- 1Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
- 2Neurology Service, Santiago Clinic Hospital (CHUS), Santiago de Compostela, Spain
- 3NeuroAging Group (NEURAL), Clinical Neurosciences Research Laboratory (LINC), Health Research Institute of Santiago de Compostela (IDIS), Santiago de Compostela, Spain
- 4Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
Introduction: This study aimed to evaluate, in adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the brain atrophy that may distinguish between three AT(N) biomarker-based profiles, and to determine its clinical value.
Methods: Structural MRI (sMRI) was employed to evaluate the volume and cortical thickness differences in MCI patients with different AT(N) profiles, namely, A−T−(N)−: normal AD biomarkers; A+T−(N)−: AD pathologic change; and A+T+(N)+: prodromal AD. Sensitivity and specificity of these changes were also estimated.
Results: An initial atrophy in medial temporal lobe (MTL) areas was found in the A+T−(N)− and A+T+(N)+ groups, spreading toward the parietal and frontal regions in A+T+(N)+ patients. These structural changes allowed distinguishing AT(N) profiles within the AD continuum; however, the profiles and their pattern of neurodegeneration were unsuccessful to determine the current clinical status.
Conclusion: sMRI is useful in the determination of the specific brain structural changes of AT(N) profiles along the AD continuum, allowing differentiation between MCI adults with or without pathological AD biomarkers.
Introduction
Recently, the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) proposed the AT(N) research framework in which Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is defined as a biological entity based on a dichotomous classification of normal (−) or abnormal (+) β-amyloid deposition (A), pathological tau (T), and neurodegeneration (N) biomarkers in living persons. While A and T are considered specific of AD, neuropathological (N) biomarkers [e.g., cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) total tau, FDG PET hypometabolism, and atrophy on MRI] are considered non-specific indicators of damage for AD which may derive from a variety of etiologies (Jack et al., 2016, 2018).
However, structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) studies showed that atrophy progression in the AD continuum follows the stereotypical pattern of cortical tau tangles spread formalized into the Braak’s staging model (Braak and Braak, 1991). According to this model, cortical tau tangles are first observed in medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures, including the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, then advances toward other MTL and limbic structures, and finally spread to frontal, parietal, and temporal cortical regions, as well as to subcortical structures, although different trajectories have been recently identified (Vogel et al., 2021). Therefore, the structural changes of brain assessed with MRI measures and progression could be used as objective and more specific indexes of neurodegeneration in the AD continuum (Bayram et al., 2018; Chandra et al., 2019).
Efforts have been made in finding more accurate neurodegenerative indices with precise anatomical location of gray matter (GM) or white matter (WM) changes. Allison et al. (2019) found that different MRI indices differentiated individuals across the AD continuum (Allison et al., 2019), with global atrophy being a robust general marker for neurodegeneration, equivalent to total tau measures, but, therefore, non-specific. Ekman et al. (2018) established a sequence of brain atrophy from A+T−(N)− to A+T+(N)− to A+T+(N)+; however, they measured the brain atrophy patterns using visual rating scales that only allow a global examination of neurodegenerative changes. Schwarz et al. (2016) recommended an AD signature combining several cortical thickness measures to distinguish the severity of AD (Schwarz et al., 2016), correlating with Braak’s neurofibrillary tangle staging, but this study did not distinguish between groups following the AT(N) framework.
Consequently, there is still a scarce evidence of the differences of brain atrophy between profiles proposed within the AT(N) framework. In addition, concerns about the prognostic value and clinical relevance of the profiles have also been raised (Morris et al., 2018). In this regard, it should be mentioned two recent studies conducted in elderly non-demented population (Ingala et al., 2021) and in adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), defined as a clinical syndrome in which activities of daily living are preserved but there is an objective cognitive impairment (Petersen et al., 2014), and patients with dementia of Alzheimer’s type (DAT) (Allegri et al., 2020).
Ingala et al. (2021) found that healthy non-demented population with positivity in phosphorylated tau (T +) showed cognitive dysfunction, especially in the memory domain, and non-demented adults with positivity in one or more AT(N) biomarkers (i.e., A+T−(N)−, A+T+(N)−, A+T−(N)+, and A+T+(N)+) displayed reduced volume in the amygdala, entorhinal cortex, and nucleus accumbens, showing an increase posterior cortical atrophy and cerebrovascular burden across the AD continuum profiles. Comparing cognitively unimpaired (CU) adults, individuals with early MCI or late MCI, and DAT patients, Allegri et al. (2020) found higher neurodegeneration in DAT patients compared with early MCI and late MCI subtypes and higher neurodegeneration in all patient groups (i.e., early MCI, late MCI, and DAT) compared with CU adults. In addition, they found that conversion to AD dementia was observed in a 60-month follow-up in 85% of individuals with the A+T+(N)+ profile.
