Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 09 January 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology

What makes an excellent reader? Short-term memory contrasts between two groups of children

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 2Institute of Education and Psychology at Szombathely, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 3Faculty of Psychotherapy Science, Sigmund Freud Private University, Vienna, Austria
  • 4Doctoral School of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 5Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

Purpose: Research on the association of short-term memory (STM) and reading expertise are dominated by studies with typically developing children and children with reading impairment. Many studies confirmed the role of short-term memory in reading development and reading, especially in the case of verbal and phonological STM. The current study takes an unusual perspective by contrasting age-appropriate readers with excellent readers (reading performance with at least 1 SD above average) on three different short-term memory skills: phonological STM, verbal STM and visuospatial STM.

Methods: We identified and recruited six groups of children. Three groups performed at least one SD above average in two standardized reading tasks (excellent readers), the three control groups performed within the domain of ±0.5 SD on reading (age-appropriate readers). One group of excellent readers and one group of age-appropriate readers participated in a Phonological Short-Term Memory (STM) task, one pair of groups participated in a Verbal STM task, whereas the last pair participated in a Visuospatial STM task.

Results: Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that excellent readers outperformed age-appropriate readers in Visuospatial STM. Phonological STM only differed across the groups after controlling for age. No group difference was observed in Verbal STM.

Conclusion: Our results confirm the role of short-term memory in reading expertise. However, data highlights that visuospatial and phonological information becomes more relevant in above-average readers. Results are discussed along grain-size theory, and whether and how focused educational programs can build on visuospatial short-term memory training to achieve better reading.

1 What makes an excellent reader? Short-term memory contrasts between two groups of children

Reading is among the most important academic skills. Especially Western societies rely heavily on written information: all agreements, all contracts must be documented in a written format, so literacy is inevitable to navigate within the society. In accordance, one of the major focuses of primary education is to support children in literacy development. Primary education is not only an opportunity, but most countries oblige their children to primary education. Since reading is such an important skill, it is necessary to understand how different cognitive factors support reading development, and how these underlying cognitive factors may be used to further enhance reading development. The current study is aimed to understand how different types of short-term memory underlie reading skills. While many previous studies have focused on children with deficient reading skills, our aim is to unravel how exceptionally good readers differ from their peers with age-appropriate skills. In accordance, the following study compares the short-term memory performance of excellent readers (more than 1 SD above average) and typical readers on three different short-term memory (STM) paradigms: a verbal STM task, a phonological STM task, and a visuospatial STM task.

The introduction below summarizes previous studies addressing the association between reading and working memory from various perspectives. On the one hand, research was aimed at identifying whether and how individual differences in reading are explained by underlying memory skills. On the other hand, research addressed how children with impaired reading skills differ from typical readers in working memory abilities. Reading is a complex skill during which the individual decodes visual stimuli into phonological information. Thus reading can in principle be associated with both visual and verbal short term memory functions. This is especially true due to the changing nature of reading throughout development.

In the earliest, pre-reading phase, children identify and recognize certain words, like their own names. These written words are not analyzed though, rather stored as complex visual images (Frith, 1985; Genisio and Bastien-Toniazzo, 2003). Next, children have to identify that letters and speech sounds correspond (Seymour and Elder, 1986; Morton, 1989; Froyen et al., 2009). This is the alphabetic principle, which is a prerequisite of reading (Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1989; Liberman et al., 1989; Landerl et al., 2018). By the end of kindergarten years, children learn more and more letters, but mainly associate them with letter names (Thompson, 2009). Only during the early stages of primary education do children start to decode letters and combine the underlying phonological representations (Seymour and Elder, 1986; Ehri, 1991, 1997; Georgiou et al., 2020). Structured input helps them systematically identify the letter-speech sound correspondences, fine-tune their sensitivity to visual symbols appearing in their own languages (Brem et al., 2010; Froyen et al., 2010). At this point, children mainly decode the information sequentially. With practice, children develop and store orthographic representations of larger sublexical (Roembke et al., 2019) and lexical units (Goswami et al., 1998), which opens way to sight-word reading, which is the most optimal way of reading (Ehri, 2005, 2014). There could be certain differences due to educational policies: for example, school starts in England a year earlier than in Austria (Steiner et al., 2021).

Since reading is decoding visual symbols into phonological content, phonological abilities are among the best predictors of reading development (Castles et al., 2003; Farrar and Ashwell, 2008; Landerl et al., 2018), and they also serve as one of the best clinical markers of developmental dyslexia (Alexander et al., 1991; Ramus and Szenkovits, 2008). During the process of reading development, reading becomes a fairly automated skill (Froyen et al., 2009; Roembke et al., 2019), which leads to a decreasing role of phonological awareness. Children divert from sequential decoding and one-to-one mapping of letters and speech sounds, and rely more on a more holistic way of reading, that is word recognition (Ehri, 2014). Such an advanced process rather loads on access to orthographic representations and their associated phonological forms than manipulation of speech sounds (Moll and Landerl, 2009). Empirical studies indeed demonstrated that while the resolution of phonological awareness increases with age (Goswami, 1999, 2002), its explanatory power decreases with reading expertise (Hogan et al., 2005; Powell and Atkinson, 2021).

