine the serving size a visual aid was provided where potatoes, sweet potatoes and other starchy roots were not classified as vegetables. Portion size for cooked vegetables and salads could be answered either in serving cups or in grams (1/2 serving cup cooked = 1 serving cup raw = 75 g, according to the Australian Dietary Guidelines). Sociodemographic characteristics which were considered are age, gender, height and weight, dietary pattern and place of residence.

Participants and Procedure

According to Kock and Hadaya (38), the minimum required sample size for the partial least square structural equation method to estimate a 0.170 path coefficient at significance level 0.05 with a power of 0.980, is 474 based on the inverse square root method and 454 based on the gamma-exponential method. Thus, a snowball sampling technique was utilised to recruit 475 participants via social media (Facebook) and the researchers' own networks (a link was sent by email) on 24th February 2021 to 8th March 2021. The questionnaire was developed in and administered through the Survey-Xact platform. The inclusion criteria for participation in the survey were the age range 18–35 years old, English language skills and willingness to participate. Participants younger than 18 years of age and older than 35 years of age, without English language skills and unwillingness to participate were excluded from the study. Participants were made aware about the time needed (~5–8 min) before completing the survey.

Ethical Consideration

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures involving study participants were approved by the Institutional Ethical Review Committee of Nepal Health Research Council (Reg nr. 67/2021 P). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before participating in the survey.

Data Analysis

The survey data stored on the Survey-Xact platform was transferred to a file compatible with IBM SPSS Statistics version 26.0 to perform statistical analysis. Initially, a descriptive analysis was conducted for the sociodemographic characteristics of the participants. Data were presented with percentage and number for categorical variables and median and interquartile range for continuous variable. The two items measuring behaviour were transferred into a dichotomous variable with intake of vegetables fewer than two servings per day coded as 0 and two or more servings of vegetables intake per day coded as 1. Further, the two reversed statements measuring attitudes were recorded in the same direction. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (kg)/height (m2) and categorised using Asian specific BMI cut-off values underweight was defined as <18.5 kg/m2, normal weight as 18.5–22.99 kg/m2, overweight as 23–27.49 kg/m2, and obese ≥27.5 kg/m2 (39).

Secondly, factor loadings, validity (both convergent and discriminant), reliability and multicollinearity of the constructs and correlation coefficient between the constructs were determined in conjunction with SEM analysis in WrapPLS version 7.0 software (40). A factor-based partial least square structural equation modelling (PLSF-SEM) algorithm routine was selected for testing hypotheses H1–H9. The outer model analysis (PLSF type CFM3, which employs both loadings and reliabilities from Dijkstra's consistent PLS technique to estimate measurement error and true composite weights), the inner model analysis (the default Wrap 3 algorithm) and the Stable 3 resampling method were utilised (41, 42). The underlying assumption of the model was based on the original ASE framework (i.e., a direct path from attitude, social influence and self-efficacy towards behavioural intention). Analysis of the original ASE framework and the proposed theoretical framework was conducted to test whether the proposed theoretical model fits the data more accurately. The model fit was reported by the four goodness-of-fit measures: average path coefficient (APC), average r-squared (ARS) values, average variance inflation factors (AVIF), and the goodness-of-fit (GoF) global index.

Results

Sociodemographic Characteristics

Table 1 presents the sociodemographic characteristics of the participants. After excluding 28 incomplete questionnaires, the final sample consisted of 461 young adults between the age range of 18–35 years old, 56.0% of which were females. The result also indicated that the majority of the participants followed a non-vegetarian dietary pattern (76.4% of the participants), were from the capital city Kathmandu (42.1%) and had a normal BMI (kg/m2) (54.9%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants, N = 461a.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis, Validity, Reliability, and Multicollinearity Tests