Both investigations revealed important findings about the impact of AT(N) biomarker positivity on brain atrophy and cognitive function in non-demented adults, people with MCI, or patients with AD dementia. However, there is still a need for a more complete characterization of those neurodegenerative changes that occur in people with MCI given the AT(N) biomarker levels, especially in hippocampal subfields and also in other regions with an important relevance in AD dementia etiology, such as those areas included in the AD signature proposed by Schwarz et al. (2016), and its relationship with cognitive performance.
Consequently, and with the purpose of shedding more light on this unexplored area, the aims of this study were, first, to assess, using sMRI procedures, the brain structural changes, including a previously described index (i.e., AD signature index), that may distinguish between three AT(N) biomarker profiles [1], namely, (1) normal CSF biomarkers with MCI (A−T−(N)−), (2) AD pathologic change with MCI (A+T−(N)−), and (3) AD with MCI or prodromal AD (A+T+(N)+), and, second, to determine the clinical value of these profiles, i.e., to explore the relationship of subgroups of the AD continuum and the diagnosis of MCI subtypes and compare the cognitive performance between the three AT(N) biomarker profiles, using for that purpose, an extensive neuropsychological assessment.
Adults diagnosed with MCI were recruited, and CSF biomarkers were measured. Using sMRI, we aimed to evaluate between-group differences in GM and WM volume and cortical thickness at the whole brain with a follow-up region of interest (ROI) analysis in the hippocampal subfields and its surrounding MTL structures. We hypothesize that, compared with the other groups, adults with the A+T+(N)+ profile would display higher atrophy in the MTL and hippocampal subfields as well as in the frontal and posterior parietal cortices, and, therefore, atrophy in the AD signature index. We also expect worse performance in neuropsychological tests in the A+T+(N)+ group compared with the other groups.
Materials and Methods
Participants
A total of 37 adults with clinical diagnosis of MCI according to the standard criteria (Winblad et al., 2004; Albert et al., 2011) were selected from the longitudinal Compostela Aging Study and divided into three groups matched by age, gender, and years of education (refer to Table 1) as follows: 15 A−T−(N)− normal AD biomarkers (mean age: 69.67 years, SD: 5.41), 10 A+T−(N)− AD pathologic change (mean age: 72.60 years, SD: 9.98), and 12 A+T+(N)+ prodromal AD (mean age: 74.33 years, SD: 4.48).
Table 1. Mean values and standard deviations (SD, in brackets) of demographic, CSF, and neuropsychological measures.
Participants gave their written informed consent prior the participation and did not report any previous diagnosis of any neurological disorder or psychiatric disturbances, history of clinical stroke, motor-sensory deficit, or substance abuse/dependence. All participants had normal audition and normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were right-handed as evaluated by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). The research project was approved by the Galician Clinical Research Ethics Committee and is in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki ethical standards (Lynöe et al., 1991).
Neuropsychological Assessment
Participants underwent extensive neuropsychological and cognitive assessment including the following tests: (a) short Spanish version of the “Questionnaire d’auto-évaluation de la Mémoire” (QAM) (Van der Linden et al., 1989) to evaluate subjective cognitive complains (SCCs) from patients and informants; (b) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) and Cambridge Cognitive Examination – Revised (CAMCOG-R) (Roth et al., 2015) to evaluate general cognitive functioning and several cognitive domains, respectively; (c) California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (Delis et al., 1987) to assess memory; (d) Trail Making Test (TMT-A and TMT-B) (Reynolds, 2002) to evaluate attention and executive functioning; and (e) semantic (animals) and phonological (say in 1 min words starting with “p”) verbal fluency (Lezak et al., 2004), Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Kaplan et al., 1983), and the vocabulary test (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised or WAIS-R) (Weschler, 2002) to assess language. Moreover, functional assessment was done using the Lawton and Brody Index (maximum possible scoring = 8) to evaluate instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) (Lawton and Brody, 1969) and depression was evaluated with the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) (Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986).
Mild cognitive impairment was diagnosed following the criteria proposed by Albert et al. (2011) as follows: (i) evidence of concern about a change in cognitive function, in comparison with the previous level corroborated by informants (QAM); (ii) evidence of poorer performance in one or more cognitive domains that is greater than expected for the age and educational background of patient; for this criterion, we established to have a performance score of 1.5 SD below age- and education-related norms in general cognitive functioning (MMSE) and in one or more cognitive domains (CAMCOG-R and CVLT); (iii) preservation of independence (IADL); and (iv) non-fulfillment of diagnostic of dementia according the DSM-V (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition) and NIA-AA criteria. Besides, participants were classified following the criteria proposed by Petersen (2004) and Winblad et al. (2004) into three types: (i) individuals with multiple-domain amnestic MCI (mda-MCI) (n = 24) who scored 1.5 SD below age- and education-related norms in the MMSE, and in at least two CAMCOG-R subscales and also in the CVLT (short and long delay free recall); (ii) individuals with single-domain amnestic MCI (sda-MCI) (n = 11), with normal cognitive functioning in the MMSE and CAMCOG-R subscales, but with memory impairment in the CVLT; and (iii) individuals with multiple-domain non-amnestic MCI (mdna-MCI) (n = 2) with normal memory functioning, but with cognitive impairments as in the mda-MCI type. Diagnoses were reached by consensus within the research team.