While children are expected to divert from the deployment of phonological knowledge with increasing reading experience, the role of verbal skills is not that obvious. While verbal short-term memory has also been identified as a clinical marker of dyslexia (Mann and Liberman, 1984; Brady, 1986), a causal relationship between reading and verbal short-term memory has not been fully supported (Melby-Lervåg, 2012; Melby-Lervåg and Hulme, 2013). That is, although children with dyslexia seem to have a below average verbal short-term memory capacity, individual differences in verbal STM do not seem to explain individual differences in reading within the typically developing domain. On the other hand, there can be an indirect effect, as verbal STM explains variance of the development of vocabulary (Gupta and Mac Whinney, 1997; Jarrold et al., 2004; Verhagen and Leseman, 2016), which in turn could translate into lexical quality, leading to better reading abilities (Perfetti, 2007).

The section above discussed why phonological and verbal skills may contribute to the development of reading. Not only spoken skills are relevant to reading, but also the processing of visual information. Visual input is neither invariant, nor noise-free. Letters, letter-clusters, words and scripts in general vary in numerous characteristics, like font, size, color or contrast. Despite this variance, orthographic units are still identified, at least when fonts are not dysfluent (Astley et al., 2023). The abstract letter units are assumed to be the smallest units of orthographic processing (Finkbeiner and Coltheart, 2009). These are common categories of letters with the same identity, regardless of their real physical appearance (Thompson, 2009; Carreiras et al., 2013). The existence of these abstract units demonstrate that certain visual features can be or are discounted when processing orthographic input.

Not only visual, but also spatial features can be disregarded during the process of reading. Individuals are able to activate the underlying orthographic representations even when certain letters are substituted with each other or with other letters. These are mostly known as transposed (Carreiras et al., 2007; Perea and Carreiras, 2008; Luke and Christianson, 2012) and substituted letter effect (Lété and Fayol, 2013; Varga et al., 2021; Hasenäcker and Schroeder, 2022). Research on these phenomena demonstrate that noise in letter identity as well as letter position can be disregarded during reading (flexibility was smaller in individuals with developmental dyslexia, e.g., Lété and Fayol, 2013; Kirkby et al., 2022). Since letter transposition is easier to overcome than letter substitution, it was proposed that certain sublexical orthographic units are stored and/or processed without an internal spatial structure. Open bigrams are combinations of two letters irrelevant of their order. The lack of spatial specification in these open bigrams, however, supports quick recognition and overcoming spelling errors, like the above-mentioned letter transposition (Grainger and Whitney, 2004; Lupker et al., 2015).

In sum, the above review suggests that during typical reading development, children should rely less and less on their phonological and visual processing abilities, while it is not clear whether reading experience qualifies the association verbal short-term memory and reading. Less is known about atypical readers, and even this knowledge is rooted in studies with developmental dyslexia, documenting decreased performance in all three domains, that is, phonological (Tiffin-Richards et al., 2008; Franceschini and Bertoni, 2019), verbal (Trecy et al., 2013; Majerus and Cowan, 2016), and visuospatial short-term memory (Smith-Spark et al., 2003; Bacon et al., 2013).

In the current study we take an unconventional perspective in examining how the contribution of phonological, verbal and visual short-term memory changes with reading expertise. We recruited excellent readers, that is, children who are at least 1 standard deviation above the age-appropriate level of reading, and compared their performance to children with age-appropriate reading skills (within the domain of ±0.5 SD). Excellent readers and typical readers were contrasted on their phonological, verbal and visuospatial short-term memory performance. It is important to note that Baddeley’s working memory model does not differentiate between verbal and phonological short-term memory, since both load on the phonological loop. However, the two skills differ in their characteristics. Verbal STM is the memorization of known words based on their meaning, while Phonological STM requires an accurate coding of phonological input in the absence of semantic scaffolding, thus relying more on complex phonological processes (e.g., Dillon and Pisoni, 2006). We hypothesized two possibilities. On the one hand, if reading development requires relying less and less on phonological and visuospatial processing, we would expect no difference in visuospatial and phonological short-term memory between expert and age-appropriate readers. Whereas if results from developmental dyslexia are applicable on the other side of the spectrum, we should expect better performance of short-term memory in excellent readers in all three domains.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

The current study is a part of the standardization of the Hungarian VOLT word and pseudoword reading test (Kemény et al., 2023). Within the standardization process, children from grade 1 s semester to grade 6 s semester were recruited. The aim was to collect age-norms across these 11 times points. The standardization process included data from 1952 children from various schools from western Hungary. Along with the reading tests, children did one of three memory tasks as well: the nonword repetition task measuring Phonological Short-Term Memory, the digit recall task measuring Verbal Short Term Memory, or the computerized version of the Corsi blocks task, measuring Visuospatial Short-Term Memory. Each child completed only one of the memory tasks. Out of the 1952 children, we filtered and report data of those children (1) who met the criterion of typical readers’ or excellent readers’, and (2) who had a completed memory task. Furthermore, each of the memory tasks had practice items. We only included participants who succeeded on the practice items. Altogether 319 children were included in the data analysis, one child had data from all three tasks, whereas all others completed only one of the memory paradigms. Since the different memory-tasks cannot be compared with each other, we present the results separately across the memory tasks.