The results from Supplementary Table 2 show that all items measuring the ASE constructs and additional constructs of habit and self-identity loaded highly on the pre-determined factors—normalised loadings for the items of each construct were above 0.70 (40, 43). Further, the p-values associated with the loadings of items in their corresponding constructs were <0.05, indicating acceptable convergent validity (42). The value of Cronbach's alpha and composite reliability, which assesses measurement reliability, were all above the minimum requirement of 0.70 (44). The full collinearity variance inflation factors (VIFs) of all constructs were <3.3, which suggests that no multicollinearity and no common method bias exists in the model (40). The value of average variance extracted (AVE) of each construct was above the minimum threshold of 0.50, as shown in Supplementary Table 2, and the square roots of the AVE were greater than the correlation coefficient among the constructs, as shown in Supplementary Table 3, which confirmed the discriminant validity of the constructs (45, 46).

Goodness-of-Fit Measures

The various path coefficients of the original ASE framework (Model 0) and the proposed theoretical framework (Model 1) are summarised in Figure 2. The results indicate that the Model 1 has a higher average r-squared (ARS) value than Model 0 (0.23 vs. 0.34, respectively), indicating that it accounted well for the variations in the behavioural intention and behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. Thus, the proposed theoretical framework (Model 1) has the best fit with the data despite Model 0 having a higher average path coefficient (APC) than Model 1 (0.23 vs. 0.18, respectively) (44). Further, Model 1 also has a low average block variance inflation factor (AVIF), indicating no existence of multi-collinearity (44). The value of the Tenenhaus goodness-of-fit (GoF) global index of Model 0 and Model 1 are above 0.36, indicating larger effect sizes (47), however Model 1 shows a higher GoF index than Model 0 (0.47 vs. 0.40, respectively), indicating that it has the best model-data fit (48).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Goodness-of-fit measures.

Structural Model Assessment

The results from the PLSF-SEM analysis (Table 2) shows that attitudes (β = 0.09, p < 0.05, Cohen's f 2 = 0.02) and social influence (β = 0.17, p < 0.001, Cohen's f 2 = 0.05) with small to medium effect size significantly influenced intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day, thus hypotheses H1 and H2 are supported. In addition, self-efficacy with small effect size (Cohen's f 2 = 0.02) significantly influenced behaviour (β = 0.10, p < 0.05) to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day, however no significant influence was noted between self-efficacy and intention (β = 0.07, p = 0.06). Thus, rejecting hypothesis H3, while supporting hypothesis H4. The impact of the intention on behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day was found to be positive and significant (β = 0.52, p < 0.001) with a moderate to large effect size (Cohen's f 2 = 0.28), which supports hypothesis H5. In addition to the ASE constructs, habit with moderate to large effect size (β = 0.24, p < 0.001, Cohen's f 2 = 0.11) significantly influenced intention and a small to moderate effect size was noted between habit and behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day (β = 0.08, p < 0.05, Cohen's f 2 = 0.03). This supports hypotheses H6 and H7. Finally, the results also indicate that self-identity (β = 0.30, p < 0.001, Cohen's f 2 = 0.14) with moderate to large effect size significantly influenced intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day, however no significant effect (β = 0.06, p = 0.083) was noted between self-identity and behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. This supports hypothesis H8, while rejecting hypothesis H9. In Model 1, the Stone-Geisser Q2 coefficient was 0.32 for intention was 0.30 for behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables (Figure 2), which means that the model has acceptable predictive validity (40, 50, 51), further supporting the hypotheses of this study.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Structural relationship between the constructs and hypotheses' status.

Age, gender, diet, BMI, and place of residence of the participants were controlled in this study, and it was found that the diet (non-vegetarian = 1) (β = −0.10, p = 0.014) and place of residence (Kathmandu = 1) (β = 0.11, p = 0.007) of the participants had a significant impact on their daily intake of two or more servings of vegetables. In contrast, age (under 25 years = 1) (β = −0.02, p = 0.328), gender (β = 0.02, p = 0.350), and BMI (normal BMI = 1) (β = −0.04, p = 0.209) did not have any impact on their behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. Further, the predictive power of vegetable consumption intention with controlled variables was r2 = 0.32 and that of vegetable consumption behaviour was r2 = 0.33.

