Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 25 September 2024
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias

An effective screening model for subjective cognitive decline in community-dwelling older adults based on gait analysis and eye tracking

Chenxi HaoChenxi Hao1Xiaonan Zhang,Xiaonan Zhang2,3Junpin AnJunpin An1Wenjing BaoWenjing Bao1Fan YangFan Yang4Jinyu ChenJinyu Chen1Sijia HouSijia Hou1Zhigang WangZhigang Wang5Shuning DuShuning Du1Yarong ZhaoYarong Zhao1Qiuyan WangQiuyan Wang1Guowen Min
Guowen Min1*Yang Li
Yang Li1*
  • 1Department of Neurology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, China
  • 2Department of Radiology, First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, China
  • 3Department of Medical Imaging, Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, China
  • 4State Key Laboratory of Computer Science and Beijing Key Lab of Human-Computer Interaction, Institute of Software, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 5Department of First Clinical Medicine, Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan, China

Objective: To evaluate the effectiveness of multimodal features based on gait analysis and eye tracking for elderly people screening with subjective cognitive decline in the community.

Methods: In the study, 412 cognitively normal older adults aged over 65 years were included. Among them, 230 individuals were diagnosed with non-subjective cognitive decline and 182 with subjective cognitive decline. All participants underwent assessments using three screening tools: the traditional SCD9 scale, gait analysis, and eye tracking. The gait analysis involved three tasks: the single task, the counting backwards dual task, and the naming animals dual task. Eye tracking included six paradigms: smooth pursuit, median fixation, lateral fixation, overlap saccade, gap saccade, and anti-saccade tasks. Using the XGBoost machine learning algorithm, several models were developed based on gait analysis and eye tracking to classify subjective cognitive decline.

Results: A total of 161 gait and eye-tracking features were measured. 22 parameters, including 9 gait and 13 eye-tracking features, showed significant differences between the two groups (p < 0.05). The top three eye-tracking paradigms were anti-saccade, gap saccade, and median fixation, with AUCs of 0.911, 0.904, and 0.891, respectively. The gait analysis features had an AUC of 0.862, indicating better discriminatory efficacy compared to the SCD9 scale, which had an AUC of 0.762. The model based on single and dual task gait, anti-saccade, gap saccade, and median fixation achieved the best efficacy in SCD screening (AUC = 0.969).

Conclusion: The gait analysis, eye-tracking multimodal assessment tool is an objective and accurate screening method that showed better detection of subjective cognitive decline. This finding provides another option for early identification of subjective cognitive decline in the community.

1 Introduction

In our aging society, the number of people with dementia, particularly Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is increasing year by year (Cao et al., 2020). AD, characterized by progressive cognitive dysfunction and unusual behavior, accounts for 60% of cases (Kalaria et al., 2008), and the number of Chinese AD patients over 60 years old was about 9 million in 2019 (Jia et al., 2020). Patients will gradually lose the ability to take care of themselves, which places a heavy burden on the family and society. The NIA-AA workgroups proposed a model of the clinical trajectory of AD, indicating that once a patient is diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), their cognitive function declines more rapidly in the short term compared to the slower progression from the preclinical stage to MCI (Sperling et al., 2011). Therefore, our focus is on early identification and intervention during the preclinical stage. Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) may be considered as a preclinical stage of dementia. Patients with SCD experience memory loss complaints, but their cognitive decline cannot be recognized by standardized neuropsychological scales (Jessen et al., 2014; Lin et al., 2019). The average conversion rate from SCD to cognitive impairment (CI) is 19.8% (Li et al., 2023). Specifically, about 6.6% of older people with SCD will progress to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) annually, while the corresponding number to dementia is approximately 2.3% (Mitchell et al., 2014). Current research is focused on finding objective and simple methods and establishing accurate diagnostic models.

Currently, the diagnosis of SCD relies on a series of assessments, such as neuropsychological scales, imaging, behavioral indicators, and biomarkers. However, there is no uniform standard at present (Jessen et al., 2020). In 2015, a 9-item SCD questionnaire (SCD9) was first developed for SCD screening (Gifford et al., 2015). Researchers validated the value of the Chinese version and found that its sensitivity could reach more than 0.8 (Hao et al., 2017). However, other studies have also indicated that this questionnaire does not encompass all complaints related to cognitive decline (Hao et al., 2022). Therefore, it is necessary to explore a more suitable method for SCD screening for large-scale community surveys in China. Gait and eye tracking are two emerging methods to detect cognitive impairment. Numerous clinical studies assessing MCI and AD through gait and eye movements have produced positive outcomes. The overarching concept is that brain regions and neural circuits related to executive function and attention have been extensively studied and are well recognized. Imaging studies have consistently demonstrated the activity of these brain regions during gait and eye movement tasks. We hypothesize that behavioral changes may emerge at this stage. Furthermore, we aim to identify more accurate, convenient, and validated dual-task gait and eye movement paradigms suitable for large-scale community screening. The specific details are outlined below.

