Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Educ., 30 October 2023
Sec. Special Educational Needs
This article is part of the Research Topic Interventions for Students with Combined Learning and Behavioral Difficulties View all 5 articles

The intersection of learning difficulties and behavior problems – a scoping review of intervention research

  • Leibniz University Hannover, Institute of Special Education, Hannover, Germany

Learning difficulties (LDif) and behavioral problems (BP) frequently co-occur. Affected students typically require interventions targeting learning-, social-, as well as emotional and behavioral-domains. The goal of this scoping review is therefore to provide an overview of the research on interventions that target these critical areas for students with or at-risk of disabilities. In total, 48 relevant studies were identified and analyzed regarding addressed competencies, target groups, the setting of the interventions as well as underlying causal assumptions. The review identified a variety of interventions and approaches designed to address LDif and BP, which makes it impossible to draw conclusions on a single best approach. Co-occurring LDif and BP reflect a plurality of difficulties that are incompatible with a ‘one size fits all’ approach. The vast majority of studies assumed that the relationship between LDif and BP are reciprocal/unidirectional. Few studies focused common variables that potentially affect both LDif and BP. In addition, the majority of studies addressed child-level variables. Future research should be conducted to focus on variables at the environmental or institutional level that might impact both LDif and BP.

1. Introduction

In educational contexts, learning difficulties can be observed frequently and across different settings. While most students struggle in institutionalized learning only for a limited time and in isolated areas during their school life, some students experience persistent and pervasive failure. Such failure is often labeled using different terminologies such as learning disabilities, specific learning disorders or special educational needs (e.g., Grigorenko et al., 2020; Mähler, 2020; Krämer et al., 2021). In light of the plurality of categories, it is not surprising that there ‘is no universally agreed definition of learning difficulties’ (MacKay, 2009, p. 10). ‘Learning difficulties’ might thus be regarded as an umbrella term for variations of different learning deficits (Lenhard et al., 2013) that differ in severity and chronicity. Hereby (specific) learning disabilities (LD) can be regarded as the most severe forms of low achievement (Kavale and Forness, 2000), which manifest in varying forms and are of pervasive nature. Students with LD usually exhibit major difficulties in at least one of different basic areas of learning (reading, writing, math and/or language) (Kavale and Forness, 2000). In this paper, we use the term learning difficulties to describe the overarching category of learning deficits that encompasses specific operationalizations. When research findings are based on a specific operationalization of learning deficits, however, this will be named accordingly in the theoretical explanations. This leads to a use of different terminology (learning difficulties, learning disorders, learning disabilities).

Traditionally, learning disabilities were described as underachievement that relied on ability-achievement discrepancies in the identification (e.g., Kavale and Forness, 2000; Grigorenko et al., 2020). The current ICD-11 criteria are yet other criteria that still are used to define specific learning disorders as an achievement that ‘is markedly below what would be expected for chronological age and general level of intellectual functioning’ to assign the label of developmental learning disorders (World Health Organization, 2023). The Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (UN General Assembly, 2006) however defines disabilities as a result of the interaction between individuals with impairments and various barriers in their daily environment emphasizing a social model of disability. Hence, learning disabilities might be regarded as a consequence of a mismatch between students’ profiles (e.g., working memory deficits, lack of metacognitive control) and societal barriers (e.g., the quality of the instruction, structure of teaching materials). Therefore, it is not surprising that other recent conceptualizations are increasingly moving away from emphasizing achievement discrepancy criteria and highlight inadequate responses to school-based interventions as ‘an alternative inclusion criterion’ (Grigorenko et al., 2020, p. 39).

As mentioned above, despite the variations of learning difficulties, they occur frequently in schools and often result in both academic and behavioral problems. Students with learning difficulties depict the largest group among students with special educational needs (SEN). In Germany, approximately one third of all students with SEN is identified as having learning difficulties (KMK, 2020). Consistently, Grigorenko et al. (2020) state that learning disabilities are the largest category of SEN in the United States. From a clinical perspective, Moll et al. (2014) point out that specific learning disorders are among the most common mental disorders in children and adolescents. At the same time, different and inconsistent diagnostic criteria are applied, which leads to differences in the estimated prevalence across studies (MacKay, 2009). Hence, broader or unspecific diagnostic inclusion criteria might result in higher prevalence estimates. Regardless of the exact prevalence rate, teachers are faced with the challenge of meeting the needs of students with learning difficulties on a daily basis. This becomes even more important as more and more schools move to inclusive school models, causing an increase in the number of students with special educational needs in these schools. In this regard, the importance of teachers’ self-efficacy concerning the implementation of inclusive teaching has been stressed (De Neve et al., 2015; Kiel et al., 2020). Designing appropriate educational support for students with such specific needs might further require special knowledge such as insights into potential causal explanations of learning difficulties as well as information on effective interventions.

1.1. The lnterplay of learning difficulties and emotional/behavioral problems

The development of learning difficulties can be explained from different theoretical perspectives. Neuro-biological approaches, which emphasize cognitive determinants in explaining academic achievement (Lenhard et al., 2013), belong to the most prominent explanations of LD (Büttner and Hasselhorn, 2011). Here, neuro-cognitive determinants exemplary comprise working memory (e.g., Maehler and Schuchardt, 2011), attentional processes (e.g., Gadeyne et al., 2004; Commodari, 2012; Rabiner et al., 2016) or metacognition (e.g., Ohtani and Hisasaka, 2018). However, socio-emotional variables might additionally explain the development and manifestation of learning difficulties. In line with this, associations of academic achievement and variables such as (academic) self-concept (e.g., Chapman, 1988), motivation (e.g., Sideridis, 2009) as well as emotions (e.g., Pekrun et al., 2017; Sainio et al., 2019) are described.

In light of these findings, it is not surprising that additional socio-emotional or behavioral problems are described for students who struggle in academic learning. These can be of internalizing or externalizing nature (Willcutt and Pennington, 2000; Visser et al., 2020). With regard to internalizing behaviors, Visser et al. (2020) highlighted that 20% of the students with specific learning disorders fulfilled criteria of anxiety disorders, almost 30% showed depressive symptoms. Willcutt and Pennington (2000) report high levels of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents with reading difficulties. A meta-analysis by Nelson and Harwood (2011) revealed higher anxiety scores for children with learning difficulties in comparison to their peers. In addition, struggling learners report lower levels of school well-being (Ingesson, 2007; Benassi et al., 2022).

Several studies highlight that externalizing behaviors seem to co-occur with learning difficulties as well. Some time ago, Kavale and Forness (1996) suggested that up to 75% of the students with learning difficulties show problems in their social behavior. More recently, Visser et al. (2020) report increased rates of conduct disorder and ADHD in children with specific learning disorders. Similarly, Willcutt and Pennington (2000) found that students with reading difficulties are more likely to meet the criteria of ADHD as well as oppositional and conduct disorders. Multiple studies report negative associations between behavioral problems and academic achievement per se (e.g., Nelson et al., 2004; Algozzine et al., 2011; Ennis et al., 2018). Breslau et al. (2009) found that internalizing and externalizing problems at the age of 6 negatively predicted academic achievement at the age of 17. Similar results are reported by Malinauskiene et al. (2011), who showed that delinquent behavior as well as somatic symptoms were accompanied by lower academic achievement. The relation between behavioral problems and learning difficulties seems to be particularly strong for externalizing behaviors as well as attention problems, which have negative effects on academic success (Barriga et al., 2002; Nelson et al., 2004; Masten et al., 2005; Breslau et al., 2009; McLeod et al., 2012). The specific relevance of attention processes is further stressed by studies that examine academic outcomes of children with attentional difficulties (e.g., ADHD). Rabiner et al. (2016) found that inattention in grade 1 leads to lower academic achievement in grade 5. Furthermore, a study by Mayes et al. (2000) indicated that more than two thirds of the students with ADHD showed learning difficulties. Similarly, Dietz and Montague (2006) showed high levels of comorbidity between ADHD, emotional and behavioral disorders and LD. Horbach et al. (2020) discuss that ADHD moderates the relationship between specific learning disorders in reading/spelling and behavioral difficulties. When explaining the link between learning difficulties and attentional problems, several recent studies addressed a range of variables. E.g., Child et al. (2019) examine potential correlates of reading, math and attentional skills. They identify working memory and phonological awareness to play an overlapping role. Peterson et al. (2017) extend these results by further highlighting processing speed to be associated to the overlap of reading, math and attention. At the same time, specific executive functions were related to each domain. Regarding the vast body of research on the association of ADHD and learning difficulties, it is not surprising that a range of intervention studies consequently focused co-occurring ADHD and learning difficulties (DuPaul et al., 2012; Fabiano et al., 2021).