Discussion and Implication

This exploratory study attempts to determine psychosocial determinants influencing behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day among young adults in one of the developing countries, primarily based on the ASE framework along with habit and self-identity as additional constructs. The results of this study strongly confirm the adequacy of the ASE framework to determine recommended vegetable intake levels among Nepalese young adults. The addition of habit and self-identity in the ASE framework increased the explained variance, especially of behavioural intention. The results from the present study indicate that among the ASE constructs, social influence has a strong effect on intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. These findings are inconsistent with a previous study (35). However, in Nepal, most of the young adults and their parents continue to live in the same household for multiple generations (52). Furthermore, the meals in the household are often prepared and grocery shopping is done by the mothers/guardian, therefore meal patterns and dietary intake are directly influenced by the parental knowledge of nutrition, attitude and cooking techniques (53). Furthermore, peers of similar age/partners can highly influence the behaviour positively or negatively and can provide social support (19, 54). This might explain higher social influence for intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day among the participants. Therefore, parental involvement intervention (55) and peer-based intervention (36) might be effective strategies to encourage vegetable consumption among young adults in Nepal.

Attitude was a significant predictor for intention to eat vegetables among young adults. The findings are consistent with previous studies of fruit and vegetable consumption among young adults (19, 56). Singh et al. (54) found that educational interventions significantly developed positive attitudes towards healthy eating behaviour among adolescents in Nepal. Such interventions might also consider providing nutritional advice on the health implications of consuming adequate vegetables for promoting and stimulating a positive attitude towards the recommended daily intake of vegetables among young adults (57).

The results from this study indicate that self-efficacy is a positive and significant predictor of behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day among young adults, however failed to significantly influence intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. The findings are inconsistent with findings from previous studies (25, 58). The study (59) argued that self-efficacy appeared to be the most consistent determinant in nutrition-related interventions to successfully adopt new behaviour in Nepal. Similarly, Bhandari and Kim (60) found that self-efficacy is a strong significant predictor for health promoting behaviours among Nepalese migrant workers aged 21–55 years. Thus, psychological programmes focusing on self-efficacy enhancement through modelling, persuasion and direct mastery experience (61) is recommended. Further, such interventions might also consider including planning (action and coping) skills for achieving sustained behaviour change (62, 63), and thus to ultimately form healthy dietary habits among young adults (25).

The result from this study indicate that habit strength has a positive influence on intention and behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. The findings are in line with previous studies (25, 30, 31). However, the study (25) found that habit strength had a stronger effect on behaviour than on intention, which is not aligned with the findings of this study. According to Verplanken and Orbell (64), intentions are expected to influence consumption of fruit and vegetables when habits are not formed as a result of occasional and irregular behaviour. Furthermore, as argued by Ouellette and Wood (65), repeated and consistent behaviour to eat fruit and vegetables is determined and gradually initiated by extrinsic stimuli. These extrinsic stimuli can be, for instance, lifestyle (66), seasonal influence (19), dietary diversity (67), parental consumption and their habits (29), availability of vegetables (68, 69), household income (70, 71), all of which may influence consumption of adequate vegetables among young adults in Nepal. In the context of Nepal, a previous study has found that low dietary diversity, low income and non-availability in households were strongly associated with consumption of insufficient fruit and vegetables (67). Further, seasonal availability strongly determined the intake of vegetables in Nepal (72, 73). Therefore, interventions to increase vegetable consumption among young adults in Nepal might include cue disruption, environmental engineering and vigilant monitoring (74), in order to prompt habitual behaviour. Previous studies have also proposed implementing intentions that can help overcome the so-called intention-behaviour gap (22, 75) to change unwanted food habits, however Rothman et al. (76) argue that such changes still require motivational efforts.