Children were either typical readers or excellent readers. We defined the two groups based on reading performance. Typical readers are readers who perform between −0.5 and 0.5 standard deviations on both the word and pseudoword reading task of the VOLT (Kemény et al., 2023). Excellent readers perform above 1 SD on both subtasks. Descriptive data of participants are provided in Table 1. All children were Hungarian speakers. They were recruited and tested in their own primary schools. All schools were located in Western Hungary, in and around the city of Szombathely. Parents of all children provided a written informed consent in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and the stipulations of the institutional ethics board. All children agreed to participate. The study received clearance from the Ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education of the Eötvös Loránd University.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

2.1.1 Phonological STM group

There were 72 typical readers (31 boys and 41 girls) with full dataset. Their mean age was 9.89 years (Sd: 1.8, range: 7–13.08). There were 33 children among the excellent readers, with a mean age of 9.39 years (Sd: 1.813, range: 7.17–13.17). Their reading skills were on average 1.59–1.75 standard deviations above age-appropriate (considering a word and a pseudoword reading subtest).

2.1.2 Verbal STM group

Sixty-three children (32 boys and 31 girls) were included in the group of typical readers. Their mean age was 9.66 years with a standard deviation of 2.011. Typical readers’ reading performance was around 0 on standardized values. There were 21 excellent readers (9 boys, 12 girls) with a mean age of 10.230 years (Sd: 2.128, range: 6.83–12.92). The reading skills of excellent readers were on average 1.78–1.8 standard deviations above age-appropriate.

2.1.3 Visuospatial STM group

We had 92 typical readers (45 boys and 47 girls) with 10.026 years of mean age (Sd: 1.9, range: 6.75–13.33). Their performance was compared to that of 40 excellent readers (20 boys and 20 girls) with a mean age of 10.05 years (Sd: 2.06, range: 6.92–13.17). Excellent readers were on average 1.7–1.74 standard deviations above age-appropriate reading level.

2.2 Methods

2.2.1 VOLT one-minute reading test

The VOLT is a standardized test for word- and pseudoword reading. The word reading subtest is composed of a list of 180 words. Children are asked to read the words one after the other as quick as possible within 60 s. The pseudoword reading subtest is identical with 180 pseudowords as reading stimuli. The test has been standardized for children from grade 1 spring semester to grade 6 spring semester. Separate age-norms have been reported for every semester. Data collection of all children of the current study took place in the spring semester (between April and May). The test–retest correlation of word- and pseudoword reading is 0.859–0.966 (varying across grades and subtasks). The correlation between word- and pseudoword reading is 0.677–0.897 (Kemény et al., 2023). The measure of reading was the number of words and pseudowords read within 60 s.

2.2.2 Phonological STM

We used the semi-computerized version of a standardized Hungarian pseudoword repetition task (Racsmány et al., 2005). Through headphones participants heard the pseudowords and had to repeat them with precision. The repetition was evaluated by a student assistant. Pseudowords increased in length, starting with monosyllabic pseudowords. Each length had four pseudowords. If a child was able to repeat at least two of the items, the task proceeded to the longer sequences. The task continued until the exit criterion was reached, that is, until the child was unable to repeat at least half of the pseudowords. Phonological STM is characterized by the number of correctly repeated pseudowords.

2.2.3 Verbal STM

Verbal STM was measured with the digit span task. Participants heard digits one after the other, and had to recall them when prompted. We used a computerized task programmed in E-prime (Psychology Software Tools Inc., 2016), in which digits were presented auditorily through headphones, and children had to enter the number sequence in an identical order using the computer’s keyboard. Digits were recorded by a calm male voice. The length of the digits varied between 549 and 862 milliseconds. The recordings were stereo with 44.1 KHz sampling rate. Digits followed each other with a fixed 1,000 stimulus onset asynchrony. Although the task was automatized, a student assistant was always present to make sure the participant was not distracted and to assist entering the data.

The task started with two practice items to make sure children understood what they had to do. The practice trials were 2-digit sequences. The real items followed, starting from 3-digit-long sequences. There were four items from each length. The exit criterion was identical to the phonological STM task: children only proceeded to the next length if they repeated at least half of the items correctly. Verbal STM is characterized by the number of correctly repeated sequences.

2.2.4 Visuospatial STM

Visuospatial STM was assessed using a computerized task analogous to the Corsi blocks, in which participants had to repeat a visuospatial sequence. The task was programmed and run in E-prime (Psychology Software Tools Inc., 2016). A 4 × 4 black array of rectangles appeared on a white screen. The screen resolution was set to 1,024 × 768, the array had a size of 341 × 341 pixels. During the items, one of the 16 rectangles turned red for 650 ms, then turned blank again for 500 ms, then another rectangle turned red. The aim of the participants was to remember the order of the rectangles turning red. After the sequence they were prompted to repeat the sequence using the mouse and clicking on the given rectangles.

