Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 15 September 2023
Sec. Educational Psychology

Cooperative and competitive school climate: their impact on sense of belonging across cultures

  • Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

The role of sense of belonging within an educational context has been well-documented. Previous studies on belongingness using OECD-PISA data showed that the degree to which students feel a sense of belongingness to their school varies across countries. The current study extents the prior findings by analyzing the relationship between the sense of belonging and perceived competitiveness and cooperation in schools using the 2018 PISA cycle, including data from 76 countries. Perceived cooperation across all countries and aggregated on the country level was significantly and positively correlated with belongingness. Belongingness regressed on perceived competitiveness and perceived cooperativity results in both variables being significant predictors in the expected directions. Together, the two climate variables explained 18.8% of the variance on belongingness between countries. Thus, there is robust cross-cultural evidence that sense of belongingness is negatively affected by a competitive and positively by a cooperative classroom climate.

Introduction

The motivation to form and maintain social relationships appears to be universal for humans. Baumeister and Leary (1995) conceptualized it as a fundamental motivation in the sense that goal-oriented behaviors are carried out to satisfy a need to create and maintain close social relations. Accordingly, successfully establishing meaningful social connections has positive effects on mental health and development (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Moeller et al., 2020; Watts and Thrasher, 2023).

For adolescents, the school setting plays an important role in cultivating feelings of belongingness, which has implications for healthy development and successful learning (Eccles and Roeser, 2011; Gosnell, 2020; Kathuria and Pandya, 2020). School belongingness, often understood as the feelings of connectedness that students feel towards the school community, is positively associated with self-efficacy and learning motivation which, in turn, are associated with better academic performance (Midgley et al., 1989; Goodenow, 1993; Datu and Valdez, 2019). Xie et al. (2022) monitored the change in motivation of high schoolers across the span of 2 years, and found that the feeling of belongingness, along with prior academic achievement, has a positive impact on student learning motivation.

Having positive attitudes towards peer groups and perceiving the learning environment as caring are factors that relate to the degree to which students feel connected with their school environment (Battistich et al., 1997; Ryzin et al., 2007). Belongingness is also discussed as a protective factor against developmental risk, such as substance abuse, delinquency, and depression (Anderman, 2002; Napoli et al., 2003; Baskin et al., 2010; Arslan, 2021). Sense of belongingness in an academic setting has also been shown to have implications for students’ decisions to enter health professions and STEM fields, where lower levels of belongingness are attributed with lower levels of interest and engagement in these fields (Tellhed et al., 2016; Vivekananda-Schmidt and Sandars, 2018).

Belongingness across cultures

According to previous research, culture plays an important role in shaping school belongingness (Kumar and Maehr, 2010; Crul, 2018). Sense of belongingness has been reported to be lower for immigrants and racial minority groups compared to groups from the majority culture (Battistich and Hom, 1997; Johnson et al., 2007; Lardier et al., 2019).

Additionally, there is evidence that sense of belongingness varies across countries (Glass and Westmont, 2014; Seo, 2019). Cortina et al. (2017) investigated sense of belongingness cross-nationally reanalyzing data from the 2003 cycle of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) commissioned by the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). The authors documented systematic differences in average sense of belongingness in 15-year-old students across nations and explained them in part through Hofstede’s cultural dimension theory (Hofstede et al., 2010). They examined the cultural dimensions of power distance and individualism/collectivism and explored how the macro level cultural differences between East Asian and Western countries in these dimensions manifested in meso-level differences in school climate.

Individualism–collectivism is the dimension that encompasses fundamentally different ways in which individuals interact with – and position themselves within – their social context (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). This cultural factor suggests that countries with higher levels of collectivism, i.e., countries where group harmony and social unity are emphasized values, would show higher trends in overall sense of belongingness compared to countries with higher levels of individualism, where individual uniqueness is emphasized and valued to a greater extent. Cortina et al. (2017) argued that the dimension of power distance as it relates to the power dynamic between subordinates and superiors, is the predominant factor in shaping the nature of social relations within the school context. They maintained that the discrepancies in overall belongingness between East Asian and Western countries is due to the higher overall power distance and low individualism in East Asia compared to Western Europe and North America. High-power distance as a cultural factor can be observed in the more teacher-centered instructional style prevalent in East Asian cultures, while Western cultures emphasize student-centered aspects of learning (Hofstede et al., 2010). Heavy emphasis on achievement monitoring through testing also appears to be a more salient feature in East Asian cultures, which, in turn, reinforces a climate of competition between students in the form of pressure to outperform others in standardized assessments.