The findings from this study suggest that self-identity has a strong effect on intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day, however failed to significantly influence behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. The findings align with the findings from a previous study (34). Previous studies have indicated that participants with higher self-identity are more responsive to information related to nutrition education (77, 78). The study (79) argue that health consistent behaviour can be achieved by creating healthy eating behaviour goals, and these goals for behavioural change can be fulfilled by altering them to self-as-doer identity statements. For instance, a goal can be “eat more vegetables” whereas the self-as-doer identity statement would be “vegetable eater.” Therefore, setting up goals and altering them to self-as-doer statements can increase intrinsic motivation among Nepalese young adults to eat adequate amount of vegetables (36, 80).

Strength and Limitations

The study is the first of its kind to determine the psychosocial determinants influencing daily intake of vegetables among Nepalese young adults using the ASE as a theoretical framework with habit and self-identity as additional constructs.

The study has some limitations. First, limited sample size with cross-sectional data of young adults limits the causal inference of the findings, and only participants with English language skills and access to the internet completed the survey, thus potentially limiting the generalisation to young adults in Nepal. Further, the convenience (snowball) sampling technique employed might have resulted in the selection bias. The responses in this study were self-reported, which may have resulted in biases (self-reporting, social desirability, and recall) (81). For instance, a literature review (82) showed that bias from self-reported height and weight (underestimated and overestimated) was greater in overweight and obese participants than those of normal weight, however, such bias seemed to be lower among Asian population than those from other continents. Further, we employed a brief measure of habit strength and self-identity, and thus may have limited more precise estimations of the effects of habit strength (64) and self-identity (34). Finally, this study considered self-identity and habit as an additional construct, however inclusion of other important factors such as knowledge, perceived barriers and food consumption patterns (19, 54) could have strengthened the predictability of the framework.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the study has empirically applied the ASE framework with two additional constructs: habit and self-identity. The additional constructs have increased the predictive ability of the proposed theoretical framework to determine the psychosocial determinants influencing daily intake of two or more servings of vegetables among Nepalese young adults. Overall, attitudes, social influence, habit and self-identity were significant factors influencing intention to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. Further, self-efficacy and habit significantly influenced behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. However, self-efficacy did not significantly influence intention and self-identity did not significantly influence behaviour to eat two or more servings of vegetables per day. In future, longitudinal studies with larger and statistically representative young adults are recommended. Future studies might consider investigating the effectiveness of such health behaviour models in achieving desired healthy eating behaviour among young adults in developing countries like Nepal.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Ethical Review Committee of Nepal Health Research Council. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SP and MB did the analysis of the data, data curation, methodology, and writing the original draft. DY acted as project administrator and did supervision and validation. All of the authors participated in conceptualisation, reviewing and editing the manuscript, and gave final approval to the submitted version of manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Mijash Humagain, Illu Malla Thakuri, and Damber Bahadur Gurung for their assistance with formatting the final version of the questionnaire and data collection. We are thankful to Anders Nielsen for proofreading the final version of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2021.688059/full#supplementary-material

References

1. van den Brandt PA, Goldbohm RA. Nutrition in the prevention of gastrointestinal cancer. Best Pract Res Clin Gastroenterol. (2006) 20:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.bpg.2006.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Hercberg S, Dallongeville J. Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr. (2006) 136:2588–93. doi: 10.1093/jn/136.10.2588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Godin G, Amireault S, Bélanger-Gravel A, Vohl M-C, Pérusse L, Guillaumie L. Prediction of daily fruit and vegetable consumption among overweight and obese individuals. Appetite. (2010) 54:480–4. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ford ES, Mokdad AH. Fruit and vegetable consumption and diabetes mellitus incidence among U.S. Adults. Prev Med. (2001) 32:33–9. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Carter P, Gray LJ, Troughton J, Khunti K, Davies MJ. Fruit and vegetable intake and incidence of type 2 diabetes mellitus: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. (2010) 341:c4229. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c4229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. He FJ, Nowson CA, MacGregor GA. Fruit and vegetable consumption and stroke: meta-analysis of cohort studies. Lancet. (2006) 367:320–6. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68069-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. World Health Organization. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. (2003) (accessed January 12, 2021).