There were two practice trials in the beginning with sequences of two locations. The real items started with three locations, and had four sequences with each length. The same exit criterion was used as before: the task only proceeded to the next length if at least half of the sequences were correctly repeated. Although the task was fully automated, a student assistant was always present to make sure the children complied with the task requirements, and helped them as required. Visuospatial STM is characterized by the number of correctly repeated sequences.

2.3 Results

2.3.1 Phonological STM – nonword repetition

Data from the three STM tasks by group are presented in Figure 1. We conducted an ANOVA with Nonword repetition as dependent and Group (Typical readers vs. Excellent readers) as between subject variable. The ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of group, F(1, 103) = 2.868, p = 0.093, ηp2 = 0.027. Controlling for age, however, resulted in a significant group difference, F(1, 102) = 4.058, p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.038. Age was a significant covariate, F(1, 102) = 5.354, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.050.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. STM performance by group in the three conditions. Each bar represents the average number of correctly repeated sequence. Error bars represent SEM.

2.3.2 Verbal STM – digit span

We conducted an ANOVA with Verbal STM as dependent and Group (Typical readers vs. Excellent readers) as between subject variable. The ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of group, F(1, 82) = 0.941, p = 0.335, ηp2 = 0.011. The groups did not differ even after controlling for age, F(1, 81) = 0.286, p = 0.594, ηp2 = 0.004, whereas Age was a significant covariate, F(1, 81) = 18.547, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.186.

2.3.3 Visuospatial STM

We conducted an ANOVA with Visuospatial STM as dependent and Group (Typical readers vs. Excellent readers) as between subject variable. The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of group, F(1, 130) = 6.902, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.050. This difference remained significant even after controlling for age, F(1, 129) = 9.774, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.070. Age was a significant covariate, F(1, 129) = 59.339, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.315.

3 Discussion

The aim of the current study was to unravel whether and how excellent readers differ from age-appropriate readers in their short-term memory abilities. In accordance, we have contrasted the short-term memory performance of excellent readers (reading performance at least 1 SD above average) and typical readers (reading performance within ±0.5 SD from average). The clearest effect was that excellent readers were significantly better in visuospatial STM than typical readers. Apart from that, they also outperformed typical readers in phonological STM, however, this was only observed after controlling for age. No difference was observed in verbal STM on the digit span task.

Results of the current study are not new in the sense that there is ample evidence for the substantial role of short-term memory in reading. It is a novelty though that the effect is not constraint to the verbal/phonological domain, and not even strongest in this domain. In the following section we will discuss why different types of memory processes may contribute to excellent reading, and what implications it has to further psychological research and educational development programs.

Several previous studies have identified the important role of phonological awareness as well as phonological skills in reading and developmental dyslexia (Alexander et al., 1991; Hogan et al., 2005; Ramus and Szenkovits, 2008; Landerl et al., 2018; Powell and Atkinson, 2021). Research has also documented that while phonological skills are a prerequisite for reading, its scope changes with age (Goswami, 2002), and its role decreases with development (Hogan et al., 2005; Powell and Atkinson, 2021). This is in accordance with reading development. Early readers rely heavily on letter-speech sound associations as they have to sequentially decode the observed written text. Later on, children associate visual and phonological word forms, developing orthographic representations in the individuals’ orthographic lexicon (self-teaching, Share, 1995; Share and Shalev, 2004). These representations make way to sight-word reading, a process during which the reader recognizes visual word forms and retrieves the accompanying spoken word effortless (Ehri, 1991, 2014).

Other frameworks suggested that this shift toward sight word reading is not only supported by lexical representations (stored word forms), but also by the increasing grain size of orthographic information. That is, analytic levels start from letter-level, and increase toward larger and more consistent units (Ziegler and Goswami, 2005, 2006; Caravolas, 2006). Furthermore, Grainger and Ziegler (2011) suggested that orthographic information is processed along two parallel routes, the fine-grained route and the coarse-grained route. The fine-grained route puts a special emphasis on each individual unit, whereas the coarse-grained route is an approximate activation of the underlying patterns. During development, individuals rely less and less on fine-grained analyses and use more coarse-grained activations. The lack of focus on specific details makes individuals able to overcome noise, like transposed letters (Varga et al., 2021). The above models suggest that typical reading development involves a shift from specified to less specified visuospatial orthographic representations. This only seemingly contradicts the current results. Excellent readers may also decrease their reliance on visuospatial skills, however, their better visuospatial abilities may contribute to (1) the formation of orthographic representations, (2) access to orthographic representation.

The development of orthographic representations requires children to sequentially decode yet unknown words, and associate the observed visual forms with the produced phonological forms (Share, 1995; Share and Shalev, 2004). Sequential decoding is a slow and laborious process. Children have to remember the parse the letter sequence into decodable units, translate them into speech sounds and merge the speech sounds together. While this is enough to sound out the unknown word, children also have to store the new association, which load on visuospatial short term memory processes. This is in line with previous research suggesting that processing difficulties emerge with visual complexity, that is, visual complexity is a crucial factor in orthographic learning (Abdelhadi et al., 2011; Hsu et al., 2011; McBride-Chang et al., 2011).