AT(N) Biomarker Measurement
Participants underwent a lumbar puncture, following the protocolized recommendations to standardize pre-analytical confounding factors in AD CSF biomarkers (Del Campo et al., 2012). CSF biomarkers [e.g., β-amyloid(1–42), hTAU-Ag, and phospo-tau 181P] were assessed with INNOTEST® sandwich ELISA (Fujirebio Europe, Ghent, Belgium) according to the procedures of the manufacturer. All the data were generated in a single center through the routine activity of the Clinical Neuroscience Research Laboratory (LINC) at the Health Institute of Santiago de Compostela (IDIS) as described by Jongbloed et al. (2013). Due to the number of included patients, the lots of assay kits were variable. However, the quality of the results was ensured by the use of validated standard operating procedures and internal quality controls (QCs). The range of the QC coefficient of variation for Aβ42 across the different lots was 12–13%. For tau and p-tau, the range was 10–13% and 7.3–11%, respectively. The range of the QC intra-assay coefficient of variation was <8% for all biomarkers.
The relationship of Aβ1-42, t-Tau, 181p-Tau, and ratios of their cutoff levels with test result was measured with INNOTEST® as follows: if Aβ1-42 < 638 pg/ml, the test result is positive; if t-Tau >375 pg/ml, the test result is positive; if 181p-Tau >52 pg/ml, the test result is positive; and if t-Tau/Aβ1–42 ratio >0.52, the test result is positive. These cutoff points were calculated by Fujirebio specifically for the LINC following the Alzheimer’s Biomarkers Standardization Initiative (Molinuevo et al., 2014).
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Acquisition and Data Analysis
For sMRI analysis, a sagittal T1-weighted 3D-MPRAGE sequence (i.e., repetition time/echo time = 7.45 ms/3.40 ms, flip angle = 8°; 180 slices, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm, field of view = 240 × 240 mm2, and matrix size = 240 × 240 mm) was acquired on a Philips 3T Achieva scanner (Philips Medical System, Best, Netherlands) at the University Hospital Complex in Santiago de Compostela, Galicia (Spain). Head motion was minimized by using a head restraint system and by placing foam padding around the head of subjects. Participants were provided with headphones to attenuate scanner noise.
The differences in GM and WM volume were evaluated performing a voxel-based morphometry analysis in Matlab R2016a using the Computational Anatomy Toolbox1 implemented in the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12).2 T1-weighted images were visually inspected and reoriented to the anterior-posterior commissure, segmented in GM and WM tissues (Ashburner and Friston, 2005), and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using a customized template built with the DARTEL toolbox (Ashburner, 2007). Next, the normalized GM/WM-modulated images were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel Full Width at Half Maximum (8-mm FWHM). Statistical analyses were conducted using the generalized linear modeling (GLM) approach and between-group analysis was performed using the normalized and smoothed GM- and WM-modulated images via 2 one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests with a between-subject factor Group (three levels: A−T−(N)−, A+T−(N)−, and A+T+(N)+) including the total intracranial volume as covariate. Results were assessed at p < 0.05 with family-wise error (FWE) cluster-level corrected for multiple comparisons combined with a threshold of p < 0.001 at the uncorrected voxel level.
Cortical thickness differences were evaluated performing a surface-based morphometry analysis using the FreeSurfer version 6.0 software.3 We employed the automated default preprocessing pipeline of cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation (Dale et al., 1999) that included motion correction (Reuter et al., 2010), skull stripping (Segonne et al., 2004), transformation into the Talairach space, segmentation of cortical and subcortical GM/WM volumetric structures (Fischl et al., 2002, 2004), intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), tessellation of the boundary between GM and WM, and topology correction (Segonne et al., 2007). Pial and WM segmentations were visually inspected and corrected when necessary. Between-group analysis was performed via a GLM with a Monte Carlo simulation multiple comparisons correction with 10,000 permutations, a cluster-forming threshold set at p < 0.005 and a smoothing kernel of 15-mm FWHM. Results were considered significant at p < 0.05.