1.2. Models of comorbidity and appropriate interventions

In practice, co-occurring learning difficulties and behavioral problems (such as ADHD) might pose a particular challenge for teachers and students (Rock et al., 1997). Questions on how to address co-occurring problems ask for necessary insights into causal patterns of the comorbidity of both phenomena. Among other, the interplay of learning and behavioral difficulties might be explained in light of different theoretical models of comorbidities (e.g., Neale and Kendler, 1995; Pennington, 2006). Here, different scenarios of comorbidity of multifactorial phenomena are distinguished, which might serve as a framework for clustering co-occurring learning difficulties and behavioral problems. Such explanations can be traced back to the works of Pennington (2006), who argues (in the case of reading disorder and ADHD) that “each individual disorder would each have its own profile of risk factors (both etiologic and cognitive), with some of these risk factors being shared by another disorder, resulting in comorbidity” (p. 404). Such multiple deficit models focus on overlapping explanatory variables that might be addressed to influence both domains. The causal link between learning difficulties and behavioral problems can thereby be explained by a range of variables. For example, students’ working memory capacity as well as executive functions could have an impact on their academic achievement as well as on their classroom behavior. Potentially influencing variables might be located on the neuro-biological level (e.g., self-regulation skills, executive functions, attention), as well as environmental level (e.g., socio-economic background, parenting style, instructional quality). In contrast, single-deficit models depict alternative explanations. Here, models of multiformity (or symptom phenocopy hypothesis) by Neale and Kendler (1995) describe that an increased risk for difficulties in one area leads to difficulties in a second area. For example, learning difficulties could lead to an increased risk of inattentive or hyperactive behavior in the classroom or inattentive behavior could increase the risk of having learning difficulties. Based on these assumptions, variables associated with either behavior problems or learning difficulties can be addressed in a first step to indirectly influence the second area of interest. At the same time, the validity of single deficit models for explaining heterotypic comorbidities (e.g., learning and behavioral difficulties) has been questioned (Moll et al., 2020). Nonetheless, single-deficit models are still being applied in current research.

Differentiating models of comorbidity in practice might be difficult, however, deciding on the causal model of reference and key variables to address seems to be crucial for the development of effective and appropriate interventions. Insights into effective existing interventions trying to target aspects of both learning and behavior might support practitioners’ decisions on the most appropriate approach. The importance of such interventions is additionally stressed by the fact that students with co-occurring problems might be more severely impaired than students with just one problem area (Mayes et al., 2000; McLeod et al., 2012) and benefit less from the interventions applied (Rock et al., 1997; Rabiner et al., 2004; Herzog and Casale, 2022).

1.3. Research aim

The high frequency of co-occurring learning difficulties and behavior problems stresses the importance of specific interventions for this group of students. However, up until today an overview of effective interventions is lacking. At the same time, insights in promising approaches might support researchers and practitioners in choosing and developing the most appropriate instruction for affected students. Thus, the aim of this systematic scoping review is to give an overview of evaluated interventions that address both learning difficulties and behavioral problems within their research design.

The objective of this review is to:

(1) describe the interventions applied in research studies,

(2) summarize the outcomes and competencies addressed by the interventions reviewed,

(3) determine the descriptive characteristics of students included,

(4) describe the nature and the setting of the interventions,

(5) describe the underlying causal assumptions.

2. Methods

2.1. Search procedure

To answer the aforementioned research questions, relevant articles were identified in a first step. Therefore, the databases PubPsych and PsycINFO were searched using the following search-syntax:

• To address learning and behavior problems:

• AB:(learn* OR math* OR read* OR litera* OR arithmet* OR num* OR achievement OR scholastic OR writ* OR spelling OR dyslex* OR dyscalculia OR acalculia OR agraphia)

• AB:(behav*r OR emotion* OR social* OR internal* OR externaliz* OR attention OR ADH* OR depress* OR anxiety OR fear OR psych*) AND AB:(problem* OR difficult* OR poor)

• To identify intervention studies:

• AB:(intervention OR promot* OR improve* OR enhance* OR stimulat* OR train* OR treatment OR therapy OR program OR tutor* OR support OR best practice)

• To address comorbidity:

• AB:(comorbid* OR co-occur* OR combin*)

• To identify a school-aged sample:

• AB:(child* OR adoles* OR school* OR)

• To exclude studies on intellectual disabilities and autism:

• NOT AB:(intellectual disabilit* OR autis*)

Additional to the descriptors above, German synonyms were used in the search syntax. All key areas were combined by the Boolean operator AND. The descriptors were used to browse the abstracts of records. The exclusion criteria was included after an initial search yielded in the identification of multiple records focusing autism or persons with intellectual disabilities. In order to exclude these studies early in the identification process, the initial search syntax was adapted.

2.1.1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies needed to fulfill the following criteria to be included in the review:

• The study is an intervention study. Medication studies without any further interventions were excluded. Regarding the research design, all kinds of designs (e.g., randomized controlled trials, multiple baseline designs, single case studies) were included.

• Both learning and behavioral dimensions were included as dependent variables in the study design.

• The sample was of school age (5–17 years).

• Participants had at least problems in one of the target areas (learning or behavior) prior to the intervention.

• The study was published in a peer-reviewed journal in English, German or Dutch language.

• The publication date was less than 25 years ago.

We decided to include studies, in which participants only had problems in one of the target areas (learning difficulties or behavioral problems), in cases where both learning and behavioral dimensions were included as dependent variables. This approach might result in different categories of studies: a) studies that explicitly assume comorbid learning difficulties and behavioral problems prior to the intervention; b) studies that implicitly assume comorbid learning difficulties and behavioral problems prior to the intervention; c) studies that do not assume comorbidity but examine transfer effects across domains. To decide which category a study belongs to, the rationale within the studies was examined. An example of studies in category b) would be a study in which the included sample has difficulties in only one domain, but the authors argue theoretically that students with problems in that domain typically also have difficulties in the other domain, or when they describe a risk factor that could underlie both target domains without explicitly testing for co-occurring difficulties in their sample.

2.2. Study selection

The initial search yielded in 4059 articles (02/18/21). Following the PRISMA-guidelines (Moher et al., 2009), duplicates were removed first. Afterwards, articles that were not published in any of the aforementioned languages were excluded. The remaining 3,140 articles were included in a title screening, which led to the exclusion of the majority of studies. At the same time, all sources that could not be identified as empirical articles describing an intervention study (e.g., dissertations, book chapters) were excluded. 346 studies were included in the abstract screening. Due to the long-lasting screening process, the literature search was repeated for a second time (04/29/22). At the same time, references of the included articles were searched for relevant studies. This resulted in 97 full-texts that were assessed for eligibility. A total of N = 48 full-texts were included in the review at hand (see Figure 1). Included references are marked with an asterisk in the reference list. During the screening process all studies were assessed by the first author and a second person (student assistant) separately. In case of disagreements, they were resolved by discussing the critical articles until a consensus was reached.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study selection process.

2.3. Data extraction

The platform ‘Covidence’ (Veritas Health Innovation, 2022) was used for study selection and data extraction. ‘Covidence’ allows to create a digital data extraction template, which can then be used to extract all relevant information from the included articles. Apart from the relevant information to answer the research questions, general information about the studies such as author(s), title, publication date and country, in which the study took place, were extracted. The relevant data to answer the research questions include information on:

Research questions and research design: It was coded whether a qualitative or quantitative approach was applied, which type of research design (e.g., experimental or single case) was used, whether a control group was present and if so, what kind of control group (e.g., waiting control). In addition, design specifics, such as the number of measuring points were extracted. The dependent variables, which targeted learning and behavior difficulties, as well as specific research questions were coded.

Description of the sample: The derived information about the sample include sample size, gender distribution, age range, mean age and/or class level, school level as well as the learning difficulties and/or behavior problems that were described prior to the intervention.

Intervention: To get an overview of the different interventions, several information were gathered. These include duration of the intervention (e.g., timespan, number of sessions, length of each session), implementation specifics (e.g., group size, setting) as well as addressed competencies.

Causal assumptions: The information comprised assumptions of comorbidity described in the studies. To extract this information, the rationale throughout the theoretical background of the studies was analyzed.

Main findings of the study: The main findings of the study regarding the target areas were summarized.

Data extraction was conducted by the first author. To pilot the data extraction template, 10 studies were randomly selected and additionally coded by the same student assistant that helped during the initial screening. The initial agreement between both raters was approx. 65%. Disagreements were consequently discussed, and the extraction template was adapted accordingly. To minimize the number of inaccuracies, as many single choice options as possible were used in the final template. Free text fields mostly asked for very short and clear information such as sample size, gender ratio or name of the implemented training program (if applicable). Potential limitations of this approach are discussed in the respective section at the end of the discussion.