Cortina et al. (2017) found in fact that the average belongingness across countries followed the predicted trend: East Asian countries scored significantly lower in terms of average belongingness compared to Western countries. In accordance with their predictions, Hofstede’s power distance scores explained a significant portion of the variance in belongingness observed between the countries. Additionally, the preference for cooperation as opposed to competition in the learning environment was associated with higher levels of school belongingness at the individual level across countries. Note, however, that preference for competition/cooperation was not a direct measure of actual perceived school climate, which the authors noted as a significant limitation of their study.

Competition versus cooperation as elements of the school climate

The incorporation of competitive learning styles or pedagogical practices that encourage a competitive learning environment has been a controversial topic in the educational discourse for decades (see, e.g., Maehr and Midgley, 1996). The effects of a competitive learning environment have been well-documented in multiple domains. Some studies showed that higher achievement levels were associated with competitive learning styles if the competition was perceived as constructive (Fülöp, 2000, 2004; Williams and Sheridan, 2010). Other researchers argued that alternative motivational factors, in particular mastery orientation, are more beneficial than competition and emphasized the negative repercussions of competition as a source of motivation (Ames and Ames, 1984). For college students Bergin (1995) showed that performance on a cognitive learning task was higher in students who were motivated by mastery goals as opposed to competitive motivation, i.e., performance goals. In Lam et al.’s (2001) study, competitive motivation was associated with higher achievement on easier tasks, but often reduced students’ willingness to attempt tasks that were more challenging. Some researchers maintained that competition can be harmful when students feel that success in the learning process comes at the expense of the success of other students, in which case the learning of all parties are hindered (Johnson and Johnson, 1989, 1994; Slavin, 2000).

Additionally, Posselt and Lipson (2016) demonstrated that overemphasis of competition in the learning environment has a particularly negative impact on the psychological well-being of students from underrepresented minority groups. According to Canning et al. (2019), perceived classroom competition fosters imposter syndrome in first-generation college students accompanied by a lack of identification within their academic field which, in turn, reduced their engagement in STEM courses that are preceived as highly competitive.

On the other hand, there is a large converging body of research demonstrating the benefits of a cooperative school climate. Cooperative learning styles have been observed to contribute to higher levels of self-efficacy, learning engagement, and deep learning processes (Slavin, 2014; Keramati and Gillies, 2021). The benefits of cooperative learning methods have been demonstrated consistently across different cultures as well (Tran et al., 2019; Keramati and Gillies, 2021). Lätsch (2017) reported from a study on German secondary school students that perceived helpfulness, which was related to feelings of cooperativity between peers, had a mediating effect on the relationship between prosocial behavior and perceived stress. In addition, perceived competition exacerbated the effect of peer conflict on perceived stress. Other studies have shown that cooperative learning styles, which encouraged group coherence and positive social relations with peers, were associated with increased sense of belongingness and less instances of bullying (Osterman, 2000; Ryzin and Roseth, 2019; Ryzin et al., 2020).

Given that competitive and cooperative learning environments have important implications on the academic and psychological development of students, we argue that student perception of levels of competition and cooperation in their learning environment are critical explanatory constructs to understand cultural differences in sense of belonging in international comparative studies. Thus, examining how sense of belongingness is related to perceived climate in terms of competition and cooperation in the learning environment may provide important contributions towards our understanding of the international differences in the learning context of students from a psychological perspective. Stable cultural differences require a process of reproduction through the generations and stable institutional setting like schools play a key role as socialization agents (Wentzel, 2015).