Google Scholar

8. Aryal KK, Mehata RK, Chalise B, Mehata S, Sapkota F, Dhimal M, et al. Adolescent Nutrition Survey in Nepal, 2014. Kathmandu: Nepal Health Research Council (2016). Available online at: http://nhrc.gov.np/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/latest-final-nutrition-book.pdf (accessed January 13, 2021).

Google Scholar

9. Aryal KK, Mehata S, Neupane S, Vaidya A, Dhimal M, Dhakal P, et al. The burden and determinants of non communicable diseases risk factors in nepal: findings from a nationwide STEPS survey. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0134834. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Anil OM, Yadav RS, Shrestha N, Koirala S, Shrestha S, Nikhil OM, et al. Prevalence of cardiovascular risk factors in apparently healthy urban adult population of Kathmandu. J Nepal Health Res Counc. (2019) 16:438–45. doi: 10.33314/jnhrc.v16i41.1705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Vaidya A, Oli N, Aryal U, Karki D, Krettek A. Disparties in fruit and vegetable intake by socio-demographic characteristics in Peri-Urban Nepalese adults: findings from the heart-health associated research and dissemination in the community (HARDIC) study, Bhaktapur, Nepal. J Kathmandu Med Coll. (2014) 2:3–11. doi: 10.3126/jkmc.v2i1.10512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Poobalan AS, Aucott LS, Clarke A, Smith WCS. Diet behaviour among young people in transition to adulthood (18-25 year olds): a mixed method study. Health Psychol Behav Med. (2014) 2:909–28. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2014.931232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Parcel GS, Muraskin LD, Endert CM. Community education. J Adolesc Health Care. (1988) 9:S41–S5. doi: 10.1016/0197-0070(88)90008-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. de Vries H, Dijkstra M, Kuhlman P. Self-efficacy: the third factor besides attitude and subjective norm as a predictor of behavioural intentions. Health Educ Res. (1988) 3:273–82. doi: 10.1093/her/3.3.273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lechner L. Social Psychological Determinants of Health Risk Behaviors Related to Cancer and CVD: Applications and Elaborations of the ASE Model (dissertation/doctoral thesis). Maastricht University, Maastricht (1998).

Google Scholar

16. Godin G, Kok G. The theory of planned behavior: a review of its applications to health-related behaviors. Am J Health Promot. (1996) 11:87–98. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Brug J, Debie S, van Assema P, Weijts W. Psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among adults: results of focus group interviews. Food Qual Preference. (1995) 6:99–107. doi: 10.1016/0950-3293(95)98554-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sandvik C, Gjestad R, Brug J, Rasmussen M, Wind M, Wolf A, et al. The application of a social cognition model in explaining fruit intake in Austrian, Norwegian and Spanish Schoolchildren Using Structural Equation Modelling. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2007) 4:57. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-4-57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hartman H, Wadsworth DP, Penny S, van Assema P, Page R. Psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among students in a New Zealand University. Results of Focus Group Interviews. Appetite. (2013) 65:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Conner M, Norman P (editors.) Predicting Health Behaviour: Research and Practice With Social Cognition Models, 2nd edn. Maidenhead: Open University Press (2007).