As people age, certain cognitive domains, including processing speed, executive functions, attention, memory, and visuospatial ability tend to decline (Harada et al., 2013). Large-scale studies have shown that over 50% of individuals over 70 years old reported subjective cognitive decline, despite having normal neuropsychological scale results (van Harten et al., 2018). Previous research has confirmed the correlation between gait characteristics and executive function (Ble et al., 2005; Holtzer et al., 2006), with this relationship becoming more pronounced in dual-task gait tests (Coppin et al., 2006). Several hypotheses about attention, a component of executive function, have been used to explain this more evident relationship (Yogev et al., 2008). Recent gait analysis findings related to MCI and AD have also supported the above conclusions (Bahureksa et al., 2017; Bovonsunthonchai et al., 2022). A community survey revealed that slow gait speed may manifest before the beginning of SCD, making it a considerable predictor of future cognitive dysfunction (Merchant et al., 2021). Therefore, it is reliable to study people with SCD in conducting different gait tasks.

Eye tracking involves multiple brain regions, including the cortex, superior colliculus, and thalamus, with different eye-tracking tests engaging distinct brain areas (Tao et al., 2020). Fixation, smooth pursuit, and saccade are the most commonly used methods (Opwonya et al., 2022). Attention and executive control both play crucial roles in eye movement (Luke et al., 2018; Mahon et al., 2018), and eye movement abnormalities may appear before memory deficits (Crawford and Higham, 2016). Recent reports have indicated that eye-tracking tasks can help predict the transition from normal cognition (NC) to cognitive impairment (Zola et al., 2013). A study found that young AD patients performed worse in a fixation task compared to age-matched normal controls (Pavisic et al., 2017). Another meta-analysis suggested that patients with mild cognitive impairment exhibited longer latency, lower accuracy and error correction rate in anti-saccade task (Zhang et al., 2023). Building on a consistent theoretical foundation, we aim to identify differences between individuals with SCD and cognitively normal people through gait and eye-tracking tests. Our goal is to discover more effective tasks for early and efficient identification of the SCD population.

The potential benefits of combining gait analysis and eye-tracking techniques for screening individuals with subjective cognitive decline are still uncertain. Machine learning demonstrates superior performance compared to traditional statistical methods. In our study, we employed the eye-tracking technique for the first time in a community-dwelling population and integrated it with dual-task gait analysis to obtain multimodal parameters for screening individuals with subjective cognitive decline. These parameters were evaluated using a machine learning model. Ultimately, we discovered key parameters for distinguishing the SCD population and developed a high-accuracy diagnostic model for identifying them.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

A total of 649 participants aged 65 years or older from three communities in Shanxi Province were selected for this study. Inclusion criteria for gait analysis and eye tracking included: (1) age ≥ 65 years; (2) completion of the cognitive neuropsychological assessments; (3) informed consent signed by the participants themselves or their attendant. Exclusion criteria for gait analysis included: (1) requirement of assistive devices for walking, such as walking sticks; (2) neurological diseases that may affect gait, such as cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, ataxia, motor neuron disease, traumatic brain injury; (3) other systemic diseases that may affect gait, such as osteoarthritis, tuberculosis of the spine, limb disability, and audio-visual disorders. Exclusion criteria for eye tracking included: (1) eye diseases that may affect eye tracking tests, such as cataract, glaucoma or other eye diseases; (2) other systematic diseases that may affect vision or eye movement, such as kinetic nerve paralysis; (3) inability to complete the calibration described below before the start of the test.

2.2 Neuropsychological assessment

The cognitive evaluators and neurologists in this study received professional training. First, participants scanned the QR code or completed the SCD9 scale in the help of volunteers on the spot. The Ascertain Dementia 8-item Informant Questionnaire (AD8) and Mini-Cog scales were initially assessed by a single assessor. Cognitive function was deemed normal if the AD8 score was <2 and the Mini-Cog score was ≥3. Participants with abnormal scores on either the AD8 or Mini-Cog underwent further assessment. The second stage included the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Activities of Daily Living (ADL) scale, and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR). Normal cognition was considered with CDR = 0, while cognitive impairment was CDR ≥ 0.5 (Petersen, 2004). The final diagnosis of CI was determined by two neurologists who took into account the scales, medical history, and educational background to make a comprehensive evaluation. The NC participants were further divided into two groups based on their responses to two questions regarding overall memory function on the SCD9 scale (Cheng et al., 2023). These questions were: “Do you think you have memory problems?” and “Overall, do you tend to forget things you need to do or say?” Responses were either “yes” or “no.” A “yes” to both questions indicated subjective cognitive decline (SCD), while all other responses indicated non-subjective cognitive decline (non-SCD). In total, 412 NC individuals were included: 230 with non-SCD and 182 with SCD. The flow chart of the study is shown in Figure 1. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, and all subjects signed an informed consent form.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The flow chart for SCD screening. SCD, subjective cognitive decline.