3. Results

An overview of the extracted information can be found in Tables 1, 2. In order to facilitate the presentation of the results, Table 1 provides information on the design of the included studies, whereas Table 2 contains information on the characteristics of the intervention and the main results of the included studies.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Extracted information on design characteristics of the included studies.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Extracted information on characteristics of the implemented intervention.

Each table was divided regarding the form of comorbidity that was assumed in the study. Approximately 25% of the included studies explicitly assumed a comorbidity of learning difficulties (LDif) and behavioral problems (BP) prior to the intervention. A total of 14.58% of the studies were classified as implicitly assuming comorbidity prior to the intervention [e.g., Casale et al. (2017) as well as Keller and Brunstein (2022) who assume that students with attention problems are in need of a writing intervention - and thus might be affected by learning difficulties]. A total of 20.83% of the included studies were rated as not having prior theoretical assumptions on the comorbidity of BP and LDif, but including at least partly participants that showed LDif and BP. Almost 40% of the included studies (39.58%) did not indicate any theoretical or methodological reference to the comorbidity of LDif and BP, but included measures related to LDif and BP in the study design.

The included studies were further grouped along the research design that was applied. In total, five different groups of design were distinguished: (1) randomized controlled trials (47.92%), (2) pre-post-designs with control group (20.83%), (3) pre-post-designs without control group (8.33%), (4) multiple baseline studies (14.58%), and (5) alternative forms of single case studies (8.33%). All included studies used a quantitative approach; no distinction was thus made between qualitative and quantitative designs.

3.1. Addressed competencies

During the screening process, it became clear that it was not always possible to distinguish between dependent variables that address dimensions of learning from variables that address dimensions of behavior [e.g., on−/off-task behavior (Lee et al., 1999; Wehby et al., 2003; Everatt et al., 2011; Carboni et al., 2013); executive functions (Egeland et al., 2013; Paananen et al., 2018)]. It was decided to include those studies as well in cases that had dependent variables in at least one of the target areas.

The interventions in the included studies addressed a broad range of competencies. Due to the multitude of interventions, only an overview of the addressed competencies is provided here. More detailed information can be found in Table 2.

About 30% of the included studies aimed to address some sort of literacy skills such as different reading competencies (e.g., Wehby et al., 2003; Hechtman et al., 2004; Roberts et al., 2015; Tamm et al., 2017; Tannock et al., 2018) or writing skills (e.g., Evans et al., 2004; Everatt et al., 2011; Casale et al., 2017; Keller and Brunstein, 2022). In comparison, math competencies were only considered in about one eighth of the studies. These studies either addressed broad range of (academic) competencies or targeted individual difficulties (e.g., Hechtman et al., 2004; Tannock et al., 2018; Schieltz et al., 2020). Only three studies (Lee et al., 1999; Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Mautone et al., 2005) explicitly aimed to increase mathematical competencies solely. Other academic skills addressed were study skills (e.g., Hechtman et al., 2004; Langberg et al., 2006) or learning strategies (Walter and Döpfner, 2006; Freilich and Shechtman, 2010). Some studies did not further specify the learning or academic skills (e.g., Evans et al., 2005; Langberg et al., 2006; Friedman et al., 2019).

Just over a quarter of the interventions targeted social skills in particular. These include training in interpersonal problem solving (e.g., Elias et al., 2003; Evans et al., 2011; Schultz et al., 2017), interpersonal communication (Kopelman-Rubin et al., 2012), conversation (Evans et al., 2005) or not further specified social skills (e.g., The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; Evans et al., 2016; Friedman et al., 2019; Murphy et al., 2019). In addition to academic and social skills, a third area targeted across multiple studies are cognitive skills such as working memory skills (Gray et al., 2012; Egeland et al., 2013; Chacko et al., 2014), executive functions (Paananen et al., 2018), metacognitive skills (e.g., Langberg et al., 2012; Casale et al., 2017; Tannock et al., 2018) and attention (e.g., Shalev et al., 2007; Rabiner et al., 2010; Paananen et al., 2018; Keller and Brunstein, 2022).

About a third of the interventions included training of organizational skills (e.g., desk organization, managing materials). Different forms of behavior such as on-task behavior (Paananen et al., 2018), disruptive behavior (Walter and Döpfner, 2006), classroom behavior (The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; Langberg et al., 2006) and forms of behavior management (e.g., Miranda et al., 2006; Sibley et al., 2013; Tamm et al., 2017; Koenigs et al., 2019) were targeted as well. In addition to competencies that appeared in multiple interventions some topics were only addressed in few studies. These topics include mindfulness (Carboni et al., 2013), listening skills (Dursun et al., 2021), the use of art therapy (Freilich and Shechtman, 2010), relaxation techniques (Everatt et al., 2011; Schramm et al., 2016), self-esteem (Hechtman et al., 2004), emotion regulation (Friedman et al., 2019) and self-awareness (Kopelman-Rubin et al., 2012).

3.2. Description of the target groups

The target groups of the interventions described were diverse. In all included studies, the majority of participants was male. Nine of the included studies even had a sample including only boys (Lee et al., 1999; Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Barry and Messer, 2003; Elias et al., 2003; Wehby et al., 2003; Mautone et al., 2005; Carboni et al., 2013; Casale et al., 2017; Schieltz et al., 2020). These studies mainly used some kind of single case design so that sample sizes are rather small. In the included studies, the whole school age was covered. The majority of participants was between seven and fourteen years old. Only a sixth of the studies comprised a sample that was completely over 12 years of age (Barry and Messer, 2003; Walter and Döpfner, 2006; Antshel et al., 2012; Gray et al., 2012; Boyer et al., 2015; Schramm et al., 2016; Casale et al., 2017; Dursun et al., 2021). Some studies included samples with a broad age range, such as seven to fifteen (Freilich and Shechtman, 2010; Everatt et al., 2011) or six to twelve (Fabiano et al., 2010) or 13 (Shalev et al., 2007) years. Nine studies did not provide any information on the exact age. They aimed target groups from kindergarten to grade 8. The participants are almost equally distributed between primary and secondary school. Some interventions explicitly addressed students, who were in a special education program (Lee et al., 1999; Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Fabiano et al., 2010; Everatt et al., 2011; Gray et al., 2012; Casale et al., 2017; Murphy et al., 2019; Keller and Brunstein, 2022).

Most studies (83.3%) involved participants diagnosed with ADHD or showing ADHD-related symptoms (e.g., attention problems). Moreover, some studies reported to include participants showing emotional and behavioral disorders (Lee et al., 1999; Wehby et al., 2003; Casale et al., 2017; Murphy et al., 2019; Keller and Brunstein, 2022) as well as not further specified or combined behavior problems (Elias et al., 2003; Langberg et al., 2006; Owens et al., 2008). Additional participants were described as showing problem and disruptive behaviors (Owens et al., 2005; Schieltz et al., 2020). Academic difficulties were mostly apparent in forms of not further specified learning problems or LD (31.25%). Few studies included students with reading (Roberts et al., 2015; Tamm et al., 2017; Tannock et al., 2018; Koenigs et al., 2019) or writing difficulties (Koenigs et al., 2019; Keller and Brunstein, 2022). Students with math difficulties were only included in two studies (Lee et al., 1999; Mautone et al., 2005).

When solely focusing studies that explicitly take comorbid samples into account, ADHD or attention problems and associated difficulties in reading or writing (Tamm et al., 2017; Tannock et al., 2018; Koenigs et al., 2019; Keller and Brunstein, 2022) as well as comorbid learning disabilities or academic impairment and ADHD (Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Everatt et al., 2011; Gray et al., 2012; Sibley et al., 2013) comprise the most common groups of participants. In addition, comorbid learning and behavior problems (Elias et al., 2003; Schieltz et al., 2020) were included. The combination of math and behavior problems did only appear in one study (Lee et al., 1999). The same is true for math difficulties in combination with ADHD (Mautone et al., 2005).

3.3. Description of the interventions

The interventions that were applied in the studies varied a lot. Detailed information on the interventions can be found in Table 2.

3.3.1. Intervention mode and setting

Group training and individual training was implemented approximately equally often. Within the studies that contained forms of group training, only one study implemented a class-wide intervention (Murphy et al., 2019). However, class sizes were six to eight students, which is comparable to the other group interventions that typically had group sizes of three to eight students. Seven studies did not provide clear information on the group sizes. Around one seventh of the studies used a combination of individual and group training. Approximately 37.5% of the interventions additionally included some form of parent training and around one eighth applied different versions of teacher training or counseling.