Google Scholar

21. Sheeran P, Maki A, Montanaro E, Avishai-Yitshak A, Bryan A, Klein WMP, et al. The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. (2016) 35:1178–88. doi: 10.1037/hea0000387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. (2006) 132:249–68. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Guillaumie L, Godin G, Vezina-Im L-A. Psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable intake in adult population: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2010) 7:12. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. (1977) 84:191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Brug J, de Vet E, de Nooijer J, Verplanken B. Predicting fruit consumption: cognitions, intention, and habits. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2006) 38:73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2005.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lally P, Gardner B. Promoting habit formation. Health Psychol Rev. (2013) 7:S137–S58. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2011.603640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Menon U, Belue R, Wahab S, Rugen K, Kinney AY, Maramaldi P, et al. A randomized trial comparing the effect of two phone-based interventions on colorectal cancer screening adherence. Ann Behav Med. (2011) 42:294–303. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9291-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Triandis HC. Interpersonal Behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. (1977).

Google Scholar

29. Reinaerts E, de Nooijer J, Candel M, de Vries N. Explaining school children's fruit and vegetable consumption: the contributions of availability, accessibility, exposure, parental consumption and habit in addition to psychosocial factors. Appetite. (2007) 48:248–58. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. de Bruijn G-J. Understanding college students' fruit consumption. Integrating habit strength in the theory of planned behaviour. Appetite. (2010) 54:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. de Vries H, Eggers S.M, Lechner L, van Osch L, van Stralen MM. Predicting fruit consumption: the role of habits, previous behavior and mediation effects. BMC Public Health. (2014) 14:730. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Stets JE, Burke PJ. Identity theory and social identity theory. Soc Psychol Q. (2000) 63:224. doi: 10.2307/2695870

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sparks P. Subjective expected utility-based attitude-behavior models: the utility of self-identity. In: Terry DJ, Hogg MA, editors. Applied Social Research. Attitudes, Behavior, and Social Context: The Role of Norms and Group Membership. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum (2000). p. 31–46.

Google Scholar

34. Canova L, Bobbio A, Manganelli AM. Predicting fruit consumption: a multi-group application of the theory of planned behavior. Appetite. (2020) 145:104490. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Carfora V, Caso D, Conner M. The role of self-identity in predicting fruit and vegetable intake. Appetite. (2016) 106:23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jung SE, Bice C. The role of self-identity in predicting college students' intention to consume fruits and vegetables. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2019) 51:173–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2018.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rise J, Sheeran P, Hukkelberg S. The role of self-identity in the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2010) 40:1085–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kock N, Hadaya P. Minimum sample size estimation in PLS-SEM: the inverse square root and gamma-exponential methods: sample size in PLS-Based SEM. Info Systems J. (2018) 28:227–61. doi: 10.1111/isj.12131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ke-You G, Da-Wei F. The magnitude and trends of under-and over-nutrition in Asian countries. Biomed Environ Sci. (2001) 41:53–60.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

40. Kock N. WarpPLS User Manual: Version 7.0. Laredo: ScriptWarp Systems (2020).

Google Scholar

41. Kock N. Factor-based structural equation modeling with WarpPLS. Austr Market J. (2019) 27:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ausmj.2019.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Brown TA. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research; Methodology in the Social Sciences, 2nd edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2015).

Google Scholar

43. Hair JF. Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global Perspective. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education (2010).

Google Scholar

44. Kock N. Using WarpPLS in E-collaboration studies: mediating effects, control and second order variables, and algorithm choices. Int J e-Collaboration. (2011) 7:1–13. doi: 10.4018/jec.2011070101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hair JF. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equations Modeling (PLS-SEM). Los Angeles, CA: SAGE (2014).

Google Scholar

46. Bagozzi RP, Yi Y, Phillips LW. Assessing construct validity in organizational research. Admin Sci Q. (1991) 36:421. doi: 10.2307/2393203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Wetzels M, Odekerken-Schröder G, van Oppen C. Using PLS path modeling for assessing hierarchical construct models: guidelines and empirical illustration. MIS Q. (2009) 33:177. doi: 10.2307/20650284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tenenhaus M, Amato S, Esposito Vinzi V. A global goodness-of-fit index for PLS structural equation modelling. In: Proceedings of the XLII SIS Scientific Meeting, Vol. 1. Padova: CLEUP (2004). p.739–42.