The second possibility is that visuospatial skills affect access to orthographic representation. That is, the central effect relies not on the process of storing visual word forms, but on retrieving them. Better visuospatial short-term memory in this case would provide a spatially more precise cue for accessing the orthographic representation. This would be in line with previous research (Rao and Singh, 2015) highlighting the role of visual complexity in reading at the neural level. But such a hypothesis could also be integrated with the grain-size theory, suggesting that while it is beneficial to use larger grain-size, more detailed cues could enhance the dual routes in parallel (Grainger and Ziegler, 2011), leading to quicker phonological activation. This latter assumption suggests that visuospatial STM could support word recognition by making it more effective.

A similar pattern was observed in phonological short-term memory. As discussed above, since the role of phonological processing decreases with reading experience (Hogan et al., 2005; Powell and Atkinson, 2021), that the advantage of excellent readers is rather due to phonological skills being engaged in the development of the orthographic lexicon. That is, children who are better in their phonological STM will develop their orthographic lexicon quicker and easier than children with average phonological abilities. This hypothesis should, however, be analyzed in a longitudinal design. The lack of difference in verbal STM suggests that while verbal skills are necessary for typical reading (Trecy et al., 2013; Majerus and Cowan, 2016), they may not differentiate within the non-impaired region.

3.1 Enhancing visuospatial skills to support reading

While the current results show that excellent readers and typical readers differed most reliably in visuospatial STM, the educational challenge is whether one can integrate visuospatial trainings to support the reading development of children. The first question is whether one can train visuospatial STM, the second is whether such a training could be transferred to reading skills.

Several studies have recently addressed the effectiveness of working memory trainings. Some of these studies reported strong effects of training (Jaeggi et al., 2008; Morrison and Chein, 2011; Schwarb et al., 2016), while others argue for modest or no benefit after training (Melby-Lervåg and Hulme, 2013; Sala and Gobet, 2017; Kassai et al., 2019). Studies on visual short-term memory are even scarcer, but those that are available show a beneficial effect in both children (Caviola et al., 2009; Roberts et al., 2016) and older adults (McAvinue et al., 2013). Such beneficial effects, however, may be a result of changes in the strategies employed throughout memory processes (Gonthier, 2021). Whether or not a WM training leads to a development in a wider domain of cognition is only tangentially related to our theme, as we suggest visuospatial STM to be directly involved in the recognition of words and sublexical units.

The current study provides a plausible way to clarify controversial research results in gaming-based literacy trainings, suggesting that the beneficial effect may be mediated by visuospatial STM. A handful of studies provided evidence that computerized tasks enhance reading. Methods differ in a great range: some employing various games of executive functions (Pasqualotto et al., 2022) or action video games (Franceschini et al., 2013; Antzaka et al., 2017; Peters et al., 2021). These latter studies also highlight the importance of visual attention and executive functions. Analyzing the topic from a different perspective, studies have also shown visuospatial working memory and executive functions to be crucial in gaming (Hazarika and Dasgupta, 2020; Valls-Serrano et al., 2022), and video game-based visuospatial WM training to be highly effective – at least in older adults (Toril et al., 2016). Overall, it is plausible to assume that visuospatial working memory effects explain at least a part of the benefit by computerized EF training (Pasqualotto et al., 2022) and action video gaming (Franceschini et al., 2013) to reading. Testing such a hypothesis is, however, outside the scope of the current paper though.

3.2 Limitations and future directions

The most important limitation of the study is rooted in the study design. It would support interpretability if all children had data in all domains of short-term memory. It would allow us to contrast the effect of working memory measures on excellent reading. On the other hand, a longitudinal design would allow to examine if memory processes support the formation of orthographic representations or access to them. The current study, however, was designed as a side project of the standardization of a reading test, and was constraint to such design. It is also important to note that while the current study focused on short-term memory, short-term memory is also closely associated to intelligence, which in turn is often found to be related to reading and spelling (Peng et al., 2019; Zarić et al., 2021). Thus it would be interesting to explore whether and how differences in general cognitive abilities explain group differences between excellent and age-appropriate readers, and whether and how spelling abilities may covariate. A further possibility to consider is that although decoding and reading comprehension are highly correlated, reading comprehension does not equal decoding fluency (García and Cain, 2014). Using reading comprehension measures could further widen our knowledge on how STM is associated to reading skills. Finally, it is important to address the limitations of the current design. On the one hand, the quasi-experimental design does not allow causal inferences to be drawn, on the other hand, the small number of participants in our current setting we were only able to detect large effects (delta = 0.847 for the verbal STM comparison). Larger group sizes would allow a more fine-grained analyses of the STM differences.