The length of the interventions varied greatly. While some interventions lasted only for 3 weeks (Everatt et al., 2011; Murphy et al., 2019), others lasted for up to two or three years (Hechtman et al., 2004; Roberts et al., 2015). Approximately one third of the articles did not provide enough information to extract the exact length of the interventions. In line with differences concerning the lengths of the intervention, the number of sessions and the duration of one session differed greatly. The number of sessions varied between only three sessions (Evans et al., 2011; Everatt et al., 2011) and more than 65 sessions (Freilich and Shechtman, 2010). Sessions were between 10 minutes (Murphy et al., 2019) and 3 hours (Miranda et al., 2006; Freilich and Shechtman, 2010) long. Most of the interventions had sessions between 30 and 90 min (M = 61.16 min.). Almost a third of the articles did not provide enough information on the number of sessions, the length of one session or both.

Most of the interventions took place in a face-to-face setting. Only seven interventions were computerized. These interventions aimed to improve either working memory capacities (Gray et al., 2012; Egeland et al., 2013; Chacko et al., 2014), math skills (Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Mautone et al., 2005) or attention (Shalev et al., 2007; Rabiner et al., 2010). The studies of Rabiner et al. (2010) and Gray et al. (2012) additionally included a group that participated in computerized reading and/or math training.

3.3.2. Components of the interventions

As described before, a variety of competencies was addressed in the interventions. This is also reflected in the components of the interventions. Some interventions applied multi-modal or multi-tiered approaches. The interventions ranged from pre-existing formalized programs to newly developed programs.

With regard to academic skills, modules of the interventions included many different approaches to improve reading and writing skills. For example, Casale et al. (2017) as well as Keller and Brunstein (2022) used a training which aims to enhance text writing skills by teaching writing (e.g., typical structure of a text) and metacognitive strategies such as planning how to write a text. In the study of Everatt et al. (2011), participants either just copied words or used multi-sensory learning techniques (e.g., saying the word, naming the letters of the word and tracing the word) in order to improve spelling skills. Reading skills were targeted by teaching how to decode words (e.g., Tamm et al., 2017) or blend sounds into words (Wehby et al., 2003; Tannock et al., 2018), using repeated reading practice (e.g., Tamm et al., 2017) and modeling reading strategies (e.g., Wehby et al., 2003). Mathematical competencies were mainly addressed by using instructional strategies on how to solve math tasks (Lee et al., 1999; Schieltz et al., 2020) or by computerized trainings (Ota and DuPaul, 2002; Mautone et al., 2005).

To address behavioral problems, contingency systems and Daily Report Cards (DRC) were implemented across multiple studies. For example, Koenigs et al. (2019) used a Response Cost Token system within the child sessions. Children could gain points that they could swap for toys at the end of the program. In the study of Rabiner et al. (2010), students could earn marbles for following behavior rules. After reaching a certain level, participant groups earned rewards such as pizza parties. Similarly, students in the study of Casale et al. (2017) could collect points that they could use to gain time for pleasant activities like time to read. Most often, DRC were combined with a reward system at home. For example, Fabiano et al. (2010) implemented DRC at school and trained parents to install a reward contingent at home (e.g., time on the computer). Similar approaches are described by Owens et al. (2005, 2008) as well as Hechtman et al. (2004). In the study by Friedman et al. (2019), DRC were mainly used to discuss the achievement of personal treatment goals with parents, students and teachers.

Another component, which appeared in multiple studies, were elements of parent training. These included different kind of topics such as psychoeducation (e.g., Evans et al., 2005; Tamm et al., 2017; Koenigs et al., 2019), managing difficult situations (e.g., Evans et al., 2004) and information on contingency management (e.g., Schramm et al., 2016; Tamm et al., 2017; Koenigs et al., 2019) or behavior management (e.g., Tamm et al., 2017). Teacher training or consultation addressed behavior management strategies (e.g., The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; Owens et al., 2005; Friedman et al., 2019) such as contingency management (e.g., Miranda et al., 2006; Schramm et al., 2016) or Daily Report Cards (Friedman et al., 2019) as well as psychoeducation (e.g., Owens et al., 2005; Miranda et al., 2006).

Most of the computerized interventions were used to improve aspects of the working memory. For example, Egeland et al. (2013) implemented an adaptive working memory training that consisted of different tasks, which aimed to address the auditory-verbal (e.g., letter or digit spans) and visual–spatial working memory (e.g., remembering positions of objects). The difficulty of tasks was adjusted to meet the working memory capacity of each participant. In total, 13 different tasks were trained. Participants received daily feedback on their achievement and could earn a digital reward. Almost identical approaches are described by Gray et al. (2012) and Chacko et al. (2014).

3.4. Causal assumptions

Across all publications, five groups of causal assumptions were identified. The scenario of LDif leading to BP (scenario 1) was described in 10.41% of the included studies. In addition, the majority of studies assumed that BP would compromise learning (scenario 2) (45.83%). A part of the studies that were included assumed that LDif and BP have a reciprocal relationship and influence each other (scenario 3) (8.33%). Some studies identified a third variable [e.g., executive functions (Paananen et al., 2018)] that has an impact on both areas (scenario 4) (14.58%). 20.83% of the studies further described an increased risk of having difficulties in the two areas without specifying the causal direction or just contained unclear information on the causal assumptions (scenario 5).

4. Discussion

The aim of this review was to give an overview of the body of research in intervention research that addresses learning difficulties as well as behavioral problems. These insights seem to be of relevance with regard to the importance of specific interventions for this group of students.

Overall, the scoping review at hand identified a broad variety of studies. This variety did not solely manifest in the competencies that were addressed, but also in the approaches, the target group and the underlying causal assumptions. This observation seems plausible against the background that co-occurring learning difficulties and behavioral problems can manifest in many different ways, resulting in an infinite number of potential variations of interventions that combine both target areas. Despite the underlying heterogeneity of the included studies, it has been possible to identify overarching patterns within the studies. These can provide important starting points for further research.

The studies included indicate that co-occurring LDif and BP are of relevance across the entire childhood and adolescence. Despite the limitation, that we decided to focus on school-aged participants, it became clear that all school-levels were addressed. A focus however laid on the transition from childhood to adolescence as well as the first years of adolescence. This might be discussed against the background of additional insights from previous research that indicates that prevalence estimates of BP increase with the onset of adolescence (e.g., Avenevoli et al., 2008; Danielson et al., 2018; Steinsbekk et al., 2022), which might be reflected in the increased numbers of interventions in that age group. Interventions took place in and out of school.

Additionally, some of the interventions are rather complex, both in terms of the components as well as in terms of the duration of the intervention, which stresses the potential complexity of addressing co-occurring problems. Multiple studies include parent and/or teacher training, which shows that not only variables on the child-level were addressed. Parent training can impact the parenting style (environmental level) and teacher training might influence the instructional level, which depict important variables in the explanation of LDif and BP.

On child-level, most studies addressed academic and/or social/behavioral skills. Only few studies focused general variables explaining both phenomena (e.g., self-regulation skills). Regarding BP, many studies applied and combined Daily Report Cards or variations of contingency systems. Both are well investigated, empirically evaluated and effective methods to induce behavior change (e.g., Vannest et al., 2010; Soares et al., 2016). Furthermore, most of the studies using a DRC procedure aimed to involve parents as well, which is in line with results of the meta-analysis by Vannest et al. (2010), who stress that this is an important factor to ensure the effectiveness of such interventions.

In the area of LDif, arithmetic difficulties are underrepresented. This is supported by argumentations that research on developmental dyscalculia appears to be less popular than research on dyslexia (e.g., Kaufmann et al., 2013). Similarly, Visser et al. (2019) argue that research on comorbidity of dyscalculia and BP is limited. These findings underline the importance of gaining more insights into the co-occurrence of arithmetic difficulties and behavior problems in order to develop effective interventions for affected students; e.g., by considering variables such as anxiety in mathematics (e.g., Balt et al., 2022).

One of the most noticeable results of this review is the proportion of articles, which included a target group with ADHD or attention problems. Multiple explanations might have caused this finding. Firstly, LDif and attention problems co-occur on a regular basis. For example, DuPaul et al. (2013) reviewed multiple studies that investigated the comorbidity between LDif and ADHD and found comorbidity rates between 31 and 45%. Thus, appropriate interventions targeting both areas might be highly relevant in educational practice and research. A second explanation might be seen in the fact that attention processes seem to play a crucial part in the interplay of LDif and BP. In this regard, Barriga et al. (2002) found that the relationship between problem behavior and academic underachievement was mediated by attention. Similar, Willcutt and Pennington (2000) described that ADHD functions as a mediator between reading difficulties and oppositional disorder as well as conduct disorder.

