Skip to main content

SPECIALTY GRAND CHALLENGE article

Front. Ethol., 02 November 2023
Sec. Foraging and Antipredator Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Editors' Showcase: Foraging and Antipredator Behavior View all 5 articles

Grand challenges in foraging behavior and predator-prey interactions: next generation ethology in the Anthropocene

  • Department of Biology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States


Everything is eventually eaten by something else; for most organisms, the crowning achievement of their existence is to reproduce before that happens, and (if they are lucky) perhaps have already died relatively peacefully before the eating commences. The interaction between species that are trying to consume and avoid consumption represents a fundamental force in evolution, as the outcome of that interaction shapes the genetic reproductive success of both parties. Ethologists broadly study these interactions under the umbrella of foraging and antipredator behaviors, depending on which party is the focus of the study; but natural selection makes no taxonomic distinctions, and so the processes of herbivory and parasitism would be additional examples of this consumptive species interaction that is shaped over evolutionary time. Nevertheless, for researchers who focus on the expression of behavioral phenotypes in animals, those behaviors related to finding food and avoiding becoming food often play a central role in developing an integrative understanding of an ecological community as a whole (Werner and Peacor, 2003; Nakazawa, 2017; Schmitz, 2017).

Because species interactions related to consumption structure natural communities, in many ways the challenges facing this field are a reflection of the broader challenges facing all of us who attempt to understand the evolution of behavior in natural systems. These challenges are numerous, and so by necessity I will narrow my focus to two in particular: (1) the need to understand the impact of unprecedented environmental changes induced by human development; and (2) the decline of descriptive and observational scientific research focused on organisms and the expression of their behavior in nature (i.e., the decline of natural history). Although these challenges are indeed grand, I believe the technological revolution currently underway in computational power and monitoring devices can go a long way in addressing both.

Species interactions and environmental change

Humans are having an impact on the environment that many consider to be on par with the handful of past geological events that led to mass extinction and a fundamental reordering of biodiversity across the globe (Waters et al., 2016). Because humans are releasing massive amounts of carbon dioxide into the atmosphere, which is also increasing the acidity of oceans, the resultant impact is truly global, affecting all species and ecosystems (Warren et al., 2011; Grimm et al., 2013; Spalding and Hull, 2021). Ethologists (and other types of evolutionary biologists) fully recognize that most species now exist within an environment that is undergoing rapid change (Guiden et al., 2019). Sih et al. (Sih et al., 2011) coined the term HIREC (human-induced rapid environmental changes) to characterize this problem and emphasize its prominence in animal behavior research. No biological discipline can afford to ignore HIREC, but ethology is on the front line of this battle. Perhaps the most fundamental question regarding our ability to predict or protect the future of a species is behavioral: is its behavior flexible enough to cope with change? The plasticity of behavior is what will allow a species to persist (or even thrive) in the face HIREC (Wong and Candolin, 2015; Beever et al., 2017).

Much of the research on behavioral plasticity and HIREC focuses on individual species responding directly to an impact, but ethologists focusing on foraging and antipredator behaviors must also consider the problem from a community perspective: environmental changes that directly impact one species also pull on the linkages that species has to others in the community, and therefore will have a series of rippling effects that move through the whole ecosystem (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). Ecologists cannot afford to ignore the behavioral details that mediate those responses, as the extent and type of behavioral variability will determine how natural populations respond (Creel et al., 2019). Studies of behaviorally-mediated trophic cascade at the landscape scale often rely on broad patterns of species distributions and demographics, without adequate characterization how processes and patterns are linked (Peacor et al., 2022); others have also called for the increased use of emerging monitoring technologies to better understand risk-sensitive behaviors (Prugh et al., 2019).

One of the most devastating forms of HIREC related to the ethology of predation is the introduction of species into novel habitats (Bellard et al., 2016). Invasive predators can disrupt entire ecosystems when native species lack appropriate antipredator behaviors (Cox and Lima, 2006; Sih et al., 2010). Prominent examples include the invasion of Guam by brown treesnakes (Anton et al., 2020), the spread of lionfish in the Caribbean (Anton et al., 2020), and the damage done by rats introduced to oceanic islands across the globe (Harper and Bunbury, 2015). Predicting the impact of invasive predators, or the timescale at which impacted species may adjust to the invasion, requires a detailed understanding of the expression and development of antipredator behaviors (Carthey and Blumstein, 2018), work that has a long tradition within the field of ethology (Tinbergen et al., 1967; Curio, 1976).