Google Scholar

49. Cohen J. A Power primer. Psychol Bull. (1992) 112:155–9. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Geisser S. The predictive sample reuse method with applications. J Am Stat Assoc. (1975) 70:320–8. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1975.10479865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hair JF, Ringle CM, Sarstedt M. PLS-SEM: indeed a silver bullet. J Market Theory Pract. (2011) 19:139–52. doi: 10.2753/MTP1069-6679190202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ghimire S, Singh DR, Nath D, Jeffers EM, Kaphle M. Adult children's migration and well-being of left behind nepalese elderly parents. JEGH. (2018) 8:154. doi: 10.2991/j.jegh.2018.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Wiig Dammann K, Smith C. Factors affecting low-income women's food choices and the perceived impact of dietary intake and socioeconomic status on their health and weight. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2009) 41:242–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2008.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Singh UK, Gautam N, Bhandari TR, Sapkota N. Educational intervention of intention change for consumption of junk food among school adolescents in Birgunj Metropolitan City, Nepal, Based on Theory of Planned Behaviors. J Nutr Metab. (2020) 2020:1–7. doi: 10.1155/2020/7932324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Rose T, Barker M, Maria Jacob C, Morrison L, Lawrence W, Strömmer S, et al. A Systematic Review of Digital Interventions for Improving the Diet and Physical Activity Behaviors of Adolescents. J Adolesc Health. (2017) 61:669–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Menozzi D, Sogari G, Mora C. Explaining vegetable consumption among young adults: an application of the theory of planned behaviour. Nutrients. (2015) 7:7633–50. doi: 10.3390/nu7095357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Witte K. Fishing for success: using the persuasive health message framework to generate effective campaign messages. In: Maibach EW, Parrott RL, editors. Designing Health Messages: Approaches from Communication Theory and Public Health Practice. California: SAGE Publications (1995). p. 145–66.

Google Scholar

58. Povey R, Conner M, Sparks P, James R, Shepherd R. Application of the theory of planned behaviour to two dietary behaviours: roles of perceived control and self-efficacy. Br J Health Psychol. (2000) 5:121–39. doi: 10.1348/135910700168810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kjeldsberg C, Shrestha N, Patel M, Davis D, Mundy G, Cunningham K. Nutrition-sensitive agricultural interventions and gender dynamics: a qualitative study in Nepal. Matern Child Nutr. (2018) 14:e12593. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bhandari P, Kim M. Predictors of the health-promoting behaviors of nepalese migrant workers. J Nurs Res. (2016) 24:232–9. doi: 10.1097/jnr.0000000000000120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bandura A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman (1997).