3.3 Conclusion

In the current study we examined how excellent and age-appropriate readers differ from each other in terms of short-term memory. We reported an advantage of excellent readers in visuospatial and phonological short-term memory. We suggest that although the development of typical reading requires a diversion from spatial and phonological processes, the proper maintenance of these processes can support students in becoming excellent readers. However, we are yet to understand whether and how these processes could be utilized and integrated to primary school curricula.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics board of the Faculty of Education and Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

FK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Writing – original draft. GA: Writing – review & editing. OP: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. EPR: Writing – review & editing. CL-H: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The project received support from the National Research, Development and Innovation Office of Hungary: Grant FK-22-142797 “How statistics shape reading and spelling” (PI: FK). The authors acknowledge the financial support of the University of Graz.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the help of Milla Mária Horváth and István Tafferner. We would like to thank the children for the participation, their schools and parents for the support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., and Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writ. Syst. Res. 3, 117–127. doi: 10.1093/wsr/wsr014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, A. W., Andersen, H. G., Heilman, P. C., Voeller, K. K. S., and Torgesen, J. K. (1991). Phonological awareness training and remediation of analytic decoding deficits in a group of severe dyslexics. Ann. Dyslexia 41, 193–206. doi: 10.1007/BF02648086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antzaka, A., Lallier, M., Meyer, S., Diard, J., Carreiras, M., and Valdois, S. (2017). Enhancing reading performance through action video games: the role of visual attention span. Sci. Rep. 7:Article 1. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15119-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astley, J., Keage, H. A. D., Kelson, E., Callahan, R., Hofmann, J., Thiessen, M., et al. (2023). Font disfluency and reading performance in children: an event-related potential study. Brain Cogn. 169:105986. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2023.105986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacon, A. M., Parmentier, F. B. R., and Barr, P. (2013). Visuospatial memory in dyslexia: evidence for strategic deficits. Memory 21, 189–209. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.718789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, S. (1986). Short-term memory, phonological processing, and reading ability. Ann. Dyslexia 36, 138–153. doi: 10.1007/BF02648026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brem, S., Bach, S., Kucian, K., Kujala, J. V., Guttorm, T. K., Martin, E., et al. (2010). Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter–speech sound correspondences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 7939–7944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904402107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. J. Educ. Psychol. 81, 313–321. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravolas, M. (2006). Refining the psycholinguistic grain size theory: effects of phonotactics and word formation on the availability of phonemes to preliterate children. Dev. Sci. 9, 445–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00526.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Perea, M., Gil-López, C., Mallouh, R. A., and Salillas, E. (2013). Neural correlates of visual versus abstract letter processing in Roman and Arabic scripts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1975–1985. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., and Perea, M. (2007). ERP correlates of transposed-letter similarity effects: are consonants processed differently from vowels? Neurosci. Lett. 419, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castles, A., Holmes, V. M., Neath, J., and Kinoshita, S. (2003). How does orthographic knowledge influence performance on phonological awareness tasks? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 56, 445–467. doi: 10.1080/02724980244000486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caviola, S., Marmarella, I. C., Cornoldi, C., and Lucangeli, D. (2009). A metacognitive visuospatial working memory training for children. Int. Electron. J. Elem. Educ. 2, 122–136.

Google Scholar

Dillon, C. M., and Pisoni, D. B. (2006). Non word repetition and reading skills in children who are deaf and have cochlear implants. Volta Rev. 106, 121–145. doi: 10.17955/tvr.106.2.562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehri, L. C. (1991). “Development of the ability to read words” in Handbook of reading research. eds. R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.)

Google Scholar

Ehri, L. C. (1997). “Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost” in Learning to spell: research, theory, and practice across languages. eds. C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, and M. Fayol (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 237–269.

Google Scholar

Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Sci. Stud. Read. 9, 167–188. doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary learning. Sci. Stud. Read. 18, 5–21. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2013.819356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrar, M. J., and Ashwell, S. (2008). “The role of representational ability in the development of phonological awareness in preschool children” in Literacy processes: cognitive flexibility in learning and teaching. ed. K. B. Cartwright (New York, NY: Guilford), 71–89.

Google Scholar

Finkbeiner, M., and Coltheart, M. (2009). Letter recognition: from perception to representation. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 26, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/02643290902905294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., and Bertoni, S. (2019). Improving action video games abilities increases the phonological decoding speed and phonological short-term memory in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130, 100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. 23, 462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1985). “Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia” in Surface dyslexia. eds. K. E. In, J. C. Patterson, Marshall, and M. Coltheart (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 301–330.

Google Scholar

Froyen, D. J. W., Bonte, M. L., van Atteveldt, N., and Blomert, L. (2009). The long road to automation: neurocognitive development of letter-speech sound processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 567–580. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froyen, D. J. W., van Atteveldt, N., and Blomert, L. (2010). Exploring the role of low level visual processing in letter–speech sound integration: a visual MMN study. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 4:9. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, J. R., and Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and Reading comprehension: a Meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Rev. Educ. Res. 84, 74–111. doi: 10.3102/0034654313499616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genisio, V., and Bastien-Toniazzo, M. (2003). Is logographic processing holistic or analytic? Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 18, 239–249. doi: 10.1007/BF03173246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Landerl, K., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., de Jong, P. F., et al. (2020). Reading and spelling development across languages varying in orthographic consistency: do their paths cross? Child Dev. 91, e266–e279. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonthier, C. (2021). Charting the diversity of strategic processes in visuospatial short-term memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 294–318. doi: 10.1177/1745691620950697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (1999). “Phonological development and reading by analogy: Epilinguistic and metalinguistic issues” in Reading development and the teaching of reading: a psychological perspective. eds. J. Oakhill and R. Beard (Hoboken: Blackwell Science), 174–200.

Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: a cross-linguistic perspective. Ann. Dyslexia 52, 139–163. doi: 10.1007/s11881-002-0010-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., and de Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Appl. Psycholinguist. 19, 19–52. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, J., and Whitney, C. (2004). Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 58–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, J., and Ziegler, J. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Front. Psychol. 2:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, P., and Mac Whinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: computational and neural bases. Brain Lang. 59, 267–333. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasenäcker, J., and Schroeder, S. (2022). Transposed and substituted letter effects across reading development: a longitudinal study. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 48, 1202–1218. doi: 10.1037/xlm0001064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazarika, J., and Dasgupta, R. (2020). Neural correlates of action video game experience in a visuospatial working memory task. Neural Comput. Applic. 32, 3431–3440. doi: 10.1007/s00521-018-3713-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogan, T. P., Catts, H. W., and Little, T. D. (2005). The relationship between phonological awareness and Reading. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 36, 285–293. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2005/029)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, C.-H., Lee, C.-Y., and Marantz, A. (2011). Effects of visual complexity and sublexical information in the occipitotemporal cortex in the reading of Chinese phonograms: a single-trial analysis with MEG. Brain Lang. 117, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., and Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 6829–6833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Hewes, A. K., Leeke, T. C., and Phillips, C. E. (2004). What links verbal short-term memory performance and vocabulary level? Evidence of changing relationships among individuals with learning disability. J. Mem. Lang. 50, 134–148. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z., and Takacs, Z. K. (2019). A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near- and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychol. Bull. 145, 165–188. doi: 10.1037/bul0000180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemény, F., Pachner, O. P., Remete, E., Aranyi, G., and Laskay-Horváth, C. (2023). Lexikai hozzáférés és szekvenciális dekódolás megbízható mérése: a Vasi Olvasásteszt. [Assessing lexical access and sequential decoding reliably: The reading test of Vas (VOLT)] Manuscript submitted for publication.

Google Scholar

Kirkby, J. A., Barrington, R. S., Drieghe, D., and Liversedge, S. P. (2022). Parafoveal processing and transposed-letter effects in dyslexic reading. Dyslexia 28, 359–374. doi: 10.1002/dys.1721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., Jong, P. F. D., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., et al. (2018). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Sci. Stud. Read. 23, 220–234. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1510936

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lété, B., and Fayol, M. (2013). Substituted-letter and transposed-letter effects in a masked priming paradigm with French developing readers and dyslexics. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 47–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., and Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. Available at:http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED427291

Google Scholar

Luke, S. G., and Christianson, K. (2012). Semantic predictability eliminates the transposed-letter effect. Mem. Cogn. 40, 628–641. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0170-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupker, S. J., Zhang, Y. J., Perry, J. R., and Davis, C. J. (2015). Superset versus substitution-letter priming: an evaluation of open-bigram models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 138–151. doi: 10.1037/a0038392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majerus, S., and Cowan, N. (2016). The nature of verbal short-term impairment in dyslexia: the importance of serial order. Front. Psychol. 7:1522. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, V. A., and Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory. J. Learn. Disabil. 17, 592–599. doi: 10.1177/002221948401701005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAvinue, L., Golemme, M., Castorina, M., Tatti, E., Pigni, F., Salomone, S., et al. (2013). An evaluation of a working memory training scheme in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:20. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride-Chang, C., Zhou, Y., Cho, J.-R., Aram, D., Levin, I., and Tolchinsky, L. (2011). Visual spatial skill: a consequence of learning to read? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M. (2012). The relative predictive contribution and causal role of phoneme awareness, rhyme awareness, and verbal short-term memory in reading skills: a review. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 56, 101–118. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2011.621215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., and Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? Psychol. Bull. 49, 270–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, K., and Landerl, K. (2009). Double dissociation between Reading and spelling deficits. Sci. Stud. Read. 13, 359–382. doi: 10.1080/10888430903162878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A. B., and Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 46–60. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0034-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morton, J. (1989). “An information-processing account of reading acquisition” in From reading to neurons. ed. A. M. Galaburda (Cambridge: The MIT Press)