On the other side of the predator-prey relationship, the ethology of foraging or hunting is similarly prominent in the biodiversity crisis. A recent meta-analysis of the proximate causes of species declines and extinctions in response to climate change found that altered species interactions associated with decreased ability to find food was the single most frequently cited mechanism underlying species declines (Cahill et al., 2013). Plasticity in foraging or feeding behaviors may be crucial for adapting to changing biotic and abiotic conditions (Tuomainen and Candolin, 2011). Recent prominent examples include the unexpected flexibility in forage use that allowed a population of pikas (Ochotomys princeps) to persist well outside their typical climatic niche (Varner and Dearing, 2014), the shift in foraging mode that permitted peacock groupers (Cephalopholis argus) to persist in the face of habitat degradation, and a switch to foraging in near-shore benthic habitats by black guillemots (Cepphus grylle mandtii) coping with decreased sea ice (Divoky et al., 2021).

The decline of ethological research in nature

A second crisis of a different nature has the potential to greatly exacerbate the first. Research focused on organisms in nature is in decline. For decades, natural historians have been sounding the alarm regarding the increasing difficulty in funding basic descriptive research (Wilcove and Eisner, 2000; Greene, 2005; Tewksbury et al., 2014). The rise of molecular and genetic tools in biology has revolutionized our understanding of biological systems, but has also resulted in a reductionism that increasingly prioritizes the testing of hypotheses far above the accumulation of quantitative observations (Farris, 2020; Yanai and Lercher, 2020). Natural history is a somewhat loosely defined field, but most practitioners consider it to be primarily descriptive; natural historians try to accumulate detailed and quantitative data on natural systems, often while trying to minimize pre-conceived notions about how those systems should work (Herman, 2002; Schmidly, 2005; Barrows et al., 2016). Although such observations are a fundamental component of the scientific process, they are too frequently not treated as such by reviewers and editors of scientific papers. Perhaps one of the most common general types of feedback given in the peer review process is to adopt a hypothesis-testing framework, even when the work in question may be explicitly descriptive, as if the data, discussions, and conclusions would not be meaningful if they were uncovered as part of the process of observing and quantifying natural systems without a preconceived hypothesis in mind.

Of course, hypothesis testing and laboratory studies will always occupy a central place in scientific research, as testing hypotheses is universally regarded as the crux of the scientific method. But experimental work cannot substitute for the descriptive studies, opportunistic observations, and detailed accounts of natural history that lay the foundation for testable questions (Tewksbury et al., 2014; Betts et al., 2021). Both descriptive and experimental studies are necessary and complementary components of discovery. Observations of organisms in their natural environment provide the raw material that can be refined into hypotheses and tested. Not investing in such research is the equivalent of a mining company ignoring the need to find new deposits and just focusing on refining what they have already discovered—an obviously unsustainable plan.

Next generation natural history

Like most major challenges in science, these issues cannot be solved by any single approach, but instead must be addressed by bringing to bear a series of complementary, interacting solutions. For one prominent part of the solution, I would point to the emergence of a number of new technologies that have many field biologists fundamentally rethinking the nature of natural history research (Krishtalka and Humphrey, 2000; Peay, 2014; Bakker et al., 2020; Tosa et al., 2021). Experienced natural historians have always used whatever useful tools may be at hand to aid in the process of observing details of nature (binoculars, cameras, notepads, thermometers, etc.). In recent years, the list of tools and their complexity has grown, and practitioners of ethology now have a powerful array of advanced technological devices that can help them record details of an organism and its environment in ways that previous generations could only dream of (Couzin and Heins, 2023). Tosa et al. (Tosa et al., 2021) refer to this as “Next Generation Natural History” (NGNH) and provide a detailed summary of how such approaches are revolutionizing our understanding of organisms and nature.