2.3 Gait analysis

Gait parameters were collected using an artificial motor function assessment system (ReadyGo, Beijing CAS-Ruiyi Information Technology Co., Ltd.). This system incorporates high-accuracy visual sensors and deep learning algorithms to pinpoint 32 skeletal points on the human body. It identifies and labels movements such as foot lifting, arm swinging, and turning, and then calculates the needed gait parameters automatically. The device can capture subjects within a 1 × 5 m2 area in front of the camera, so we ensure no other person appears in this space. Subjects do not need to wear sensors, making the process quick and easy. In all tests, subjects were required to walk at their normal speed. For the single-task test, subjects walked along a 3 meter walkway and turned at a designated point for three times, covering a total of 18 meters. For the dual-task test, subjects completed two tasks while walking: (i) counting backwards (e.g., 100, 99, 98, 97, …), (ii) naming as many animals as possible (e.g., monkey, turtle). During the dual-task test, subjects were asked to speak loud enough so that the operator could hear them clearly. The walking process should not be interrupted, even if the next number or animal name was not immediately recalled. We collected gait metrics including stance phase, swing phase, step width, step height, speed, step frequency, stride speed, turnaround time, coordination, step time variation, step width variation, and so on. We also calculated the dual-task cost (DTC) to reflect cognitive load affecting gait performance.

2.4 Eye tracking

Eye movement parameters were collected using an intelligent evaluation system (EyeKnow; Beijing CAS-Ruiyi Information Technology Co., Ltd.), with a 120 Hz sampling rate. We used a head-mounted VR glasses and a tablet computer to complete the test and collect data. Before the test, each subject was calibrated using a five-point process: top, bottom, left, right, and center, ensuring a maximum calibration error of 2° radius. The position of the VR glasses remained unchanged after calibration. Subjects performed smooth pursuit, fixation, pro-saccade, and anti-saccade tasks in sequence. Fixation included median fixation and lateral fixation tasks, while pro-saccade included overlap saccade and gap saccade tasks. The specific processes are detailed description as follows in Table 1. If needed, a pretest can be conducted to help subjects understand the process and requirements. In the smooth pursuit task, we analyzed parameters such as tracking accuracy and the number of offsets. In the fixation tasks, we analyzed the offsets, the total offsets, and offsets time. In the pro-saccade tasks, we analyzed accuracy, latency, and speed. In the anti-saccade task, we analyzed accuracy, latency, speed, error correction rate, and error correction time.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The specific processes of six eye tracking paradigms.

2.5 Statistical analysis

Data were processed using R software version 4.2.3 (Vienna, Austria). Continuous variables were assessed for normality and homogeneity of variance. Data that followed a normal distribution and had uniform variance were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (x ± s) and compared using the independent samples t-test. Otherwise, they were expressed as the median (interquartile range) [M (QR)] and compared using the Wilcoxon test. Categorical variables were described by frequency and rate (%) and compared using the χ2 test. A p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant. After excluding outliers and missing values, we used the SHAP (SHapley Additive exPlanation) method for variable selection, a post-hoc model interpretation technique that explains the output of any machine learning model. Then, the dataset was divided into a training set and a test set with a ratio of 7/3. The XGBoost algorithm was used to build a classification model. The model training and evaluation were repeated several times to reduce bias from different dataset divisions. Accuracy, sensitivity, specificity, and area under the curve (AUC) were used as comprehensive measures for model evaluation. Model stability was assessed using a 5-fold cross-validation method. In each iteration, four subsets are selected as the training set, and the remaining subset is used as the test set. The training set is used to train the model, and the test set is used to evaluate the performance of the model, and evaluation metrics such as accuracy, sensitivity, specificity, and AUC are calculated. The evaluation results on the test set in all iterations are summarized and average metrics are calculated. A total of 12 models were constructed (Single-task gait (ST), Dual-task gait (counting backward, CB), Dual-task gait (naming animals, NA), Single-task gait + Dual-task gait (ST + DT), Smooth pursuit (SP), Median fixation (MF), Lateral fixation (LF), Overlap saccade (OS), Gap saccade (GS), Anti-saccade (AS), Anti-saccade + Median fixation + Gap saccade (AS + MF + GS), Single-task gait + Dual-task gait + Anti-saccade + Median fixation + Gap saccade (ST + DT + AS + MF + GS)), and different modality combinations were compared. The details and results are presented in Table 2. The data collection and model-building process are shown in Figure 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Area under ROC curve and statistics of different models of two groups.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Process of data collection and model construction for gait and eye tracking test. SCD, subjective cognitive decline.