A follow-up ROI analysis was performed over MTL given that the whole-brain analysis revealed significant differences in this area (refer to Table 2). Hippocampal subfields were automatically segmented with FreeSurfer (Iglesias et al., 2015) (refer to Figure 1). After a visual QC, these volume measurements were exported: whole hippocampus and its head, body, and tail; parasubiculum; and head and body of presubiculum, subiculum, CA1, CA3 (CA2 is included in CA3), CA4, granulate cell of the molecular layer of dentate gyrus, hippocampal molecular layer, hippocampal fissure, fimbria, and the hippocampus-amygdala transition area. The entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus volume and all hippocampal subfields volume measurements were adjusted using the estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV) employing a formula: adjusted_volume = volume_observed - b × (eTIV - mean_eTIV), where mean_eTIV is the average eTIV of all subjects and b is the regression coefficient between the volume observed and the eTIV. In comparison to other adjustment approaches, this adjustment method, also known as residual approach, demonstrated to be optimal for discriminating not only between CU adults and individuals with AD dementia but also between people with MCI and adults with AD dementia (Voevodskaya, 2014). Between-group analysis was performed using a multivariate GLM including the Group as the fixed factor and all the volume measures as dependent variables. The Holm-Bonferroni method was employed to correct for multiple comparisons, and significance level was set at p < 0.05.
Table 2. Brain regions that showed significant gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) volume differences and cortical thickness differences in the between-group analyses.
Figure 1. An illustrative example of the hippocampal subfield segmentation performed in FreeSurfer. CA, cornu ammonis; GC-ML-DG, granule cell of the molecular layer of dentate gyrus; ML-HP, molecular layer of the hippocampus; HATA, hippocampus-amygdala-transition-area; HP-fissure, hippocampus fissure; HP-tail, hippocampus tail.
To contrast the relationship between groups (established by CSF AT(N) profiles) and clinical diagnosis (different MCI subtypes), a chi-squared test was carried out. One-way ANOVAs with CSF biomarker profile as main factor with three levels, namely, A−T−(N)−, A+T−(N)−, and A+T+(N)+, were performed to compare the cognitive performance in a battery of neuropsychological tests, and pairwise comparisons corrected to Holm–Bonferroni were performed in case of significant effects (p < 0.05). Moreover, AD signature (Schwarz et al., 2016) was computed by averaging the FreeSurfer thickness estimates from the entorhinal cortex, inferior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior parietal lobe, fusiform gyrus, and precuneus. To determine the sensitivity and specificity of the brain structural changes to distinguish between AT(N) profiles, receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analyses were performed.
Results
Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging
The WM volume showed a significant group effect in the parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus, and inferior/middle temporal gyrus of the left hemisphere. Compared with the A−T−(N)− group, the A+T−(N)− group showed a significant reduced WM volume in the parahippocampal gyrus and hippocampus of the left hemisphere, and the A+T+(N)+ group showed a significant reduced WM volume in the parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus, lingual gyrus, and middle temporal gyrus of the left hemisphere; bilateral inferior temporal gyrus; and fusiform gyrus, middle frontal gyrus (orbital part), and anterior cingulate of the right hemisphere (refer to Figure 2 and Table 2). No significant group effect was found in the GM volume.
Figure 2. Brain regions with significant white matter (WM) volume and cortical thickness differences in the between-group analyses.
Cortical thickness showed a significant group effect in the left inferior parietal lobule and bilateral lateral occipital cortex. Compared with the A−T−(N)− group, the A+T+(N)+ group showed a significant thinning in the inferior parietal lobule, precuneus, posterior cingulate, supramarginal gyrus, and entorhinal cortex of the left hemisphere and in the lateral occipital cortex and superior frontal gyrus of the right hemisphere (refer to Figure 2 and Table 2).
The follow-up ROI analyses of MTL (refer to Table 3) showed, for brain volume and thickness, significant group effects. A+T+(N)+ participants showed reduced volume compared with A−T−(N)− participants at the bilateral regions (e.g., whole hippocampus and its head, body, and tail; head and body of subiculum; head of CA1; head and body of the hippocampal molecular layer; and the entorhinal cortex) and left regions (e.g., head and body of presubiculum, body of CA1, body of CA3, head and body of CA4, head and body of granulate cell of dentate gyrus, and parahippocampal gyrus). Also, a cortical thickness reduction in the bilateral entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus was found.
Table 3. Mean values and standard deviations (SD, in brackets) of the adjusted hippocampal subfields measures and its surrounding medial temporal lobe regions in which the between-group analyses revealed significant volume and thickness differences.
A+T+(N)+ participants showed reduced volume compared with A+T−(N)− participants at the bilateral regions (e.g., body of CA1) and left regions (e.g., whole hippocampus and its body and tail, body of CA3, body of CA4, and body of the hippocampal molecular layer).
A + T−(N)− participants showed reduced volume compared with A−T−(N)− participants at the right fimbria and left parahippocampal gyrus.
For the AD signature index, the ANOVA revealed a significant effect of the group factor, given that this index was larger for A+T+(N)+ participants than that for the A+T−(N)− (p = 0.048) and A−T−(N)− (p = 0.003) participants. Table 4 shows the ROC curve results. The AD signature index revealed two AUC values over 0.80, with high sensitivity and specificity to distinguish between A+T+(N)+ and A−T−(N)− and the profiles of AD continuum (A+T+(N)+ and A+T−(N)−) with respect to the A−T−(N)− profile.