Many NGNH approaches are directly applicable to the quantification of behavior, and are likely to be on the front lines of future efforts to understand the details of foraging and antipredator behaviors (Gomez-Marin et al., 2014). Perhaps one of the most promising is the increasing use of animal-borne biologgers to quantify the moment-to-moment details of a behavior, even when an animal is not under direct observation (Brown et al., 2013). Accelerometery loggers as small as 2 grams can store hundreds of values per second, every second, for days at a time. If a large database of validation observations can be accumulated, machine learning models can often determine, with high levels of accuracy, which behavior corresponds to a given acceleration pattern, thereby allowing researchers to accumulate a detailed record of naturalistic behaviors across a variety of individuals and situations (Wang, 2019). This technique could provide unparalleled insight into predatory behaviors, since one of the major limitations in quantifying such behaviors in free ranging animals is that, for many predators, these interactions occur rarely and unpredictably (Viviant et al., 2009; Hanscom et al., 2023). Accelerometry is similarly useful for quantifying rates and types of antipredator behaviors in free ranging animals (Zenone et al., 2020), an approach that will likely be able to inform more dynamic and realistic “landscape of fear” models for balancing risk and reward in natural environments (Palmer et al., 2022). Accelerometry represents just one part of a biologging revolution; sensor devices can include gyroscopes, thermometers, heart or breath rate loggers, depth loggers, and different options for location sensing, memory, and data retrieval (Jeantet et al., 2020; Williams et al., 2020; Papastamatiou et al., 2022; Wild et al., 2023).

Another NGNH approach that is likely to transform ethology is the expanding use of image-based tracking (Dell et al., 2014; Weinstein, 2018). A number of software programs are available that allow researchers to use machine learning programs to automatically identify and track the detailed movements of hundreds or thousands of individual animals or their component parts from digitized video recordings of behavior (Crall et al., 2015; Mathis et al., 2018; Graving et al., 2019; Walter and Couzin, 2021). These approaches have been employed to identify individual animals within large social groups (Ferreira et al., 2020) or categorize species across millions of images (Steenweg et al., 2017; Ahumada et al., 2020). As with many NGNH techniques, these tools do not necessarily do something that was impossible before, but they make it possible to bring massive datasets to bear on questions that previously required weeks or years of effort (Kellenberger et al., 2021).

These examples are not meant to be comprehensive, just illustrative. Additional advances in animal monitoring, tracking, and observation are detailed in a number of other review papers (Peters et al., 2014; Valletta et al., 2017; Farley et al., 2018; Lahoz-Monfort and Magrath, 2021; Tosa et al., 2021; Tuia et al., 2022; Couzin and Heins, 2023). The power of big data accumulated via NGNH represents not just a revitalization of the field of natural history, but also rebalances the emphasis in scientific research between quantitative descriptive research and hypothesis testing. Many NGNH studies are explicitly descriptive, representing not the test of preconceived hypotheses based on past observation, but instead the detailed documentation of how animals interact with their environment. Such studies are vital, as they provide inspiration and fodder for the formulation and testing of key hypotheses. NGNH also represents a primary tool in the arsenal of researchers conducting HIREC studies. Do you suspect that increasing temperatures may be facilitating the spread of an invasive predator and driving the extinction of numerous native species (Hellman et al., 2008)? Accumulating large datasets via animal-borne biologging on how individuals of those species interact (i.e., how their feeding and antipredator behaviors play out under different thermal regimes), could be a key tool for determining if temperature is associated with movement or interaction rates, laying the groundwork for focused experimental tests.

Although it is undoubtedly the case that future research on the ethology of predator-prey interactions will continue to be difficult to fund, and that the very subjects we seek to study will become even more imperiled, our research community can embrace the powerful techniques stemming from the explosion of computational power and our increasing ability to manufacture smaller and more affordable monitoring devices. We should use these developments to inspire and train young students with a love of the natural world and a desire to preserve it. In this way, ethological research can provide some of the most salient solutions to the biodiversity crisis.