3 Results

3.1 Demographic and clinical characteristics

We recruited 649 participants from three communities in Shanxi Province for this study. Among them, 412 were diagnosed with normal cognition (NC): 230 with non-subjective cognitive decline (non-SCD) and 182 with subjective cognitive decline (SCD). Table 3 summarizes the baseline characteristics of the participants. Age, years of education, and BMI between the two groups showed no statistically significant differences (p > 0.05). There were more females in the SCD group compared to the non-SCD group, with a statistically significant difference (p < 0.05). The SCD9 score was significantly higher in the SCD group than in the non-SCD, with a statistically significant difference (p < 0.05).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Basic information comparisons of two groups.

3.2 Gait parameters and eye tracking parameters with differences between non-SCD and SCD groups

We collected a total of 125 gait parameters and 36 eye-tracking parameters. Then, 9 gait parameters and 13 eye tracking parameters were found significantly different between the two groups, as shown in Figure 3. In the single-task test, only the parameter of stride left was found significant difference between the two groups. About dual-task tests, three parameters including step width-DTC, step time left variation, and stride left variation-DTC, had statistical significance in two types of tests between two groups. In eye tracking paradigms, offsets were significantly different in smooth pursuit, median fixation, and lateral fixation between non-SCD and SCD Groups. In the anti-saccade test, accuracy, completion time, and error correction rate showed statistically significant differences in the two groups.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. 9 distinctive gait features and 13 distinctive eye tracking features (p < 0.05) between the SCD and non-SCD groups. SCD, subjective cognitive decline; DTC (dual-task cost) = ([single-task parameter − dual-task parameter]/single-task parameter) × 100%.

3.3 Performance of gait models for SCD detection

To evaluate the effectiveness of single-task and dual-task gait tests for SCD detection, we generated four machine learning models and evaluated the discriminative abilities of the single-task test, the counting backwards dual-task test, the naming animals dual-task test, and the combination of single-task and two dual-task tests, as shown in Figure 4A. We aimed to determine whether the dual-tasks were superior to the single-task and to compare the two dual-tasks in order to identify the more effective task for further research. Additionally, we sought to understand the extent to which combining a single task with a dual-task enhances discriminative ability. The model based on naming animals dual-task gait traits (AUC: 0.841) discriminated between SCD and non-SCD populations better than the model based on single-task gait traits (AUC: 0.823), which had a similar discriminatory ability as the counting backwards dual-task gait model (AUC: 0.824). The model combining single-task and two dual-task gait features (AUC: 0.862) outperformed all other gait models.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Power of different detect models to discriminate SCD and non-SCD population. (A) Models based on ST, CB, NA, and ST + DT features. (B) Models based on SP, MF, LF, OS, GS, AS, and AS + MF + GS features. (C) Models based on SCD9, ST + DT, AS + MF + GS, ST + DT + AS + MF + GS features. ST, the single task gait; DT, the dual task gait; CB, the counting backwards dual-task gait; NA, the naming animals dual-task gait; SP, smooth pursuit; MF, median fixation; LF, lateral fixation; OS, overlap saccade; GS, gap saccade; AS, anti-saccade.

3.4 Performance of eye tracking models in SCD detection

To evaluate the effectiveness of different eye-tracking paradigms for SCD detection, we generated seven machine learning models and evaluated them using metrics such as AUC, accuracy, sensitivity, and specificity. Previously, no consensus existed on which eye-tracking task was superior, and the specific processes used varied between studies. Therefore, we sought to determine which eye-tracking paradigm is superior and whether combined features can improve the results. We first evaluated the discrimination abilities of the smooth pursuit, median fixation, lateral fixation, overlap saccade, gap saccade, and anti-saccade tasks. Among the six eye-tracking paradigms, the classification model using parameters in the anti-saccade task had the highest accuracy (AUC: 0.911), while the next two better models were gap saccade (AUC: 0.904) and median fixation (AUC: 0.891). In addition, the smooth pursuit paradigm had the lowest accuracy (AUC: 0.842). Then, we combined the three paradigms with the highest AUCs to build a comprehensive model and found that the classification model using the anti-saccade, gap saccade, and median fixation tasks had the highest ability (AUC: 0.919), surpassing any single eye-tracking paradigm model to discriminate the SCD and non-SCD people, as shown in Figure 4B.

3.5 Performance of the multimodal for SCD detection

We attempted to compare the diagnostic value of the traditional scale with emerging behavioral assessment tools, such as gait analysis and eye tracking, in distinguishing SCD populations. Therefore, we compared four models: SCD9, gait single-task + dual-task, anti-saccade + gap saccade + median fixation, and gait single-task + dual-task + anti-saccade + gap saccade + median fixation. The results are shown in Figure 4C. Among the four classification models, the gait and eye tracking combination model was the most effective, with an AUC of 0.969, superior to single gait or eye tracking models. The SCD9 model had the lowest classification ability (AUC: 0.762).