However, the frequency of ADHD in the studies included might potentially reflect a limitation of the search approach applied. The inclusion of ADH* and attention in the search syntax could have led to an overrepresentation of studies including a sample with ADHD. The number of studies focusing participants with ADHD could have further influenced at least two findings of this review. Boys are more often diagnosed with ADHD (e.g., Huss et al., 2008), which might have caused the gender imbalance across all included studies. To a similar extend, the high number of ADHD-related studies might have influenced the pattern of causal assumptions that became clear across this review. ADHD symptoms were rated as form of BP, so it was assumed that interventions targeting ADHD related behaviors might have a subsequent impact on LDif.

In some studies, it became clear that the presence of comorbid difficulties prior to the intervention affected the effectiveness of the applied intervention. Friedman et al. (2019) found that students with ADHD and LDif benefit less from a behavioral treatment than students with ADHD alone. Gray et al. (2012) describe that students with comorbid LDif and ADHD profit less from a working memory training if they are rated as being more inattentive and hyperactive. These results are in line with results by Rabiner et al. (2004), who found no effect of reading tutoring on the reading skills of children with reading difficulties and attention problems. Children with difficulties in only one area showed moderate to substantial improvements. Concerning arithmetic difficulties a similar effect was found by Herzog and Casale (2022), who described that students with co-occurring internalizing or externalizing BP benefited less from a computerized math intervention. In addition, externalizing BP led to the least intervention effects.

Across all included studies, it became clear that most studies assumed a one-directional or reciprocal relationship between LDif and BP and vice versa. Only few studies focused common explanatory variables of LDif and BP. Future studies should consider such variables in more detail. Some potentially relevant factors on child-level, such as emotions (e.g., Pekrun et al., 2017) and their regulation (e.g., Kopelman-Rubin et al., 2020) were barely considered. At the same time, common explanatory variables might also be located on environmental level (e.g., socio-economic background, parenting style) or institutional level (e.g., instructional quality). It is not surprising that such variables were not addressed in the identified studies, as variables of social inequalities manifest themselves in manifold ways and can hardly be addressed in the context of short-term interventions. Aspects of social and institutional inequalities appear to be key common variables in explaining LDif and BP. This effect should not be underestimated by purely focusing on the identified target processes in this review. Addressing and considering institutional inequalities in the context of LDif and BP remains a key challenge for practitioners and researchers.

4.1. Limitations

The results of this study need to be discussed against the background of major limitations. Firstly, the decisions concerning the search syntax applied as well as data bases searched might have influenced the body and type of studies that were found. The same applies for the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Especially the decision to include studies in which the sample only had difficulties in one of the target areas prior to the intervention led to the inclusion of a range of studies in which the second area of interest was only briefly included (e.g., as potential transfer effects). Although the assumption of potential transfer effects somehow reflects an underlying hypothesis of a relevant relation between constructs, such studies do not fall within the original narrow interest of the review. Nevertheless, we find that relevant studies provide some guidance for the design of effective support in both areas (LDif and BP).

Secondly, a major limitation is the decision that the main part of data extraction was carried out by only one person. Despite the use of single choice options, piloting of the data extraction template, additional coding by a student assistant of a share of all documents, subsequent discussion of disagreements, and adaption of the extraction template, the extracted information remains subjective. This becomes particularly clear as the resulting interrater agreement after the piloting of the extraction template was comparatively low. Hence, the extracted information needs to be interpreted carefully, as reliability might not be adequate. In addition, it became clear that it was not always possible to distinguish between dependent variables that target LDif and such that target BP. It was decided to code those variables in a third category, which comprised constructs related to both target areas (e.g., on- and off-task behavior). This might have led to an overlap with variables in the other primary categories and consequently complicated the comparison of results. A relevant portion of articles did not provide all relevant information so that the information on the implemented programs might not be complete. This could have led to an additional bias in the extracted information.

4.2. Conclusion and implications for research and practice

The findings of this review demonstrate the complexity of dealing with the co-occurrence of LDif and BP in practice and research. A variety of interventions and approaches were identified in the findings. Hence, LDif and BP reflect a variety of difficulties that do not call for a ‘one size fits all’ approach. This has implications for research and practice. In practice, teachers need to become aware of the causal assumptions that guide their decision making in designing interventions. Only if there is a fit between causal assumption and factual reality, sustainable effects can be expected. Our results indicate different interventional approaches. Firstly, most studies assume that LDif and BP are reciprocally or unidirectionally related which may be associated with models of a single deficit. Such assumptions call teachers to address one domain in a first step to influence the second domain of interest. Secondly, given the more recent ideas of multiple deficit models, addressing overlapping variables to influence LDif and BP may be another promising approach for teachers in practice. However, the results of our scoping review indicate, that so far, only the minority of studies addresses such overarching competencies. Combining academic and behavioral elements in an intervention might be a third approach, however identifying the active components of such multimodal intervention programs additionally asks for future dismantling research designs. Future intervention research should address this gap by extending the body of research on the usefulness of potential overlapping explanations (e.g., working memory, self-regulation skills).

In addition, it became clear that certain facets were underrepresented. Arithmetical competencies were only addressed in a handful of studies. Similar, interventions for students with internalizing behavior problems are lacking. Additionally, information on how other risk factors like social economical disadvantages influence the effect of such interventions are missing. Future research might also focus variables on an environment level (e.g., socio-economic background, parenting style) or institutional level that might impact both LDif and BP, as they, depending on the conception of disability applied, play a crucial role in the development of disability/special needs.

Despite the lack of clarity in the results, this review of existing interventions might nonetheless serve as an orientation for the future development of appropriate interventions that target students with co-occurring LDif and BP.

Author contributions

TG: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation and Formal analysis. MB-R: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The publication of this article was funded by the Open Access Fund of Leibniz Universität Hannover.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Anna-Lena Vetter and especially Julia Gelhaus for their support during the screening process and the piloting of the data extraction sheet.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adams, M. J., Bereiter, C., Carruthers, I., Case, R., Hirshberg, J., McKeough, A., et al. (2000). Open court reading. SRA/McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Algozzine, B., Wang, C., and Violette, A. S. (2011). Reexamining the relationship between academic achievement and social behavior. J. Posit. Behav. Interv. 13, 3–16. doi: 10.1177/1098300709359084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., and Gordon, M. (2012). Cognitive behavioral treatment outcomes in adolescent ADHD. Focus 10, 334–345. doi: 10.1176/appi.focus.10.3.334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avenevoli, S., Knight, E., Kessler, R. C., and Merikangas, K. R. (2008). “Epidemiology of depression in children and adolescents” in Handbook of depression in children and adolescents. eds. J. R. Z. Abela and B. L. Hankin (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 6–32.

Google Scholar

Balt, M., Börnert-Ringleb, M., and Orbach, L. (2022). Reducing math anxiety in school children: a systematic review of intervention research. Front. Educ. 7:798516. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.798516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barriga, A. Q., Doran, J. W., Newell, S. R., Morrison, E. M., Barbetti, V., and Robbins, B. D. (2002). Relationships between problem behaviors and academic achievement in adolescents: the unique role of attention problems. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 10, 233–240. doi: 10.1177/10634266020100040501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Barry, L. M., and Messer, J. J. (2003). A practical application of self-management for students diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Posit. Behav. Interv., 5, 238–248. doi: 10.1177/10983007030050040701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benassi, E., Camia, M., Giovagnoli, S., and Scorza, M. (2022). Impaired school well-being in children with specific learning disorder and its relationship to psychopathological symptoms. Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 37, 74–88. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2020.1842975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyer, B. E., Kuin, M., and an der Oord, S. (2014). Zelf Oplossingen Bedenken [Solution Focused Treatment]. Uitgeverij Lannoo-Campus.