Author contributions

RWC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) RWC declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahumada J. A., Fegraus E., Birch T., Flores N., Kays R., O’Brien T. G., et al. (2020). Wildlife insights: a platform to maximize the potential of camera trap and other passive sensor wildlife data for the planet. Environ. Conserv. 47, 1–6. doi: 10.1017/s0376892919000298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anton A., Cure K., Layman C. A., Puntila R., Simpson M. S., Bruno J. F. (2020). Prey naiveté to invasive lionfish Pterois volitans on Caribbean coral reefs. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 544, 257–269. doi: 10.3354/meps11553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakker F. T., Antonelli A., Clarke J. A., Cook J. A., Edwards S. V., Ericson P. G. P., et al. (2020). The Global Museum: natural history collections and the future of evolutionary science and public education. PeerJ 8, e8225. doi: 10.7717/peerj.8225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrows C. W., Murphy-Mariscal M. L., Hernandez R. R. (2016). At a crossroads: the nature of natural history in the twenty-first century. BioScience 66, 592–599. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biw043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beever E. A., Hall L. E., Varner J., Loosen A. E., Dunham J. B., Gahl M. K., et al. (2017). Behavioral flexibility as a mechanism for coping with climate change. Front. Ecol. Environ. 15, 299–308. doi: 10.1002/fee.1502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellard C., Cassey P., Blackburn T. M. (2016). Alien species as a driver of recent extinctions. Biol. Lett. 12, 20150623. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betts M. G., Hadley A. S., Frey D. W., Frey S. J. K., Gannon D., Harris S. H., et al. (2021). When are hypotheses useful in ecology and evolution? Ecol. Evol. 11, 5762–5776. doi: 10.1002/ece3.7365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown D. D., Kays R., Wikelski M., Wilson R., Klimley A. P. (2013). Observing the unwatchable through acceleration logging of animal behavior. Anim. Biotelemetry 1, 20. doi: 10.1186/2050-3385-1-20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill A. E., Aiello-Lammens M. E., Fisher-Reid M. C., Hua X., Karanewsky C. J., Ryu H. Y., et al. (2013). How does climate change cause extinction? Proc. R. Soc B: Biol. Sci. 280, 20121890. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carthey A. J. R., Blumstein D. T. (2018). Predicting predator recognition in a changing world. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 106–115. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2017.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couzin I. D., Heins C. (2023). Emerging technologies for behavioral research in changing environments. Trends Ecol. Evol. 38, 346–354. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2022.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox J. G., Lima S. L. (2006). Naiveté and an aquatic–terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of introduced predators. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 674–680. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crall J. D., Gravish N., Mountcastle A. M., Combes S. A. (2015). BEEtag: a low-cost, image-based tracking system for the study of animal behavior and locomotion. PloS One 10, e0136487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creel S., Becker M., Dröge E., M’soka J., Matandiko W., Rosenblatt E., et al. (2019). What explains variation in the strength of behavioral responses to predation risk? A standardized test with large carnivore and ungulate guilds in three ecosystems. Biol. Conserv. 232, 164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2019.02.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curio E. (1976). The ethology of predation (Heidelberg: Springer Verlag). doi: 10.1016/j.gecco.2015.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell A. I., Bender J. A., Branson K., Couzin I. D., de Polavieja G. G., Noldus L. P. J. J., et al. (2014). Automated image-based tracking and its application in ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 417–428. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Divoky G. J., Brown E., Elliott K. H. (2021). Reduced seasonal sea ice and increased sea surface temperature change prey and foraging behaviour in an ice-obligate Arctic seabird, Mandt’s black guillemot (Cepphus grylle mandtii). Polar Biol. 44, 701–715. doi: 10.1007/s00300-021-02826-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farley S. S., Dawson A., Goring S. J., Williams J. W. (2018). Situating ecology as a big-data science: current advances, challenges, and solutions. BioScience 68, 563–576. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biy068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farris S. M. (2020). The rise to dominance of genetic model organisms and the decline of curiosity-driven organismal research. PloS One 15, e0243088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira A. C., Silva L. R., Renna F., Brandl H. B., Renoult J. P., Farine D. R., et al. (2020). Deep learning-based methods for individual recognition in small birds. Methods Ecol. Evol. 11, 1072–1085. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.13436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Marin A., Paton J. J., Kampff A. R., Costa R. M., Mainen Z. F. (2014). Big behavioral data: psychology, ethology and the foundations of neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1455–1462. doi: 10.1038/nn.3812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graving J. M., Chae D., Naik H., Li L., Koger B., Costelloe B. R., et al. (2019). DeepPoseKit, a software toolkit for fast and robust animal pose estimation using deep learning. eLife 8, e47994. doi: 10.7554/elife.47994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene H. W. (2005). Organisms in nature as a central focus for biology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 23–27. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimm N. B., Chapin F. S., Bierwagen B., Gonzalez P., Groffman P. M., Luo Y., et al. (2013). The impacts of climate change on ecosystem structure and function. Front. Ecol. Environ. 11, 474–482. doi: 10.1890/120282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiden P. W., Bartel S. L., Byer N. W., Shipley A. A., Orrock J. L. (2019). Predator–prey interactions in the anthropocene: reconciling multiple aspects of novelty. Trends Ecol. Evol. 34, 616–627. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanscom R. J., DeSantis D. L., Hill J. L., Marbach T., Sukumaran J., Tipton A. F., et al. (2023). How to study a predator that only eats a few meals a year: high-frequency accelerometry to quantify feeding behaviours of rattlesnakes (Crotalus spp.). Anim. Biotelemetry 11, 20. doi: 10.1186/s40317-023-00332-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper G. A., Bunbury N. (2015). Invasive rats on tropical islands: Their population biology and impacts on native species. Glob. Ecol. Conserv. 3, 607–627. doi: 10.1016/j.gecco.2015.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellman J. J., Byers J. E., Bierwagen B. G., Dukes J. S. (2008). Five potential consequences of climate change for invasive species. Conserv. Biol. 22, 534–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2008.00951.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman S. G. (2002). Wildlife biology and natural history: time for a reunion. J. Wildl. Manage. 66, 933. doi: 10.2307/3802927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeantet L., Planas-Bielsa V., Benhamou S., Geiger S., Martin J., Siegwalt F., et al. (2020). Behavioural inference from signal processing using animal-borne multi-sensor loggers: a novel solution to extend the knowledge of sea turtle ecology. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 200139. doi: 10.1098/rsos.200139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellenberger B., Veen T., Folmer E., Tuia D. (2021). 