Neuropsychological Assessment
Analysis of variances showed a significant group effect only in four different cognitive tests (refer to Table 1). Concretely, A+T+(N)+ presented worse performance than A−T−(N)− in the total score of CAMCOG-R, the memory test of CAMCOG-R, false positive in recognition of CVLT, and TMT-B. No further significant differences were found between groups. Moreover, chi-squared test revealed no differences in the proportion of the different MCI subtypes into the three profiles of the AT(N) classification (χ2 = 3.28; p = 0.512; refer to proportions in Table 1).
Discussion
This study had two aims: to obtain the indices of brain atrophy that may distinguish between three profiles within the AT(N) framework and to determine the clinical value of this classification, thereby relating it to the cognitive performance and clinical status.
The results confirm that the profiles based on CSF biomarkers are useful to estimate the degree of brain atrophy. Consistent with the results reported by Ekman et al. (2018) using visual rating scales, a gradation in brain atrophy indices (WM volume and/or cortical thickness) is appreciated, which allows distinguishing not only between those participants with normal AD biomarker (A−T−(N)−) profile and the AT(N) profiles of the AD continuum but also between those with the AD pathologic change (A+T−(N)−) and the prodromal AD (A+T+(N)+) profiles.
Specifically, compared with A−T−(N)− participants, the A+T−(N)− profile displayed atrophy in the right fimbria and left parahippocampal gyrus, but the A+T+(N)+ group showed more severe degeneration in several MTL structures (e.g., parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex, and hippocampal subfields). The WM volume changes in the parahippocampal gyrus were previously reported in AD patients (Li et al., 2012) and represent a possible biomarker of disease progression (Solodkin et al., 2013). The entorhinal cortex shows the earliest neurodegenerative changes in AD (Leal and Yassa, 2013), and its thinning predicts hippocampal atrophy when abnormal amyloid and p-tau elevated levels are present in MCI and AD dementia (Desikan et al., 2010). Moreover, this region is an important hub of the perforant pathway, a tract essential for episodic memory function by which the entorhinal cortex communicates to the dentate gyrus and other hippocampal subfields the inputs received from multiple cortical areas (Witter, 2007). The fimbria establish connections with the fornix, forming another important tract that connects the hippocampus with the hypothalamus (Leal and Yassa, 2013). Recent research demonstrated that neural damage in the fornix is related to amyloid burden and episodic memory decline (Rabin et al., 2019). Although other neuroimaging modalities (e.g., diffusion tensor imaging) can assess in more detail the microstructural damage of these tracts, the volume changes found in the fimbria in the A+T−(N)− group, and the extensive damage displayed by the A+T+(N)+ group in the entorhinal cortex and several hippocampal subfields, suggest a progressive disintegration of two brain networks functionally related to episodic memory, the neurocognitive system most affected in AD dementia.
Moreover, in line with the progression of subsequent stages of development of AD (Braak and Braak, 1991), the A+T+(N)+ participants showed, compared with A−T−(N)− participants, WM reductions in the temporal (inferior/middle temporal gyrus), frontal (orbital part of middle frontal gyrus), and occipito-temporal (fusiform and lingual gyrus) areas and the anterior cingulate cortex. This group also displayed a significant thinning in cortical thickness of the inferior parietal lobule, precuneus, posterior cingulate, supramarginal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, and the lateral occipital cortex. Overall, these structural changes found in MTL and occipito-temporal regions suggest that neurodegeneration is affecting regions with an important role in the consolidation and retrieval of episodic memories. However, the increased brain atrophy found in the A+T+(N)+ group seems to indicate that AD progression is expanding toward additional regions of parietal and frontal cortex involved in other cognitive domains, such as executive control.
These changes in brain integrity of the AD continuum profiles and the expected neurodegeneration process in AD according to Braak’s neurofibrillary tangle staging are especially evident in the structures that conform the AD signature index (Schwarz et al., 2016). In fact, ROC analyses evidenced that this index distinguished with high specificity and sensitivity between participants within the AD continuum and those with normal CSF levels of amyloid and tau. Given this clear relationship, it is possible that the AT(N) profiles represent a good predictor of disease progression, in line with previous reports (Bakkour et al., 2009; Dickerson and Wolk, 2013). Our results also indicate that exploring the AD signature may be useful as a biomarker to estimate that patients are in the AD continuum and to determine the current state of global neurodegeneration, regardless their cognitive performance and clinical status.
Precisely, once the validity of the CSF AT(N) framework in terms of brain atrophy has been confirmed, another aim of this study focused on checking the clinical relevance of these diagnostic profiles. In this sense, consistent with the brain atrophy results, the most extreme groups of the AT(N) framework (A−T−(N)− vs. A+T+(N)+) showed differences in a global measure of cognitive performance (CAMCOG-R Total), two measures of memory (CAMCOG-R Memory and CVLT false positives in recognition), and a measure of executive functions (TMT-B). However, no further differences were found between groups.