3.6 Validation of gender difference and other machine learning models

As shown in Table 3, there was a significant difference in the gender factor between the two groups. To assess the impact of gender differences, we recalculated the diagnostic model by including gender as a factor, with the results displayed in Table 4. From these results, there was no significant change in the performance of model after adjusting for gender. Additionally, we compared four commonly used machine learning models, such as logistic regression, RF, SVM, and LightGBM, in addition to XGBoost to explore their detection abilities. The results indicated that XGBoost performed best, followed by LightGBM. Specific details are shown in Table 5.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Area under ROC curve and statistics of different models of two groups adjusted for gender.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Main results of different machine learning models.

4 Discussion

This study used eye-tracking technology for the initial screening of SCD and combined it with gait single-task and dual-task tests, expecting to explore a new and effective screening method for SCD. Nine gait parameters and thirteen eye-tracking parameters were found significantly different between non-SCD and SCD individuals. Using the machine learning classification models constructed in this study, we found that combining gait and eye tracking achieved the best result (AUC: 0.969) in distinguishing between non-SCD and SCD individuals. This outperformed the traditional SCD9 scale (AUC: 0.762) in terms of diagnostic accuracy. In previous studies, similar positive results have been observed in neurodegeneration diseases using these methods. One study investigated cognitive impairment using gait, speech, and drawing, achieving a combined model with an accuracy was 0.93 (Yamada et al., 2021). Additionally, another large-scale community study using dual-task gait and eye movement achieved an AUC of 0.987 for cognitive impairment detection (Lin et al., 2023). Both studies employed machining learning algorithms. Thus, we believe that this screening tool is valuable for identifying SCD in community-dwelling elderly population in the future.

Currently, research on gait analysis involves various devices to collect gait parameters and dual-task paradigms to identify cognitive decline. The equipment used in this study integrates video recording, parameter acquisition and analysis, with no need of additional sensors or video recording equipment, which makes it a more practical tool, suitable for the large-scale community screening. In this study, two types of dual-task tests were used, including counting backwards and naming animals, which have been found to be efficient in discriminating subjective cognitive impairment (Åhman et al., 2020). The ability of counting backwards test modal (AUC: 0.824) to detect SCD was not found to be significantly better than the single test gait (AUC: 0.823), suggesting its limitation in distinguishing SCD. The theory behind dual-task tests assumes that attentional resources are limited, and performance declines when two attention-demanding are performed simultaneously (Tombu and Jolicoeur, 2003). Therefore, it may be relatively simple and less cognitively demanding for the SCD population to conducted counting task (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017), consistent with previous findings (Tseng et al., 2014), similar to the task of recitation of the alphabet (Ghoraani et al., 2021). When analyzing data from the naming animals dual-task model, we found that it can enhance the ability of the single task and counting backwards dual task to discriminate SCD individuals with an AUC of 0.841. A cross-sectional study also found that the knee peak extension angle in naming animals dual-task test differed in distinguishing different cognitive groups (Ali et al., 2022). The animal-naming test assesses verbal fluency, requiring extensive knowledge and active retrieval processes. In a comparison study of three dual-task tests, researchers found that the tasks of subsequent 100-7 and naming animals had similar effects on gait performance in the MCI group (Du et al., 2023). For SCD subjects, the animal-naming task posed a higher cognitive demand than counting backward. Some gait characteristics were observed to be different in the two groups such as stride, step width and step time variation. This may be attributed to the shared neural pathways between gait and cognition, particularly in the prefrontal cortex, temporal regions, and entorhinal cortex (Sakurai et al., 2019), which are connected with executive function. Disruption caused by neurodegenerative processes may impact both functions (Grande et al., 2019). Future studies should further explore other valuable dual tasks for differentiating SCD.