Present study

Cortina et al. (2017) found supporting evidence for a negative association between preference for competition and sense of belongingness in Western countries, but found an opposite effect for East Asian countries, where preference for competition had a positive association with belonging. As noted by the authors, the PISA 2003 student questionnaire did not contain a scale that directly measured the perceived levels of competition in the school, and instead used as a proxy a scale that asked about preferred style of learning in mathematics with competitive or cooperative styles as options. But a preference is clearly distinct from the perception of the actual climate.

The most recent cycle of the PISA student questionnaire data (OECD, 2018) included scales that directly measure the perceived levels of competition and cooperation in the school environment and is not constrained to the context of one specific class or school subject. Instead, these scales inquire about the student’s feelings about their overall school environment, which provides a much more accurate measure for the constructs of perceived competition and cooperation in the learning context. One purpose of the current study is therefore to investigate the hypothesized school climate effect on sense of belonging using more accurate and valid measures. Additionally, a limitation of the previous study was that PISA data from a single cycle. Therefore, to monitor any changes in overall belongingness that may have occurred over the past 15 years, the present study will start with comparing global belongingness trends between the two time points. The 2018 PISA cycle also has twice the number of participating countries that have data on sense of belongingness compared to the 2003 PISA cycle, and thus we were able to test our hypotheses using a more globally encompassing data set. The present study thus has the following aims:

1. Replicating the prior analysis with respect to international differences regarding the average sense of school belonging in the population of 15-year-old students,

2. Directly assess the hypothesis that perceived competitive climate has a universal negative effect on sense of belonging, and

3. Test whether the international differences discovered in 1) are reduced or disappear entirely when the climate perception (cooperative/competitive) are controlled (mediation hypothesis).

Methods

Participants

The present study analyzed the data from the 2018 cycle of the PISA study, which sampled from 79 countries (82 sampled regions), including data from 609,673 students in 20,842 schools. The PISA study collected achievement test data and questionnaire responses of 15-year- old students in accordance with specific guidelines, and the data were weighted accordingly to be representative of the national population for each country to allow for cross-national comparisons (OECD, 2018). The average age of the participants was M = 15.79, SD = 0.291, 50.2% were male.

Sampled regions with no valid response data to the belongingness, perceived competition, or perceived cooperation scales were excluded from the analysis, bringing the total number of countries included in the analysis to 74.

Measures

PISA includes a student questionnaire on different aspects of their academic lives, including motivational and affective measures. Several scales from the PISA 2018 student survey were used as the outcome and predictor variables of this study, detailed below. Reliabilities were calculated for each of the scales used in this study separately for each country.

Belongingness scale

School belongingness as an outcome variable was measured using six statements that students rated their agreement on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree) (e.g., “My school is a place where… I feel like I belong” OECD, 2004, 2018).

The belongingness scale has an overall Cronbach’s alpha of 0.80, which indicates a high level of reliability. Cronbach’s alpha values for the belongingness scale for all countries separately exceed 0.64, indicating moderate levels of reliability. The countries with the lowest reliabilities are Morocco, France, Jordan, Kosovo, and Vietnam. Excluding these countries, Cronbach’s alpha values for the belongingness scale for all other countries exceed 0.70, indicating satisfactory to high levels of reliability.

Perceived competitiveness scale

Perception of the competitiveness of the learning environment was measured using a four-item scale with the item stem: “Think about your school: how true are the following statements” followed by four statements, e.g., “Students seems to value competition” (OECD, 2018). Students rated their response to each item on a Likert scale of 1 (not at all true) to 4 (extremely true). Cronbach’s alpha values for the perceived competitiveness scale for all countries exceed 0.73, indicating satisfactory to high levels of reliability.

Perceived cooperativity scale

Perceived cooperative climate was measured similarly to perceived competitive climate using the following four statements, e.g., “It seems that students are cooperating with each other” (OECD, 2018). Students responded to each statement using the same response scale of 1 to 4 as for the perceived competitiveness scale. Cronbach’s alpha values for the perceived cooperativity scale for all countries exceed 0.82, indicating high levels of reliability.