Previous investigations conducted in non-demented adults demonstrated that positivity in phosphorylated tau (T +) drives cognitive dysfunction, especially in the memory domain (Ingala et al., 2021), although there is also evidence that cognitive deficits in episodic memory, executive function, and global cognitive function are positively associated with amyloid burden in adults without a diagnosis of MCI or AD dementia (Hedden et al., 2013). Highly consistent with the present results, Ekman et al. (2018) found reduced episodic memory performance in MCI adults with an overall biomarker positivity (A+T+(N)+) in comparison to their counterparts with the A−T−(N)− profile. Thus, taking into account this previous evidence, the present results support the suggested idea that A+T+(N)+ profile represents an advanced-stage disease within the AD continuum in which disease progression is affecting the functioning of several cognitive domains, such as episodic memory or executive function (Allegri et al., 2020; Junquera et al., 2020).
Furthermore, no relationship was found between biomarker-based profiles in CSF and MCI clinical diagnosis. In fact, similar proportions of the different MCI subtypes were found in all three profiles. Therefore, AT(N) profiles do not seem to correspond with a progression in clinical severity among the different MCI subtypes, considering mda-MCI the most severe and sda-MCI the less severe (Brambati et al., 2009). Previous studies have already shown a lack of correspondence between CSF biomarker profiles and cognitive performance (Jack et al., 2019). Thus, as suggested in previous studies, there is a dissociation between the biomarker profile within the AD continuum and clinical status (Jack et al., 2019). Other factors such as the cognitive reserve may be involved in this dissociation (Facal et al., 2019; Stern et al., 2020).
The implications of these findings are present at different levels. In contrast, many studies have reported that the discrepancy in diagnostic criteria with respect to MCI may explain the divergence of results in previous literature in terms of characterization of brain integrity and activity in participants with MCI (Brambati et al., 2009), but, in line with the assessment of the NIA-AA (Jack et al., 2018), these discrepancies may be modulated by pathophysiological features in terms of the presence of amyloid and pathological tau. It is possible that, at least partially, this neuropathological heterogeneity of participants with MCI may also explain the variability in conversion rates to DAT found in previous studies (Facal et al., 2015). In contrast, even sharing a similar state of neurodegenerative changes, there does not seem to be a direct relationship between biomarker status and clinical status of a person. Therefore, apart from the importance of characterizing patients according to their biomarkers for the development of specific therapies that seek to slow the progression of neuropathological processes, the data obtained emphasize the need to obtain possible (e.g., genetic, molecular, and cognitive) triggering and/or protective factors that translate the neurodegenerative process associated with the AD continuum into the appearance of relevant clinical symptoms, with the aim of preventing and/or treating the appearance of symptoms that affect the cognitive performance of patients.
Finally, some limitations of this study are worth noting. Although no age differences were obtained between the three AT(N) groups, further research with larger samples is needed to explore the interaction between age and the CSF biomarker profiles. Also, a larger sample would allow to focus on possible interactions between AT(N) profiles and/or clinical diagnosis with different cognitive domains.
Conclusion
In sum, the study of sMRI measures revealed a progressive neurodegeneration along the AT(N) profiles consistent with the change pattern in AD (initial damage of MTL areas spreading toward the posterior parietal and frontal regions). Furthermore, the AD signature index, measuring the cortical thickness in brain regions vulnerable to AD, presented moderate-to-high sensitivity and specificity distinguishing the profiles within the AD continuum compared with those with normal CSF biomarkers. However, the AT(N) profiles and their pattern of degeneration were unsuccessful to determine the current clinical status; further research should clarify the factors that prevent or facilitate poorer cognitive performance and the presence of clinical symptoms given a certain amount of brain atrophy.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Galician Clinical Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
MR-F contributed to the design of methodology, conducted data collection and formal analyses, wrote the initial draft, and participated in the preparation of the published work. ML contributed to conceptualization and formulation of research goals, manuscript revision, design of methodology, data collection, preparation of the published work, and supervision. MZ and FD contributed to conceptualization and formulation of research goals, manuscript revision, data collection, provision of study materials, supervision, project administration, and funding acquisition. JA-V and JP-P contributed to conceptualization and formulation of research goals, design methodology, data collection, and formal analyses. LV-V contributed to CSF biomarker characterization. AP contributed to conceptualization and formulation of research goals, design methodology, data collection, formal analyses, and project administration. CL-S and AN-V contributed to data collection and formal analyses. SG- contributed to conceptualization and formulation of research goals, manuscript revision, design of methodology, data collection, preparation of the published work, and supervision.