Google Scholar

62. Kreausukon P, Gellert P, Lippke S, Schwarzer R. Planning and self-efficacy can increase fruit and vegetable consumption: a randomized controlled trial. J Behav Med. (2012) 35:443–51. doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9373-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Luszczynska A, Horodyska K, Zarychta K, Liszewska N, Knoll N, Scholz U. Planning and self-efficacy interventions encouraging replacing energy-dense foods intake with fruit and vegetable: a longitudinal experimental study. Psychol Health. (2016) 31:40–64. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2015.1070156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Verplanken B, Orbell S. Self-reported habit: a self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2003) 33:1313–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ouellette JA, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: the multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol Bull. (1998) 124:54–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Grosso G, Marventano S, Nolfo F, Rametta S, Bandini L, Ferranti R, et al. A personal eating, lifestyle, and family-related behaviors correlate with fruit and vegetable consumption in adolescents living in Sicily, Southern Italy. Int J Vitam Nutr Res. (2013) 83:355–66. doi: 10.1024/0300-9831/a000177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Singh JK, Acharya D, Gautam S, Adhikari M, Park J-H, Yoo S-J, et al. Socio-demographic and diet-related factors associated with insufficient fruit and vegetable consumption among adolescent girls in rural communities of Southern Nepal. IJERPH. (2019) 16:2145. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16122145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Owens E, Marsh T, Rittenberry L, de Moor C. Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100% fruit juice, and vegetables influence children's dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav. (2003) 30:615–26. doi: 10.1177/1090198103257254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Harris-Fry HA, Paudel P, Shrestha N, Harrisson T, Beard BJ, Jha S, et al. Status and determinants of intra-household food allocation in rural Nepal. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2018) 72:1524–36. doi: 10.1038/s41430-017-0063-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Riediger ND, Shooshtari S, Moghadasian MH. The influence of sociodemographic factors on patterns of fruit and vegetable consumption in canadian adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc. (2007) 107:1511–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Miller KD, Siegel RL, Lin CC, Mariotto AB, Kramer JL, Rowland JH, et al. A cancer treatment and survivorship statistics, 2016. CA Cancer J Clin. (2016) 66:271–89. doi: 10.3322/caac.21349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Dhungana RR, Devkota S, Khanal MK, Gurung Y, Giri RK, Parajuli RK, et al. A prevalence of cardiovascular health risk behaviors in a remote rural community of Sindhuli District, Nepal. BMC Cardiovasc Disord. (2014) 14:92. doi: 10.1186/1471-2261-14-92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Broaddus-Shea ET, Thorne-Lyman AL, Manohar S, Nonyane BAS, Winch PJ, West KP. Seasonality of consumption of nonstaple nutritious foods among young children from Nepal's 3 agroecological zones. Curr Dev Nutr. (2018) 2:nzy058. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzy058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Wood W, Neal DT. Healthy through habit: interventions for initiating & maintaining health behavior change. Behav Sci Policy. (2016) 2:71–83. doi: 10.1353/bsp.2016.0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Adriaanse MA, Verhoeven A. Breaking habits using implementation intentions. In: Verplanken B, editor. The Psychology of Habit. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2018). p. 169–88.

Google Scholar

76. Rothman AJ, Sheeran P, Wood W. Reflective and automatic processes in the initiation and maintenance of dietary change. Ann Behav Med. (2009) 38:4–17. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9118-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Strachan SM, Brawley LR. Healthy-eater identity and self-efficacy predict healthy eating behavior: a prospective view. J Health Psychol. (2009) 14:684–95. doi: 10.1177/1359105309104915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kendzierski D. A Self-schema approach to healthy eating. J Am Psychiatr Nurses Assoc. (2007) 12:350–7. doi: 10.1177/1078390306298983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Brouwer AM, Mosack KE. “I Am a Blood Sugar Checker”: intervening effects of self-as-doer identity on the relationship between self-efficacy and diabetes self-care behaviors. Self Identity. (2012) 11:472–91. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2011.603901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Brouwer AM, Mosack KE. Motivating healthy diet behaviors: the self-as-doer identity. Self Identity. (2015) 14:638–53. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2015.1043335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Althubaiti A. Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. JMDH. (2016) 9:211–7. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S104807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Maukonen M, Männistö S, Tolonen H. A comparison of measured versus self-reported anthropometrics for assessing obesity in adults: a literature review. Scand J Public Health. (2018) 46:565–79. doi: 10.1177/1403494818761971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vegetable intake, psychosocial determinants, partial least square structural equation modelling, young adult, Nepal

Citation: Pandey S, Budhathoki M and Yadav DK (2021) Psychosocial Determinants of Vegetable Intake Among Nepalese Young Adults: An Exploratory Survey. Front. Nutr. 8:688059. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.688059

Received: 30 March 2021; Accepted: 18 May 2021;
Published: 10 June 2021.

Edited by:

Jena Shaw Tronieri, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Reviewed by:

Umesh Ghimire, New ERA, Nepal
Natalie Caine-Bish, Kent State University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Pandey, Budhathoki and Yadav. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sujita Pandey, psujita01@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.