Google Scholar

*Boyer, B. E., Geurts, H. M., Prins, P. J. M., and Van Der Oord, S. (2015). Two novel CBTs for adolescents with ADHD: the value of planning skills. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 24, 1075–1090. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0661-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., and Schweitzer, J. (2009). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics 123, 1472–1476. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büttner, G., and Hasselhorn, M. (2011). Learning disabilities: debates on definitions, causes, subtypes, and responses. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 58, 75–87. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2011.548476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Carboni, J. A., Roach, A. T., and Fredrick, L. D. (2013). Impact of mindfulness training on the behavior of elementary students with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder. Res. Hum. Dev., 10, 234–251. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2013.818487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Casale, G., Husakovic, M., Hagen, T., Hövel, D. C., Krull, J., and Spilles, M. (2017). Effekte eines kognitiv-behavioralen Aufsatztrainings auf die Schreibleistung und das Lern- und Arbeitsverhalten bei Schülern mit ADHS in der Sekundarstufe I einer Förderschule [effects of a cognitive-behavioral based essay training on the writing skills and the academically engaged behavior of secondary level students with ADHD in a special school]. Empirische Sonderpädagogik, 9, 341–364. doi: 10.25656/01:15389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Chacko, A., Bedard, A. C., Marks, D. J., Feirsen, N., Uderman, J. Z., Chimiklis, A., et al. (2014). A randomized clinical trial of Cogmed working memory training in school-age children with ADHD: a replication in a diverse sample using a control condition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 55, 247–255. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, J. W. (1988). Learning disabled children’s self-concepts. Rev. Educ. Res. 58, 347–371. doi: 10.3102/00346543058003347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Child, A. E., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Willcutt, E. G., and Fuchs, L. S. (2019). A cognitive dimensional approach to understanding shared and unique contributions to Reading, math, and attention skills. J. Learn. Disabil. 52, 15–30. doi: 10.1177/0022219418775115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commodari, E. (2012). Attention skills and risk of developing learning difficulties. Curr. Psychol. 31, 17–34. doi: 10.1007/s12144-012-9128-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Ghandour, R. M., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., and Blumberg, S. J. (2018). Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among U.S. children and adolescents, 2016. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 199–212. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson and Associates. (1997). Math Blaster Ages 6-9. Knowledge Adventure.

Google Scholar

Davidson and Associates (1999). Math Blaster. Knowledge Adventure.

Google Scholar

Dishion, T., and Kavanagh, K (2003). Intervening in adolescent problem behavior: a family-centered approach. Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

De Neve, D., Devos, G., and Tuytens, M. (2015). The importance of job resources and self-efficacy for beginning teachers’ professional learning in differentiated instruction. Teach. Teach. Educ. 47, 30–41. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, S., and Montague, M. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder comorbid with emotional and behavioral disorders and learning disabilities in adolescents. Exceptionality 14, 19–33. doi: 10.1207/s15327035ex1401_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., Eckert, T. L., and Vilardo, B. (2012). The effects of school-based interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a Meta-analysis 1996–2010. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 41, 387–412. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087496

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., Gormley, M. J., and Laracy, S. D. (2013). Comorbidity of LD and ADHD: implications of DSM-5 for assessment and treatment. J. Learn. Disabil. 46, 43–51. doi: 10.1177/0022219412464351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Dursun, P., Fidan, U., and Karayagiz, S. (2021). Probable role of listening therapy in the management of ADHD symptoms: three case studies. Curr. Psychol., 40, 4219–4234. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01419-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Egeland, J., Aarlien, A. K., and Saunes, B.-K. (2013). Few effects of far transfer of working memory training in ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One, 8,:e75660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Elias, L. C. D. S., Marturano, E. M., Motta, A. M. D. A., and Giurlani, A. G. (2003). Treating boys with low school achievement and behavior problems: comparison of two kinds of intervention. Psychol. Rep., 92, 105–116. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.92.1.105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennis, R. P., Evanovich, L. L., Losinski, M., Jolivette, K., and Kimball-Greb, K. (2018). “Behavioral, academic, and social characteristics of students with behavioral difficulties served in a residential facility” in Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities. eds. T. J. Landrum, B. G. Cook, and M. Tankersley, vol. 30 (Bingley: Emerald Publishing Limited), 93–111.

Google Scholar

Evans, S. W. (2001). The Challenging Horizons Program treatment manual. Unpublished manuscript.

Google Scholar

*Evans, S. W., Axelrod, J., and Langberg, J. M. (2004). Efficacy of a school-based treatment program for middle school youth with ADHD: pilot data. Behav. Modif., 28, 528–547. doi: 10.1177/0145445503259504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Evans, S. W., Langberg, J., Raggi, V., Allen, J., and Buvinger, E. C. (2005). Development of a school-based treatment program for middle school youth with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord., 9, 343–353. doi: 10.1177/1087054705279305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., Schultz, B. K., Vaughn, A., Altaye, M., Marshall, S. A., et al. (2016). Evaluation of a school-based treatment program for young adolescents with ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 84, 15–30. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Evans, S. W., Schultz, B. K., DeMars, C. E., and Davis, H. (2011). Effectiveness of the challenging horizons after-school program for young adolescents with ADHD. Behav. Ther., 42, 462–474. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Everatt, J., Al-Sharhan, A., Al-Azmi, Y., Al-Menaye, N., and Elbeheri, G. (2011). Behavioural/attentional problems and literacy learning difficulties in children from non-English language/cultural backgrounds. Support Learn., 26, 127–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9604.2011.01491.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiano, G. A., Schatz, N. K., Aloe, A. M., Pelham, W. E., Smyth, A. C., Zhao, X., et al. (2021). Comprehensive Meta-analysis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder psychosocial treatments investigated within between group studies. Rev. Educ. Res. 91, 718–760. doi: 10.3102/00346543211025092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Fabiano, G. A., Vujnovic, R. K., Pelham, W. E., Waschbusch, D. A., Massetti, G. M., Pariseau, M. E., et al. (2010). Enhancing the effectiveness of special education programming for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using a daily report card. Sch. Psychol. Rev., 39, 219–239. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2010.12087775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Freilich, R., and Shechtman, Z. (2010). The contribution of art therapy to the social, emotional, and academic adjustment of children with learning disabilities. Arts Psychother., 37, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2010.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Friedman, L. M., Mcburnett, K., Dvorsky, M. R., Hinshaw, S. P., and Pfiffner, L. J. (2019). Learning disorder confers setting-specific treatment resistance for children with ADHD, predominantly inattentive presentation. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol., 49, 854–867. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2019.1644647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. G., and Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-assisted learning strategies: Making classrooms more responsive to diversity. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 174–206. doi: 10.3102/00028312034001174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaser, C., and Palm, D. (2014). Aufsatztraining für 4. bis 6. Klassen. Ein Lehrermanual mit Unterrichtsmaterialien zur Förderung von Schreibkompetenz und Arbeitsverhalten [Essay training for 4th to 6th grade. A teacher\u0027s manual with teaching materials to promote writing competence and work behavior.]. Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Gadeyne, E., Ghesquiere, P., and Onghena, P. (2004). Psychosocial functioning of young children with learning problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 510–521. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00241.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Gray, S. A., Chaban, P., Martinussen, R., Goldberg, R., Gotlieb, H., Kronitz, R., et al. (2012). Effects of a computerized working memory training program on working memory, attention, and academics in adolescents with severe LD and comorbid ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 53, 1277–1284. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02592.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigorenko, E. L., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Wagner, R. K., Willcutt, E. G., and Fletcher, J. M. (2020). Understanding, educating, and supporting children with specific learning disabilities: 50 years of science and practice. Am. Psychol. 75, 37–51. doi: 10.1037/amp0000452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hechtman, L., Abikoff, H., Klein, R. G., Weiss, G., Respitz, C., Kouri, J., et al. (2004). Academic achievement and emotional status of children with ADHD treated with long-term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 43, 812–819. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000128796.84202.eb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herzog, M., and Casale, G. (2022). The effects of a computer-based mathematics intervention in primary school students with and without emotional and behavioral difficulties. Int. Electron. J. Elem. Educ. 3, 303–317. doi: 10.26822/iejee.2022.246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horbach, J., Mayer, A., Scharke, W., Heim, S., and Günther, T. (2020). Development of behavior problems in children with and without specific learning disorders in reading and spelling from kindergarten to fifth grade. Sci. Stud. Read. 24, 57–71. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2019.1641504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huss, M., Hölling, H., Kurth, B.-M., and Schlack, R. (2008). How often are German children and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD? Prevalence based on the judgment of health care professionals: results of the German health and examination survey (KiGGS). Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 17, 52–58. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-1006-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingesson, S. G. (2007). Growing up with dyslexia: interviews with teenagers and young adults. Sch. Psychol. Int. 28, 574–591. doi: 10.1177/0143034307085659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, L., Mazzocco, M. M., Dowker, A., Von Aster, M., Göbel, S. M., Grabner, R. H., et al. (2013). Dyscalculia from a developmental and differential perspective. Front. Psychol. 4:516. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavale, K. A., and Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and learning disabilities: a Meta-analysis. J. Learn. Disabil. 29, 226–237. doi: 10.1177/002221949602900301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavale, K. A., and Forness, S. R. (2000). What definitions of learning disability say and don’t say: a critical analysis. J. Learn. Disabil. 33, 239–256. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Keller, E. R., and Brunstein, J. C. (2022). Kombiniertes Aufmerksamkeits- und Schreibtraining bei Förderschulkindern [combining attention training with writing strategy instruction in special education children]. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 11, 63–77. doi: 10.1024/2235-0977/a000366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiel, E., Braun, A., Muckenthaler, M., Heimlich, U., and Weiss, S. (2020). Self-efficacy of teachers in inclusive classes. How do teachers with different self-efficacy beliefs differ in implementing inclusion? Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 35, 333–349. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2019.1683685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

KMK. (2020). Sonderpädagogische Förderung in Schulen 2009 bis 2018 [special education support in schools 2009 to 2018]. Available at: https://www.kmk.org/fileadmin/Dateien/pdf/Statistik/Dokumentationen/Dok223_SoPae_2018.pdf

Google Scholar

*Koenigs, J., Schuchardt, K., and Mähler, C. (2019). Wirksamkeit eines kombinierten Lese-Rechtschreib- und Aufmerksamkeitstrainings [effectiveness of a combined training for children with reading and spelling disabilities and attention disorders]. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 8, 21–32. doi: 10.1024/2235-0977/a000248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kopelman-Rubin, D., Klomek, A. B., Al-Yagon, M., Mufson, L., Apter, A., and Mikulincer, M. (2012). Psychological intervention for adolescents diagnosed with learning disorders—"I can succeed" (ICS): treatment model, feasibility, and acceptability. J. Learn. Disabil., 1. 37–54.