Google Scholar

Pasqualotto, A., Altarelli, I., De Angeli, A., Menestrina, Z., Bavelier, D., and Venuti, P. (2022). Enhancing reading skills through a video game mixing action mechanics and cognitive training. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 545–554. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01254-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, P., Wang, T., Wang, C., and Lin, X. (2019). A meta-analysis on the relation between fluid intelligence and reading/mathematics: effects of tasks, age, and social economics status. Psychol. Bull. 145, 189–236. doi: 10.1037/bul0000182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perea, M., and Carreiras, M. (2008). Do orthotactics and phonology constrain the transposed-letter effect? Lang. Cogn. 23, 69–92. doi: 10.1080/01690960701578146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Sci. Stud. Read. 11, 357–383. doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, J. L., Crewther, S. G., Murphy, M. J., and Bavin, E. L. (2021). Action video game training improves text reading accuracy, rate and comprehension in children with dyslexia: a randomized controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 11:Article 1. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-98146-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, D., and Atkinson, L. (2021). Unraveling the links between rapid automatized naming (RAN), phonological awareness, and reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 113, 706–718. doi: 10.1037/edu0000625

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Psychology Software Tools Inc. (2016). E-Prime 3.0 [Computer software]. Available at:https://support.pstnet.com/

Google Scholar

Racsmány, M., Lukács, Á., Németh, D., and Pléh, C. (2005). A verbális munkamemória magyar nyelvű vizsgálóeljárásai. Magy. Pszichol. Szle. 60, 479–506. doi: 10.1556/mpszle.60.2005.4.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F., and Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 129–141. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, C., and Singh, N. C. (2015). Visuospatial complexity modulates reading in the brain. Brain Lang. 141, 50–61. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, G., Quach, J., Spencer-Smith, M., Anderson, P. J., Gathercole, S., Gold, L., et al. (2016). Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 170:e154568. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roembke, T. C., Hazeltine, E., Reed, D. K., and McMurray, B. (2019). Automaticity of word recognition is a unique predictor of reading fluency in middle-school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 314–330. doi: 10.1037/edu0000279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala, G., and Gobet, F. (2017). Working memory training in typically developing children: a meta-analysis of the available evidence. Dev. Psychol. 53, 671–685. doi: 10.1037/dev0000265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarb, H., Nail, J., and Schumacher, E. H. (2016). Working memory training improves visual short-term memory capacity. Psychol. Res. 80, 128–148. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0648-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seymour, P. H., and Elder, L. (1986). Beginning reading without phonology. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 3, 1–36. doi: 10.1080/02643298608252668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Share, D. L., and Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Read. Writ. 17, 769–800. doi: 10.1007/s11145-004-2658-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith-Spark, J., Fisk, J., Fawcett, A., and Nicolson, R. (2003). Investigating the central executive in adult dyslexics: evidence from phonological and visuospatial working memory performance. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 567–587. doi: 10.1080/09541440340000024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, A. F., Banfi, C., Finke, S., Kemény, F., Clayton, F. J., Göbel, S. M., et al. (2021). Twenty-four or four-and-twenty: language modulates cross-modal matching for multidigit numbers in children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 202:104970. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, G. B. (2009). The long learning route to abstract letter units. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 26, 50–69. doi: 10.1080/02643290802200838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiffin-Richards, M. C., Hasselhorn, M., Woerner, W., Rothenberger, A., and Banaschewski, T. (2008). Phonological short-term memory and central executive processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with/without dyslexia – evidence of cognitive overlap. J. Neural Transm. 115, 227–234. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0816-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., Mayas, J., and Ballesteros, S. (2016). Video game training enhances visuospatial working memory and episodic memory in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:206. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trecy, M. P., Steve, M., and Martine, P. (2013). Impaired short-term memory for order in adults with dyslexia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 2211–2223. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valls-Serrano, C., De Francisco, C., Vélez-Coto, M., and Caracuel, A. (2022). Visuospatial working memory and attention control make the difference between experts, regulars and non-players of the videogame league of legends. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:933331. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.933331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varga, V., Tóth, D., Amora, K. K., Czikora, D., and Csépe, V. (2021). ERP correlates of altered orthographic-phonological processing in dyslexia. Front. Psychol. 12:723404. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.723404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhagen, J., and Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 65–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarić, J., Hasselhorn, M., and Nagler, T. (2021). Orthographic knowledge predicts reading and spelling skills over and above general intelligence and phonological awareness. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 36, 21–43. doi: 10.1007/s10212-020-00464-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., and Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol. Bull. 131, 3–29. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., and Goswami, U. (2006). Becoming literate in different languages: similar problems, different solutions. Dev. Sci. 9, 429–436. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00509.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: word reading, pseudoword reading, excellent readers, phonological short-term memory, verbal short-term memory, visuospatial short-term memory

Citation: Kemény F, Aranyi G, Pachner O, P. Remete E and Laskay-Horváth C (2024) What makes an excellent reader? Short-term memory contrasts between two groups of children. Front. Educ. 8:1325177. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1325177

Received: 20 October 2023; Accepted: 22 December 2023;
Published: 09 January 2024.

Edited by:

Jonathan Glazzard, University of Hull, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Nicolas Stefaniak, Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, France
Aimee Quickfall, Leeds Trinity University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Kemény, Aranyi, Pachner, P. Remete and Laskay-Horváth. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ferenc Kemény, ZmVyZW5jLmtlbWVueUB1bmktZ3Jhei5hdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.