Funding
This study was supported by grants from the Spanish Government, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (PSI2017-89389-C2-R and PID2020-114521RB-C21/C22); the Galician Government, Axudas para a Consolidación e Estruturación de Unidades de Investigación Competitivas do Sistema Universitario de Galicia: GRC (GI-1807-USC); Refs: ED431-2017/27 and ED431C-2021/04; all with ERDF/FEDER funds.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
- ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/
- ^ https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
- ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/
References
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
Allegri, R. F., Chrem Méndez, P., Calandri, I., Cohen, G., Martín, M. E., Russo, M. J., et al. (2020). Prognostic value of ATN Alzheimer biomarkers: 60-month follow-up results from the Argentine Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Alzheimers Dement. Diagnosis Assess. Dis. Monit 12:e12026. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12026
Allison, S. L., Koscik, R. L., Cary, R. P., Jonaitis, E. M., Rowley, H. A., Chin, N. A., et al. (2019). Comparison of different MRI-based morphometric estimates for defining neurodegeneration across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. NeuroImage Clin. 23:101895. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101895
Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007
Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26, 839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018
Bakkour, A., Morris, J. C., and Dickerson, B. C. (2009). The cortical signature of prodromal AD: Regional thinning predicts mild AD dementia. Neurology 72, 1048–1055. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000340981.97664.2f
Bayram, E., Caldwell, J. Z. K., and Banks, S. J. (2018). Current understanding of magnetic resonance imaging biomarkers and memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement Transl. Res. Clin. Interv. 4, 395–413. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2018.04.007
Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809
Brambati, S. M., Belleville, S., Kergoat, M. J., Chayer, C., Gauthier, S., and Joubert, S. (2009). Single- and multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment: two sides of the same coin? Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 28, 541–549. doi: 10.1159/000255240
Chandra, A., Dervenoulas, G., Politis, M., and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2019). Magnetic resonance imaging in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J. Neurol. 266, 1293–1302. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-9016-3
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395
Del Campo, M., Mollenhauer, B., Bertolotto, A., Engelborghs, S., Hampel, H., Simonsen, A. H., et al. (2012). Recommendations to standardize preanalytical confounding factors in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers: an update. Biomark. Med. 6, 419–430. doi: 10.2217/bmm.12.46
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. A. (1987). California Verbal Learning Test, 1st Version, Manual. New York, NY: Psychol. Corp. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Desikan, R. S., Sabuncu, M. R., Schmansky, N. J., Reuter, M., Cabral, H. J., Hess, C. P., et al. (2010). Selective disruption of the cerebral neocortex in alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One 5:12853. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012853
Dickerson, B. C., and Wolk, D. A. (2013). Biomarker-based prediction of progression in MCI: Comparison of AD signature and hippocampal volume with spinal fluid amyloid-β and tau. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00055
Ekman, U., Ferreira, D., and Westman, E. (2018). The A/T/N biomarker scheme and patterns of brain atrophy assessed in mild cognitive impairment. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26151-8
Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., and Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2015). Diagnostic transitions in mild cognitive impairment by use of simple Markov models. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30, 669–676. doi: 10.1002/gps.4197
Facal, D., Valladares-Rodriguez, S., Lojo-Seoane, C., Pereiro, A. X., Anido-Rifon, L., and Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2019). Machine learning approaches to studying the role of cognitive reserve in conversion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 34, 941–949. doi: 10.1002/gps.5090
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Van Der Kouwe, A. J. W., Makris, N., Ségonne, F., Quinn, B. T., et al. (2004). Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. NeuroImage 23, S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016
Fischl, B., Salat, D.-H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Hedden, T., Oh, H., Younger, A. P., and Patel, T. A. (2013). Meta-analysis of amyloid-cognition relations in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 80, 1341–1348. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828ab35d
Iglesias, J. E., Augustinack, J. C., Nguyen, K., Player, C. M., Player, A., Wright, M., et al. (2015). A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. Neuroimage 115, 117–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042
Ingala, S., De Boer, C., Masselink, L. A., Vergari, I., Lorenzini, L., Blennow, K., et al. (2021). Application of the ATN classification scheme in a population without dementia: findings from the EPAD cohort. Alzheimers Dement 17, 1189–1204. doi: 10.1002/alz.12292
Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., et al. (2018). NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 14, 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018
Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Feldman, H. H., Frisoni, G. B., et al. (2016). A/T/N: An unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology 87, 539–547. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002923
Jack, C. R., Therneau, T. M., Weigand, S. D., Wiste, H. J., Knopman, D. S., Vemuri, P., et al. (2019). Prevalence of Biologically vs. Clinically defined alzheimer spectrum entities using the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association research framework. JAMA Neurol. 76, 1174–1183. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1971
Jongbloed, W., Kester, M. I., Van Der Flier, W. M., Veerhuis, R., Scheltens, P., Blankenstein, M. A., et al. (2013). Discriminatory and predictive capabilities of enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay and multiplex platforms in a longitudinal Alzheimer’s disease study. Alzheimers Dement 9, 276–283. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.01.004
Junquera, A., Garcia-Zamora, E., Olazaran, J., Parra, M. A., and Fernandez-Guinea, S. (2020). Role of executive functions in the conversion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 77, 641–653. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200586
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PN: Lea & Febiger.