Google Scholar

Kopelman-Rubin, D., Siegel, A., Weiss, N., and Kats-Gold, I. (2020). The relationship between emotion regulation, school belonging, and psychosocial difficulties among adolescents with specific learning disorder. Child. Sch. 42, 216–224. doi: 10.1093/cs/cdaa022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krämer, S., Möller, J., and Zimmermann, F. (2021). Inclusive education of students with general learning difficulties: a Meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 91, 432–478. doi: 10.3102/0034654321998072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuin, M., Boyer, B. E., and Van der Oord, S. (2013). Zelf Plannen [Plan My Life]. Uitgeverij Lannoo-Campus.

Google Scholar

*Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Becker, S. P., Girio-Herrera, E., and Vaughn, A. J. (2012). Evaluation of the homework, organization, and planning skills (HOPS) intervention for middle school students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as implemented by school mental health providers. Sch. Psychol. Rev., 41, 342–364. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Urbanowicz, C. M., Simon, J. O., and Graham, A. J. (2008). Efficacy of an organization skills intervention to improve the academic functioning of students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sch. Psychol. Q., 23, 407–417. doi: 10.1037/1045-3830.23.3.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Langberg, J. M., Smith, B. H., Bogle, K. E., Schmidt, J. D., Cole, W. R., and Pender, C. A. S. (2006). A pilot evaluation of small group challenging horizons program (CHP): a randomized trial. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol., 23, 31–58. doi: 10.1300/J370v23n01_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langberg, J. M., Vaughn, A. J., Williamson, P., Epstein, J. N., Girio-Herrera, E., and Becker, S. P. (2011). Refinement of an Organizational Skills Intervention for Adolescents with ADHD for Implementation by School Mental Health Providers. School Mental Health, 3, 143–155. doi: 10.1007/s12310-011-9055-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauth, G. W., and Schlottke, P. F. (2009). Training mit aufmerksamkeitsgestörten Kindern (6. Aufl). [Training with Attention-Disordered Children (6th ed.)]. Beltz.

Google Scholar

Linderkamp, F., Hennig, T., and Schramm, S. A. (2011). ADHS bei Jugendlichen: Das Lerntraining LeJA [ADHD in adolescence: The learning skills training for adolescents with ADHD]. Beltz PVU.

Google Scholar

Lovett, M. W., Borden, S. L., DeLuca, T., Lacerenza, L., Benson, N. J., and Brackstone, D. (1994). Treating the core deficits of developmental dyslexia: Evidence of transfer of learning after phonologically- and strategy-based reading training programs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 805–822. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovett, M. W., Lacerenza, L., and Borden, S. L. (2000). Putting struggling readers on the PHAST track: A program to integrate phonological and strategy-based remedial reading instruction and maximize outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 458–476. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lee, Y.-Y., Sugai, G., and Horner, R. H. (1999). Using an instructional intervention to reduce problem and off-task behaviors. J. Posit. Behav. Interv., 1, 195–204. doi: 10.1177/109830079900100402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenhard, W., Lenhard, A., and Meyer, L. (2013). “Learning difficulties” in Oxford bibliographies in education (Oxford: Oxford University Press)

Google Scholar

MacKay, T. (2009). Severe and complex learning difficulties: issues of definition, classification and prevalence. Educ. Child Psychol. 26, 9–18. doi: 10.53841/bpsecp.2009.26.4.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maehler, C., and Schuchardt, K. (2011). Working memory in children with learning disabilities: rethinking the criterion of discrepancy. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 58, 5–17. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2011.547335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mähler, C. (2020). Diagnostik von Lernstörungen: Zeit zum Umdenken [diagnosis of specific learning disorders – time to rethink]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie 35, 217–227. doi: 10.1024/1010-0652/a000291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malinauskiene, O., Vosylis, R., and Zukauskiene, R. (2011). Longitudinal examination of relationships between problem behaviors and academic achievement in young adolescents. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 15, 3415–3421. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.04.311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Riley, J. R., et al. (2005). Developmental cascades: linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Dev. Psychol. 41, 733–746. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mautone, J. A., DuPaul, G. J., and Jitendra, A. K. (2005). The effects of computer-assisted instruction on the mathematics performance and classroom behavior of children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord., 9, 301–312. doi: 10.1177/1087054705278832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mautone, J. A., Marshall, S. A., Sharman, J., Eiraldi, R. B., Jawad, A. F., and Power, T. J. (2012). Development of a family–school intervention for young children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sch. Psychol. Rev., 41, 447–466. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., and Crowell, E. W. (2000). Learning disabilities and ADHD: overlapping spectrum disorders. J. Learn. Disabil. 33, 417–424. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLeod, J. D., Uemura, R., and Rohrman, S. (2012). Adolescent mental health, behavior problems, and academic achievement. J. Health Soc. Behav. 53, 482–497. doi: 10.1177/0022146512462888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Miranda, A., Jarque, S., and Rosel, J. (2006). Treatment of children with ADHD: Psychopedagogical program at school versus psychostimulant medication. Psicothema, 18, 335–341

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and Meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, K., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., Bruder, J., and Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Specific learning disorder: prevalence and gender differences. PLoS One 9:e103537. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, K., Snowling, M. J., and Hulme, C. (2020). Introduction to the special issue “comorbidities between reading disorders and other developmental disorders”. Sci. Stud. Read. 24, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2019.1702045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Murphy, J. M., Hawkins, R. O., and Nabors, L. (2019). Combining social skills instruction and the good behavior game to support students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Contemp. Sch. Psychol., 24, 228–238. doi: 10.1007/s40688-019-00226-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neale, M. C., and Kendler, K. S. (1995). Models of comorbidity for multifactorial disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 57, 935–953.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., Lane, K., and Smith, B. W. (2004). Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Except. Child. 71, 59–73. doi: 10.1177/001440290407100104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. M., and Harwood, H. (2011). Learning disabilities and anxiety: a Meta-analysis. J. Learn. Disabil. 44, 3–17. doi: 10.1177/0022219409359939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohtani, K., and Hisasaka, T. (2018). Beyond intelligence: a meta-analytic review of the relationship among metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. Metacogn. Learn. 13, 179–212. doi: 10.1007/s11409-018-9183-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ota, K. R., and DuPaul, G. J. (2002). Task engagement and mathematics performance in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of supplemental computer instruction. Sch. Psychol. Q., 17, 242–257. doi: 10.1521/scpq.17.3.242.20881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Owens, J. S., Girio, E. L., Himawan, L. K., and Abbott, M. Y. (2008). Implementing and evaluating evidence-based treatments in schools: successes and lessons learned. Adv. School Ment. Health Promot., 1, 39–51. doi: 10.1080/1754730X.2008.9715721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Owens, J. S., Richerson, L., Beilstein, E. A., Crane, A., Murphy, C. E., and Vancouver, J. B. (2005). School-based mental health programming for children with inattentive and disruptive behavior problems: first-year treatment outcome. J. Atten. Disord., 9, 261–274. doi: 10.1177/1087054705279299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paananen, M., Heinonen, J., Knoll, J., Leppänen, U., and Närhi, V. (2011). Kummi 8. Maltti – tarkkaavuuden ja toiminnanohjauksen ryhmakuntoutus [Maltti – Group based intervention of deficits in attention and executive functions]. NMI.