Previous studies have shown that eye-tracking techniques are valuable in distinguishing between cognitively impaired and cognitively normal populations. Oyama et al. (2019) found that eye-tracking had good diagnostic performance in detecting patients with cognitive impairment (AUC: 0.888). In a study using pro-saccade and anti-saccade task, saccade behavior exhibited more errors, omissions, and fewer corrections in the MCI group compared to cognitively normal individuals, suggesting the probability of saccade tasks as cognitive markers of MCI (Chehrehnegar et al., 2022). Pro-saccade and anti-saccade involve activation of frontal and parietal lobes, lenticular nuclei, and occipital cortex which are responsible for planning and executing saccadic eye movement (Matsuda et al., 2004). Few of eye-tracking research studies have focused on the SCD population, especially in community survey. Xue et al. (2022) adopted a visual search task and found that gaze duration and times in the area of interest were increased in SCD patients, potentially enabling differentiation between SCD and NC. Taken together, this suggests the potential for recognizing SCD using eye-tracking technologies. In our study, we found significant differences of offsets and offset time in the median fixation task and offsets and total offsets in the lateral fixation task. One possible reason for the different results of the two studies is that we used different tasks and stimulators. To better understand the paradigms of eye tracking in SCD people, particularly at the community level, we utilized six tasks including smooth pursuit, median fixation, lateral fixation, overlap saccade, gap saccade, and anti-saccade tasks to advance our research. The anti-saccade (AUC: 0.911), gap saccade (AUC: 0.904), and median fixation (AUC: 0.891) paradigms had the strongest differentiation ability. Similar to our results, prior studies on cognitive impairment also proved the value of these three tasks (Wolf and Ueda, 2021; Lin et al., 2023; Wolf et al., 2023). We initially constructed a combination model of anti-saccade, gap saccade, median fixation and found better results (AUC: 0.919). The theory of detecting cognitive decline using eye-tracking parameters is still unknown. Researchers found that eye-tracking parameters have a correlation with neuropsychological scale scores. Total eye-tracking scores significantly decreased in CI population, correlating favorably with the scores on the MMSE (Tadokoro et al., 2021). In addition, eye tracking tests show an advantage over scales in that they have less impact on educational attainment. Also, fixation, saccade, and smooth pursuit, as demonstrated in prior studies, require the engagement of various cortical and subcortical regions including the frontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, reflecting the executive function and attention. Hence, eye-tracking assessments offer promise in cognitive function evaluation (Tao et al., 2020). To further explore whether combining gait and eye movement enhances SCD recognition and to investigate a model with superior classification, we fused data from the three eye tracking paradigms with the best classification performance with gait single-task and dual-task paradigms. This integration yielded the most successful model with an AUC of 0.969, suggesting the value of multimodal analysis. Remarkably, this study was the first to integrate eye-tracking tasks with a machine learning algorithm for community-based SCD screening. Future investigations should delve into paradigms which can offer enhanced classification efficacy and develop more intelligent evaluation models using eye-tracking technologies.

This study combined eye tracking and gait data, yielding improved outcomes. While the accuracy of these behavioral parameter models may not be equal to cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers and imaging like PET-CT, they surpass the traditional SCD9 scale in advantage of accuracy, objectivity and simplicity. These models are anticipated to emerge as validated tools for distinguishing SCD, MCI, and dementia in the future. In conclusion, our study demonstrates, for the first time, the effectiveness of combining eye tracking and dual-task gait analysis for SCD assessment, leading to promising results. This innovative approach shows significant potential for widespread clinical and community screening in the future.

Our study possesses several limitations. Firstly, the sample size was only 412 elderly individuals aged over 65 from three communities, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. Future research should aim to validate the conclusions in larger cohorts. Secondly, the average education level of approximately 9 years may not accurately represent individuals with lower education levels, such as those who are illiterate or have only completed primary education, thus restricting the generalizability of our findings to those with lower education. Future studies should validate these results across diverse populations with varying living conditions and cultural backgrounds. Lastly, our study adopted a cross-sectional design, which means we could not observe the progression of SCD and its association with MCI and dementia. Future endeavors should include longitudinal follow-up to understand how multimodal behavioral parameters evolve in SCD progression.

5 Conclusion

Overall, we observed that the naming animals dual task showed better discriminatory value in SCD detection compared to the counting backwards dual task, thereby enhancing the diagnostic capability of single-task gait analysis. Among the eye-tracking tasks, the anti-saccade, gap saccade, and median fixation paradigms exhibited the highest classification efficacy. Combining these three paradigms with gait analysis yielded an optimal classification model. These findings imply that the integration of eye-tracking and gait analysis has the potential to improve early SCD diagnosis, support the development of early intervention strategies and brain health management, and could be useful for widespread use in community screening programs.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the First Hospital of Shanxi Medical University, and all subjects signed an informed consent form.

Author contributions

CH: Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XZ: Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. JA: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. WB: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. FY: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. JC: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. SH: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. ZW: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. SD: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. YZ: Data curation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. QW: Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GM: Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. YL: Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study received assistance from the Natural Science Research Surface Project (project number: 202203021221255).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Åhman, H. B., Cedervall, Y., Kilander, L., Giedraitis, V., Berglund, L., McKee, K. J., et al. (2020). Dual-task tests discriminate between dementia, mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment, and healthy controls—a cross-sectional cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 20:258. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01645-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, N., Liu, J., Tian, H., Pan, W., Tang, Y., Zhong, Q., et al. (2022). A novel dual-task paradigm with story recall shows significant differences in the gait kinematics in older adults with cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:992873. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.992873

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahureksa, L., Najafi, B., Saleh, A., Sabbagh, M., Coon, D., Mohler, M. J., et al. (2017). The impact of mild cognitive impairment on gait and balance: a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies using instrumented assessment. Gerontology 63, 67–83. doi: 10.1159/000445831

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ble, A., Volpato, S., Zuliani, G., Guralnik, J. M., Bandinelli, S., Lauretani, F., et al. (2005). Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 410–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53157.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bovonsunthonchai, S., Vachalathiti, R., Hiengkaew, V., Bryant, M. S., Richards, J., and Senanarong, V. (2022). Quantitative gait analysis in mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and cognitively intact individuals: a cross-sectional case-control study. BMC Geriatr. 22:767. doi: 10.1186/s12877-022-03405-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Q., Tan, C., Xu, W., Hu, H., Cao, C., Qong, Q., et al. (2020). The prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimer’s Disease JAD 73, 1157–1166. doi: 10.3233/JAD-191092