Lawton, M. P., and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9, 179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_part_1.179
Leal, S. L., and Yassa, M. A. (2013). Perturbations of neural circuitry in aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 12, 823–831. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.01.006
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, J. H., and Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Li, J. P., Pan, P. L., Huang, R., and Shang, H. F. (2012). A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies of white matter volume alterations in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 757–763. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.001
Lynöe, N., Sandlund, M., Dahlqvist, G., and Jacobsson, L. (1991). Informed consent: study of quality of information given to participants in a clinical trial. BMJ 303, 610–613. doi: 10.1136/bmj.303.6803.610
Molinuevo, J. L., Blennow, K., Dubois, B., Engelborghs, S., Lewczuk, P., Perret-Liaudet, A., et al. (2014). The clinical use of cerebrospinal fluid biomarker testing for Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis: a consensus paper from the Alzheimer’s Biomarkers Standardization Initiative. Alzheimers Dement 10, 808–817. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.03.003
Morris, G. P., Clark, I. A., and Vissel, B. (2018). Questions concerning the role of amyloid-β in the definition, aetiology and diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 136, 663–689. doi: 10.1007/s00401-018-1918-8
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Int. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x
Petersen, R. C., Caracciolo, B., Brayne, C., Gauthier, S., Jelic, V., and Fratiglioni, L. (2014). Mild cognitive impairment: A concept in evolution. J. Intern. Med. 275, 214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190
Rabin, J. S., Perea, R. D., Buckley, R. F., Johnson, K. A., Sperling, R. A., and Hedden, T. (2019). Synergism between fornix microstructure and beta amyloid accelerates memory decline in clinically normal older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 81, 38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.05.005
Reuter, M., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2010). Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage 53, 1181–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020
Roth, M., Tym, E., and Mountjoy, C. Q. (2015). CAMDEX. A standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 149, 698–709. doi: 10.1192/bjp.149.6.698
Schwarz, C. G., Gunter, J. L., Wiste, H. J., Przybelski, S. A., Weigand, S. D., Ward, C. P., et al. (2016). A large-scale comparison of cortical thickness and volume methods for measuring Alzheimer’s disease severity. NeuroImage Clin. 11, 802–812. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.05.017
Segonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22, 1060–1075.
Segonne, F., Pacheco, J., and Fischl, B. (2007). Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 26, 518–529. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.887364
Sheikh, J. I., and Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. 5, 165–173. doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01_09
Sled, J. G., Zijdenbos, A. P., and Evans, A. C. (1998). A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in mri data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 17, 87–97. doi: 10.1109/42.668698
Solodkin, A., Chen, E. E., Van Hoesen, G. W., Heimer, L., Shereen, A., Kruggel, F., et al. (2013). In vivo parahippocampal white matter pathology as a biomarker of disease progression to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 4300–4317. doi: 10.1002/cne.23418
Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., et al. (2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement 16, 1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219
Van der Linden, M., Wijns, C., Von Frenckell, R., Coyette, F., and Seron, X. (1989). Un Questionnaire D’auto-Évaluation de la Mémoire (QAM). Bruxelles: Editest.
Voevodskaya, O. (2014). The effects of intracranial volume adjustment approaches on multiple regional MRI volumes in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:264. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00264
Vogel, J. W., Young, A. L., Oxtoby, N. P., Smith, R., Ossenkoppele, R., Strandberg, O. T., et al. (2021). Four distinct trajectories of tau deposition identified in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Med. 27, 871–881. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01309-6
Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment - Beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x
Keywords: Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), AT(N), amyloid, tau, structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI), clinical diagnosis
Citation: Rivas-Fernández MÁ, Lindín M, Zurrón M, Díaz F, Aldrey-Vázquez JM, Pías-Peleteiro JM, Vázquez-Vázquez L, Pereiro AX, Lojo-Seoane C, Nieto-Vieites A and Galdo-Álvarez S (2022) Brain Atrophy and Clinical Characterization of Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment and Different Cerebrospinal Fluid Biomarker Profiles According to the AT(N) Research Framework of Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:799347. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.799347
Received: 21 October 2021; Accepted: 10 January 2022;
Published: 25 February 2022.
Edited by:
Til Menge, LVR Klinik Düsseldorf, GermanyReviewed by:
Gabriel Gonzalez-Escamilla, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, GermanyDaniel Llano, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United States
Copyright © 2022 Rivas-Fernández, Lindín, Zurrón, Díaz, Aldrey-Vázquez, Pías-Peleteiro, Vázquez-Vázquez, Pereiro, Lojo-Seoane, Nieto-Vieites and Galdo-Álvarez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Miguel Ángel Rivas-Fernández, bWlndWVsYW5nZWwucml2YXMuZmVybmFuZGV6QHVzYy5lcw==