Data analysis

The analysis methods used to address the hypotheses of the present study are detailed below. Belongingness scores, as well as perceived competitiveness scores and perceived cooperativity scores were calculated for each country in order to perform analyses at the country level. Correlation and regression analysis of the outcome variable of belongingness and the predictor variables of perceived competitive and cooperative climate were carried out using SPSS, version 29.

Descriptive statistics

To calculate the belongingness score for each student, items 2 and 5 of the belongingness scale were recoded, and responses to all items were then averaged, giving a total belongingness score between 1 and 4, with 1 indicating the lowest level and 4 indicating the highest level of belonging. Belongingness scores were then aggregated on the country level and mean and standard deviation values for belongingness were calculated. Student data for this and all the following scales were weighted according to the PISA recommendations to approximate a random sample for 15-year-olds for each country.

The responses on the perceived competitive and cooperate climate scales were calculated similarly by averaging the item responses, generating a score for each student between 1 and 4, with 1 indicating a low level and 4 indicating a high level of perceived competition or cooperation, respectively. Both climate scales were also aggregated to the country level with mean and standard deviation values calculated for each country.

Analyses

Of the 76 countries that were included in the 2018 cycle of PISA data, 38 also participated in the 2003 PISA cycle. These countries that were present for both cycles of PISA data were ranked based on their belongingness score, and a rank correlation between the two datasets was calculated to assess the relative stability of belongingness levels across countries. Correlational analysis between belongingness and perceived climate were carried out initially for each country separately. The mean correlations of belongingness and each of the perceived climate variables, as well as between the two perceived climate variables across all countries were calculated by converting the raw correlation coefficients for each country into Fisher’s z-scores to account for the range limit of the correlation coefficient, thus avoiding a slightly skewed distribution of the means towards 0. The Fisher z-scores were then averaged and converted back to a correlation coefficient. Note that on the country level, each nation is equally weighted. Belongingness was regressed on perceived cooperative and competitive climate on the national level in a structural equation model. In a final step, a multilevel structural equation model (using Mplus 8) was used to confirm the aggregated regression analysis to take into consideration the hierarchical data structure in PISA where schools are sampled first and then students within school. The student data were aggregated to the school level (level 1) and country was treated as level 2. Critical coefficients are the regression coefficients on level 2.

Descriptive statistics

Mean scores for belongingness, perceived competitiveness, and perceived cooperativity across all countries in the PISA data are listed in Table 1 and presented visually in Figure 1. Overall, the average belongingness scores range was 2.77 < M < 3.30.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean belongingness and perceived competitive and cooperative climates.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average sense of belonging by country (scale truncated).

Correlation analyses

Average belongingness by country from the PISA 2003 cycle had a moderate correlation with the average belongingness scores from the PISA 2018 cycle, r(36) = 0.49, p < 0.001. However, excluding four countries that had the largest discrepancies between the two cycles, Japan, Korea, Lithuania, and Sweden, strengthens the correlation substantially, r(32) = 0.67, p < 0.001, indicating that the change in the belongingness scores of these four countries accounted for most of the instability in the overall relative belongingness scores of these countries over time.

Overall, significant correlations between belongingness and perceived competitiveness were weak and present in both directions (−0.12 < r < 0.23, p < 0.001) with a mean correlation of 0.05 (calculated using Fisher’s z-transformation). By contrast, the correlations between belongingness and perceived cooperativity were stronger and significantly positive for all countries (0.11 < r < 0.40, p < 0.001). The mean (Fisher’s z) correlation was r = 0.25. The correlations between perceived competitive climate and cooperative climate were all significant except for two countries, and positively correlated except for one country (−0.05 < r < 0.46, p < 0.001). The average (Fisher transformed) correlation was r = 0.22. When aggregated on the country level, the correlation between perceived competition and perceived cooperation was significant and positive, r(74) = 0.40, p < 0.001. The specific calculated correlation coefficients and p values for each country can be found in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between belongingness and perceived climates.