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chehrehnegar, N., Shati, M., Esmaeili, M., and Foroughan, M. (2022). Executive function deficits in mild cognitive impairment: evidence from saccade tasks. Aging Ment. Health 26, 1001–1009. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1913471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, G.-R., Liu, D., Huang, L.-Y., Han, G.-B., Hu, F.-F., Wu, Z.-X., et al. (2023). Prevalence and risk factors for subjective cognitive decline and the correlation with objective cognition among community-dwelling older adults in China: results from the Hubei memory and aging cohort study. Alzheimers Dement. 19, 5074–5085. doi: 10.1002/alz.13047

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppin, A. K., Shumway-Cook, A., Saczynski, J. S., Patel, K. V., Ble, A., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2006). Association of executive function and performance of dual-task physical tests among older adults: analyses from the InChianti study. Age Ageing 35, 619–624. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, T. J., and Higham, S. (2016). Distinguishing between impairments of working memory and inhibitory control in cases of early dementia. Neuropsychologia 81, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, S., Ma, X., Wang, J., Mi, Y., Zhang, J., Du, C., et al. (2023). Spatiotemporal gait parameter fluctuations in older adults affected by mild cognitive impairment: comparisons among three cognitive dual-task tests. BMC Geriatr. 23:603. doi: 10.1186/s12877-023-04281-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghoraani, B., Boettcher, L. N., Hssayeni, M. D., Rosenfeld, A., Tolea, M. I., and Galvin, J. E. (2021). Detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease using dual-task gait assessments and machine learning. Biomed Signal Process Control 64:102249. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2020.102249

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, K. A., Liu, D., Romano, R., Jones, R. N., and Jefferson, A. L. (2015). Development of a subjective cognitive decline questionnaire using item response theory: a pilot study. Alzheimers Dement 1, 429–439. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grande, G., Triolo, F., Nuara, A., Welmer, A.-K., Fratiglioni, L., and Vetrano, D. L. (2019). Measuring gait speed to better identify prodromal dementia. Exp. Gerontol. 124:110625. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2019.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, L., Jia, J., Xing, Y., and Han, Y. (2022). An application study-subjective cognitive decline Questionnaire9 in detecting mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Aging Ment. Health 26, 2014–2021. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1980860

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, L., Wang, X., Zhang, L., Xing, Y., Guo, Q., Hu, X., et al. (2017). Prevalence, risk factors, and complaints screening tool exploration of subjective cognitive decline in a large cohort of the chinese population. J. Alzheimers Dis. 60, 371–388. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170347

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harada, C. N., Natelson Love, M. C., and Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 29, 737–752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Xue, X., and Lipton, R. B. (2006). Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: results from the Einstein aging study. Neuropsychology 20, 215–223. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., Buckley, R. F., van der Flier, W. M., Han, Y., Molinuevo, J. L., et al. (2020). The characterization of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 19, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30368-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, L., Du, Y., Chu, L., Zhang, Z., Li, F., Lyu, D., et al. (2020). Prevalence, risk factors, and management of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in adults aged 60 years or older in China: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Public Health 5, e661–e671. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30185-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalaria, R. N., Maestre, G. E., Arizaga, R., Friedland, R. P., Galasko, D., Hall, K., et al. (2008). Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia in developing countries: prevalence, management, and risk factors. Lancet. Neurol. 7, 812–826. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70169-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Tan, C., Tan, L., and Xu, X. (2023). Predictors of cognitive deterioration in subjective cognitive decline: evidence from longitudinal studies and implications for SCD-plus criteria. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 94, 844–854. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2022-330246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Shan, P.-Y., Jiang, W.-J., Sheng, C., and Ma, L. (2019). Subjective cognitive decline: preclinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol. Sci. Off. J. Ital. Neurol. Soc. Ital. Soc. Clin. Neurophysiol. 40, 41–49. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3620-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J., Xu, T., Yang, X., Yang, Q., Zhu, Y., Wan, M., et al. (2023). A detection model of cognitive impairment via the integrated gait and eye movement analysis from a large Chinese community cohort. Alzheimers Dement. 20, 1089–1101. doi: 10.1002/alz.13517