21000 birds in 4.5 h: efficient large-scale seabird detection with machine learning. Remote Sens. Ecol. Conserv. 7, 445–460. doi: 10.1002/rse2.200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishtalka L., Humphrey P. S. (2000). Can natural history museums capture the future? BioScience 50, 611–617. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2000)050[0611:cnhmct]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahoz-Monfort J. J., Magrath M. J. L. (2021). A comprehensive overview of technologies for species and habitat monitoring and conservation. BioScience 71, 1038–1062. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biab073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathis A., Mamidanna P., Cury K. M., Abe T., Murthy V. N., Mathis M. W., et al. (2018). DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagelkerken I., Munday P. L. (2016). Animal behaviour shapes the ecological effects of ocean acidification and warming: moving from individual to community-level responses. Global Change Biol. 22, 974–989. doi: 10.1111/gcb.13167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakazawa T. (2017). Individual interaction data are required in community ecology: a conceptual review of the predator–prey mass ratio and more. Ecol. Res. 32, 5–12. doi: 10.1007/s11284-016-1408-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer M. S., Gaynor K. M., Becker J. A., Abraham J. O., Mumma M. A., Pringle R. M. (2022). Dynamic landscapes of fear: understanding spatiotemporal risk. Trends Ecol. Evol. 37, 911–925. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2022.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papastamatiou Y. P., Mourier J., TinHan T., Luongo S., Hosoki S., Santana-Morales O., et al. (2022). Social dynamics and individual hunting tactics of white sharks revealed by biologging. Biol. Lett. 18, 20210599. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2021.0599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peacor S. D., Dorn N. J., Smith J. A., Peckham N. E., Cherry M. J., Sheriff M. J., et al. (2022). A skewed literature: few studies evaluate the contribution of predation-risk effects to natural field patterns. Ecol. Lett. 25, 2048–2061. doi: 10.1111/ele.14075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peay K. G. (2014). Back to the future: natural history and the way forward in modern fungal ecology. Fungal Ecol. 12, 4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.funeco.2014.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters D. P. C., Havstad K. M., Cushing J., Tweedie C., Fuentes O., Villanueva-Rosales N. (2014). Harnessing the power of big data: infusing the scientific method with machine learning to transform ecology. Ecosphere 5, 1–15. doi: 10.1890/es13-00359.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prugh L. R., Sivy K. J., Mahoney P. J., Ganz T. R., Ditmer M. A., van de Kerk M., et al. (2019). Designing studies of predation risk for improved inference in carnivore-ungulate systems. Biol. Conserv. 232, 194–207. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2019.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidly D. J. (2005). What it means to be a naturalist and the future of natural history at American universities. J. Mammal. 95, 449–456. doi: 10.1644/1545-1542(2005)86[449:wimtba]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz O. (2017). Predator and prey functional traits: understanding the adaptive machinery driving predator–prey interactions. F1000Research 6, 1767. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.11813.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih A., Bolnick D. I., Luttbeg B., Orrock J. L., Peacor S. D., Pintor L. M., et al. (2010). Predator–prey naïveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos 119, 610–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18039.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih A., Ferrari M. C. O., Harris D. J. (2011). Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evolution. Appl. 4, 367–387. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-4571.2010.00166.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spalding C., Hull P. M. (2021). Towards quantifying the mass extinction debt of the Anthropocene. Proc. R. Soc B 288, 20202332. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.2332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenweg R., Hebblewhite M., Kays R., Ahumada J., Fisher J. T., Burton C., et al. (2017). Scaling-up camera traps: monitoring the planet’s biodiversity with networks of remote sensors. Front. Ecol. Environ. 15, 26–34. doi: 10.1002/fee.1448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tewksbury J. J., Anderson J. G. T., Bakker J. D., Billo T. J., Dunwiddie P. W., Groom M. J., et al. (2014). Natural history’s place in science and society. BioScience 64, 300–310. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biu032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinbergen N., Impekoven M., Franck D. (1967). An experiment on spacing-out as a defence against predation. Behaviour 28, 307–320. doi: 10.1163/156853967x00064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tosa M. I., Dziedzic E. H., Appel C. L., Urbina J., Massey A., Ruprecht J., et al. (2021). The rapid rise of next-generation natural history. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.698131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuia D., Kellenberger B., Beery S., Costelloe B. R., Zuffi S., Risse B., et al. (2022). Perspectives in machine learning for wildlife conservation. Nat. Commun. 13, 792. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-27980-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuomainen U., Candolin U. (2011). Behavioural responses to human-induced environmental change. Biol. Rev. 86, 640–657. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.2010.00164.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valletta J. J., Torney C., Kings M., Thornton A., Madden J. (2017). Applications of machine learning in animal behaviour studies. Anim. Behav. 124, 203–220. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varner J., Dearing M. D. (2014). Dietary plasticity in pikas as a strategy for atypical resource landscapes. J. Mammal. 95, 72–81. doi: 10.1644/13-mamm-a-099.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviant M., Trites A. W., Rosen D. A. S., Monestiez P., Guinet C. (2009). Prey capture attempts can be detected in Steller sea lions and other marine predators using accelerometers. Polar Biol. 33, 713–719. doi: 10.1007/s00300-009-0750-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter T., Couzin I. D. (2021). TRex, a fast multi-animal tracking system with markerless identification, and 2D estimation of posture and visual fields. eLife 10, e64000. doi: 10.7554/elife.64000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang G. (2019). Machine learning for inferring animal behavior from location and movement data. Ecol. Inform. 49, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoinf.2018.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren R., Price J., Fischlin A., Santos S., de la N., Midgley G. (2011). Increasing impacts of climate change upon ecosystems with increasing global mean temperature rise. Clim. Change 106, 141–177. doi: 10.1007/s10584-010-9923-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters C. N., Zalasiewicz J., Summerhayes C., Barnosky A. D., Poirier C., Gałuszka A., et al. (2016). The Anthropocene is functionally and stratigraphically distinct from the Holocene. Science 351, aad2622. doi: 10.1126/science.aad2622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein B. G. (2018). A computer vision for animal ecology. J. Anim. Ecol. 87, 533–545. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner E. E., Peacor S. D. (2003). A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84, 1083–1100. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1083:arotii]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcove D. S., Eisner T. (2000). The impending extinction of natural history. Chronicle Higher Educ.