Google Scholar

Pfiffner, L. J., Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E., Zalecki, C., Kaiser, N. M., Villodas, M., et al. (2014). A two-site randomized clinical trial of integrated psychosocial treatment for ADHD-inattentive type. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 1115–1127. doi: 10.1037/a0036887

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Paananen, M., Aro, T., Närhi, V., and Aro, M. (2018). Group-based intervention on attention and executive functions in the school context. Educ. Psychol., 38, 859–876. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1407407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., and Goetz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance: longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Dev. 88, 1653–1670. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, B. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 101, 385–413. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. L., Boada, R., McGrath, L. M., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., and Pennington, B. F. (2017). Cognitive prediction of reading, math, and attention: shared and unique influences. J. Learn. Disabil. 50, 408–421. doi: 10.1177/0022219415618500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Power, T. J., Mautone, J. A., Soffer, S. L., Clarke, A. T., Marshall, S. A., Sharman, J., et al. (2012). A family–school intervention for children with ADHD: results of a randomized clinical trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 80, 611–623. doi: 10.1037/a0028188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiner, D. L., Carrig, M. M., and Dodge, K. A. (2016). Attention problems and academic achievement: do persistent and earlier-emerging problems have more adverse long-term effects? J. Atten. Disord. 20, 946–957. doi: 10.1177/1087054713507974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiner, D. L., and Malone, P. S., Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (2004). The impact of tutoring on early reading achievement for children with and without attention problems. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 32, 273–284. doi: 10.1023/B:JACP.0000026141.20174.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rabiner, D. L., Murray, D. W., Skinner, A. T., and Malone, P. S. (2010). A randomized trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with attention difficulties. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 38, 131–142. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9353-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Roberts, G., Rane, S., Fall, A.-M., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., and Vaughn, S. (2015). The impact of intensive reading intervention on level of attention in middle school students. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol., 44, 942–953. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.913251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rock, E. E., Fessler, M. A., and Church, R. P. (1997). The concomitance of learning disabilities and emotional/behavioral disorders: a conceptual model. J. Learn. Disabil. 30, 245–263. doi: 10.1177/002221949703000302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, D. A., Harrison, J. R., Vannest, K. J., and McClelland, S. S. (2016). Effect Size for Token Economy Use in Contemporary Classroom Settings: A Meta-Analysis of Single-Case Research. School Psychology Review, 45, 379–399. doi: 10.17105/SPR45-4.379-399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safren, S. A., Otto, M. W., Sprich, S., Winett, C. L., Wilens, T. E., and Biederman, J. (2005). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for ADHD in medication-treated adults with continued symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 831–842. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sainio, P. J., Eklund, K. M., Ahonen, T. P. S., and Kiuru, N. H. (2019). The role of learning difficulties in adolescents’ academic emotions and academic achievement. J. Learn. Disabil. 52, 287–298. doi: 10.1177/0022219419841567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schieltz, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Suess, A. N., Graber, J. E., Lustig, N. H., and Detrick, J. (2020). Evaluating the effects of positive reinforcement, instructional strategies, and negative reinforcement on problem behavior and academic performance: an experimental analysis. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. doi: 10.1007/s10882-019-09696-y, 32, 339–363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schramm, S. A., Hennig, T., and Linderkamp, F. (2016). Training problem solving and organizational skills in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 15, 391–411. doi: 10.1891/1945-8959.15.3.391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schultz, B. K., Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., and Schoemann, A. M. (2017). Outcomes for adolescents who comply with long-term psychosocial treatment for ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 85, 250–261. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Shalev, L., Tsal, Y., and Mevorach, C. (2007). Computerized progressive attentional training (CPAT) program: effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol., 13, 382–388. doi: 10.1080/09297040600770787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shure, M. B. (1992). I can problem solve (ICPS): An Interpersonal Cognitive Problem Solving Program. Research Press.

Google Scholar

*Sibley, M. H., Graziano, P. A., Kuriyan, A. B., Coxe, S., Pelham, W. E., Rodriguez, L., et al. (2016). Parent–teen behavior therapy + motivational interviewing for adolescents with ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 84, 699–712. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Sibley, M. H., Pelham, W. E., Derefinko, K. J., Kuriyan, A. B., Sanchez, F., and Graziano, P. A. (2013). A pilot trial of supporting teens’ academic needs daily (STAND): a parent-adolescent collaborative intervention for ADHD. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess., 35, 436–449. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9353-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sideridis, G. D. (2009). “Motivation and learning disabilities-past, present, and future” in Handbook of motivation at school. eds. K. Wentzel and D. B. Miele (London: Routledge), 605–625.

Google Scholar

Soffer, S. L., and Power, T. J. (2005). Treatment manual for Family-School Success: A family-school intervention program for children with ADHD in grades 2 to 6. Unpublished manual.

Google Scholar

Steinsbekk, S., Ranum, B., and Wichstrøm, L. (2022). Prevalence and course of anxiety disorders and symptoms from preschool to adolescence: a 6-wave community study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 63, 527–534. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tamm, L., Denton, C. A., Epstein, J. N., Schatschneider, C., Taylor, H., Arnold, L. E., et al. (2017). Comparing treatments for children with ADHD and word reading difficulties: a randomized clinical trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 85, 434–446. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tannock, R., Frijters, J. C., Martinussen, R., White, E. J., Ickowicz, A., Benson, N. J., et al. (2018). Combined modality intervention for ADHD with comorbid reading disorders: a proof of concept study. J. Learn. Disabil., 51, 55–72. doi: 10.1177/0022219416678409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*The MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 56, 1073–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

UN General Assembly. (2006). Convention on the rights of persons with disabilities. UN General Assembly New York, NY.

Google Scholar

Vannest, K. J., Davis, J. L., Davis, C. R., Mason, B. A., and Burke, M. D. (2010). Effective intervention for behavior with a daily behavior report card: a Meta-analysis. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 39, 654–672. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2010.12087748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veritas Health Innovation. (2022). Covidence systematic review software [computer software]. Veritas Health Innovation. Available at: www.covidence.org

Google Scholar

Visser, L., Büttner, G., and Hasselhorn, M. (2019). Komorbidität spezifischer Lernstörungen und psychischer Auffälligkeiten: Ein Literaturüberblick [comorbidity between specific learning disabilities and psychopathological problems: a state of the art review]. Lernen und Lernstörungen 8, 7–20. doi: 10.1024/2235-0977/a000246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, L., Kalmar, J., Linkersdörfer, J., Görgen, R., Rothe, J., Hasselhorn, M., et al. (2020). Comorbidities between specific learning disorders and psychopathology in elementary school children in Germany. Front. Psych. 11:292. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter, D., Rademacher, C., Schürmann, S., and Döpfner, M. (2006). “SELBST – Ein Therapieprogramm zur Behandlungvon Jugendlichen mit Selbstwert-, Leistungs- und Beziehungsstörungen, vol 1: Grundlagen der Selbstmanagementtherapie [SELBST: A program for the treatment of adolescents with problems in self-esteem, activity and affect, achievement, and relationships, vol. 1: Basics of self-management therapy]” in SELBST – Ein Therapieprogramm für Jugendliche mit Selbstwert-, Leistungs- und Beziehungsstörungen [SELBST: A program for the treatment of adolescents with problems in self-esteem, activity and affect, achievement, and relationships]. Eds. M. Döpfner, D. Walter, C. Rademacher, and S. Schürmann (Göttingen, Hogrefe).

Google Scholar

*Walter, D., and Döpfner, M. (2006). Die Behandlung von Jugendlichen mit Leistungsstörungen mit dem SELBST-Programm—Kurzzeiteffekte [Treatment of adolescents with academic underachievement using the SELBST Program—Short term effects]. Verhaltenstherapie, 16, 257–265. doi: 10.1159/000095553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wehby, J. H., Falk, K. B., Barton-Arwood, S., Lane, K. L., and Cooley, C. (2003). The impact of comprehensive reading instruction on the academic and social behavior of students with emotional and behavioral disorders. J. Emot. Behav. Disord., 11, 225–238. doi: 10.1177/10634266030110040401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., and Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 1039–1048. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2023). International classification of diseases 11th revision. Available at: https://icd.who.int/en

Google Scholar

Keywords: learning difficulties, behavioral problems, intervention, literature review, school – aged children

Citation: Gabriel T and Börnert-Ringleb M (2023) The intersection of learning difficulties and behavior problems – a scoping review of intervention research. Front. Educ. 8:1268904. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1268904

Received: 28 July 2023; Accepted: 11 October 2023;
Published: 30 October 2023.

Edited by:

Robert Volpe, Northeastern University, United States

Reviewed by:

Mack Burke, Baylor University, United States
George DuPaul, Lehigh University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Gabriel and Börnert-Ringleb. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taina Gabriel, taina.gabriel@ifs.uni-hannover.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.