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luke, S. G., Darowski, E. S., and Gale, S. D. (2018). Predicting eye-movement characteristics across multiple tasks from working memory and executive control. Mem. Cogn. 46, 826–839. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0798-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahon, A., Clarke, A. D. F., and Hunt, A. R. (2018). The role of attention in eye-movement awareness. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 80, 1691–1704. doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1553-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, T., Matsuura, M., Ohkubo, T., Ohkubo, H., Matsushima, E., Inoue, K., et al. (2004). Functional MRI mapping of brain activation during visually guided saccades and antisaccades: cortical and subcortical networks. Psychiatry Res. 131, 147–155. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2003.12.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merchant, R. A., Goh, J., Chan, Y. H., Lim, J. Y., and Vellas, B. (2021). Slow gait, subjective cognitive decline and motoric cognitive risk syndrome: prevalence and associated factors in community dwelling older adults. J. Nutr. Health Aging 25, 48–56. doi: 10.1007/s12603-020-1525-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. J., Beaumont, H., Ferguson, D., Yadegarfar, M., and Stubbs, B. (2014). Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 130, 439–451. doi: 10.1111/acps.12336

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M. M., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Speechley, M., Borrie, M. J., Hachinski, V. C., Wells, J., et al. (2017). Association of dual-task gait with incident dementia in mild cognitive impairment: results from the gait and brain study. JAMA Neurol. 74:857. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.0643

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Opwonya, J., Doan, D. N. T., Kim, S. G., Kim, J. I., Ku, B., Kim, S., et al. (2022). Saccadic eye movement in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 32, 193–227. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09495-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oyama, A., Takeda, S., Ito, Y., Nakajima, T., Takami, Y., Takeya, Y., et al. (2019). Novel method for rapid assessment of cognitive impairment using high-performance eye-tracking technology. Sci. Rep. 9:12932. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49275-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavisic, I. M., Firth, N. C., Parsons, S., Rego, D. M., Shakespeare, T. J., Yong, K. X. X., et al. (2017). Eyetracking metrics in young onset alzheimer’s disease: a window into cognitive visual functions. Front. Neurol. 8:377. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, R., Bartha, R., and Montero-Odasso, M. (2019). Entorhinal cortex volume is associated with dual-task gait cost among older adults with mci: results from the gait and brain study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 698–704. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tadokoro, K., Yamashita, T., Fukui, Y., Nomura, E., Ohta, Y., Ueno, S., et al. (2021). Early detection of cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease with a novel eye tracking test. J. Neurol. Sci. 427:117529. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2021.117529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tao, L., Wang, Q., Liu, D., Wang, J., Zhu, Z., and Feng, L. (2020). Eye tracking metrics to screen and assess cognitive impairment in patients with neurological disorders. Neurol. Sci. 41, 1697–1704. doi: 10.1007/s10072-020-04310-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombu, M., and Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 29, 3–18. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.29.1.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, B. Y., Cullum, C. M., and Zhang, R. (2014). Older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment exhibit exacerbated gait slowing under dual-task challenges. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 11, 494–500. doi: 10.2174/1567205011666140505110828

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Harten, A. C., Mielke, M. M., Swenson-Dravis, D. M., Hagen, C. E., Edwards, K. K., Roberts, R. O., et al. (2018). Subjective cognitive decline and risk of MCI: the Mayo Clinic study of aging. Neurology 91, e300–e312. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005863

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, A., Tripanpitak, K., Umeda, S., and Otake-Matsuura, M. (2023). Eye-tracking paradigms for the assessment of mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1197567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1197567

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, A., and Ueda, K. (2021). Contribution of eye-tracking to study cognitive impairments among clinical populations. Front. Psychol. 12:590986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590986

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, C., Tang, Y., Wang, C., Yang, H., and Li, L. (2022). The effects of normal aging, subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, or Alzheimer’s disease on visual search. J. Alzheimers Dis. 88, 1639–1650. doi: 10.3233/JAD-220209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, Y., Shinkawa, K., Kobayashi, M., Caggiano, V., Nemoto, M., Nemoto, K., et al. (2021). Combining multimodal behavioral data of gait, speech, and drawing for classification of Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 84, 315–327. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210684

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. 23, 329–342. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Huang, X., An, R., Xiao, W., and Wan, Q. (2023). The application of saccades to assess cognitive impairment among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 2307–2321. doi: 10.1007/s40520-023-02546-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zola, S., Manzanares, C., Clopton, P., Lah, J., and Levey, A. (2013). A behavioral task predicts conversion to mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 28, 179–184. doi: 10.1177/1533317512470484

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective cognitive decline, machine learning, gait analysis, eye tracking, screening model, in the community

Citation: Hao C, Zhang X, An J, Bao W, Yang F, Chen J, Hou S, Wang Z, Du S, Zhao Y, Wang Q, Min G and Li Y (2024) An effective screening model for subjective cognitive decline in community-dwelling older adults based on gait analysis and eye tracking. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1444375. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1444375

Received: 05 June 2024; Accepted: 03 September 2024;
Published: 25 September 2024.

Edited by:

Sara Palermo, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Valeria Calsolaro, University of Pisa, Italy
Kosuke Fujita, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology (NCGG), Japan

Copyright © 2024 Hao, Zhang, An, Bao, Yang, Chen, Hou, Wang, Du, Zhao, Wang, Min and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Guowen Min, 13994261795@163.com; Yang Li, 15035182003@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.