Google Scholar

Wild T. A., Wikelski M., Tyndel S., Alarcón-Nieto G., Klump B. C., Aplin L. M., et al. (2023). Internet on animals: Wi-Fi-enabled devices provide a solution for big data transmission in biologging. Methods Ecol. Evol. 14, 87–102. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.13798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams H. J., Taylor L. A., Benhamou S., Bijleveld A. I., Clay T. A., Grissac S., et al. (2020). Optimizing the use of biologgers for movement ecology research. J. Anim. Ecol. 89, 186–206. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.13094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong B. B. M., Candolin U. (2015). Behavioral responses to changing environments. Behav. Ecol. 26, 665–673. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanai I., Lercher M. (2020). A hypothesis is a liability. Genome Biol. 21, 231. doi: 10.1186/s13059-020-02133-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zenone A., Ciancio J. E., Badalamenti F., Buffa G., D’Anna G., Pipitone C., et al. (2020). Influence of light, food and predator presence on the activity pattern of the European spiny lobster Palinurus elephas: an investigation using tri-axial accelerometers. Ecol. Indic. 113, 106174. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2020.106174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: climate change, HiREC, natural history, hypothesis testing, predation, feeding ecology, biologging

Citation: Clark RW (2023) Grand challenges in foraging behavior and predator-prey interactions: next generation ethology in the Anthropocene. Front. Ethol. 2:1304654. doi: 10.3389/fetho.2023.1304654

Received: 29 September 2023; Accepted: 23 October 2023;
Published: 02 November 2023.

Edited and Reviewed by:

Gordon M. Burghardt, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States

Copyright © 2023 Clark. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rulon W. Clark, cmNsYXJrQHNkc3UuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.