Regression analysis

In the regression models, belongingness was regressed on perceived cooperativity and perceived competitiveness and both variables were shown to be significant predictors of overall belongingness aggregated in most countries. In more than three fourth of the countries, competitive climate is a negative, cooperation a positive predictor (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression by country.

Integrated into a multilevel SEM model, 18.8% of the variance between countries is explained with aggregated perceived competition and cooperation as predictors (Table 4). The significance of the residual indicates that the average school climate variables do not explain all of the variance between countries in their average sense of belonging.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Level 2 regression coefficients in a two-level SEM model (df = 68).

Discussion

The current study investigated the relationship between student sense of belonging and perceived competitiveness and cooperativeness in the context of explaining intercultural differences in sense of school belonging.

The first aim was to examine the relative stability of sense of belongingness over time for countries that were present during both 2003 and 2018 cycles of PISA data. The second aim was to test the hypothesis that higher perceived competitiveness in the learning environment corresponded with lower reported levels of belongingness across all countries. The third aim was to assess whether the variance in belongingness between countries was explained at a significant level by perceived competitive and cooperative climate.

We found a moderate positive correlation between the relative belongingness scores of countries in both the 2003 and 2018 cycles of PISA data. However, after excluding four countries that had the largest discrepancies between the datasets, Japan, Korea, Lithuania, and Sweden, there was a strong positive correspondence between the overall relative belongingness scores. This result indicates that the discrepancies in these countries accounted for most of the instability of the relative belongingness scores overall. A possible explanation could be that changes were made to the translation of the items between the two datasets over time that caused a significant change in potential interpretation of the response items. Even more impactful would be a change in the wording of the Likert response scale anchors (e.g., from “fully agree” to “very much agree”) because it keeps the factorial structure of the scales intact but makes mean comparison over time problematic (Sun et al., 2019). Further examination of the response items in the language administered for each country for both datasets would be required to fully assess this possibility. The alternative explanation would be that the increase in belongingness was caused by a substantial shift in the national pedagogy which seem rather unlikely.

Although no strong significant direct correlation was found between belongingness and perceived competitive climate overall across countries, a significant positive correlation was found between belongingness and perceived cooperative climate for all countries, confirming what prior research found in country-specific analyses (Lätsch, 2017; Tran et al., 2019; Ryzin et al., 2020; Keramati and Gillies, 2021). When belongingness was regressed on both climate perception variables, perceived competition becomes a significant negative predictor in all but 5 countries, which follows our theoretical predictions and supports the notion in prior research that a competitive learning climate can have detrimental effect on the psychosocial wellbeing in schools (Slavin, 2000; Posselt and Lipson, 2016). Our results suggest that although perceived competition does not directly correlate with belongingness, there is a possible interaction effect that perceived cooperation has on perceived competition: Competition can be beneficial for promoting a sense of belongingness, but only when it is perceived to be a friendly and not threatening competition, i.e., when the dominant sense is that of a cooperative and supportive climate. In this case competition has a more playful, not threatening character. Another possible explanation could be that perceived cooperation may act as a protective factor against the negative effects of perceived competition on belongingness. Further research is needed to address the exact relationship between the perceived climate variables.

The residual variance in belongingness across countries remains significant after controlling for the two key predictor variables, suggesting that other variables also contribute to the differences between countries in terms of average school belongingness. One likely factor that generally contributes to additional (random) variance is translation imprecision of survey items. This is particularly likely in international studies like the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) and PISA when items are translated into over 50 different languages. Even in cases where there are no translation imprecisions in terms of item meaning, the semantic of item anchors of Likert scales may vary enough to cause artificial mean differences across languages (translation of terms like “some,” “very” and “totally” will differ in valence). Therefore, we maintain that the amount of variance explained by perceived climate variables is a lower-bound estimate of its actual importance to explain intercultural differences in the sense of school belongingness.

Limitations

One potential limitation of this study could be the variation in sample sizes between countries which can affect the power of the significance test. However, for all countries the absolute sample size can be considered saturated in the sense that even small true correlations with limited theoretical relevance would become significant in all subsamples.

The obtained rank correlation between the 2003 PISA data cycle and the 2018 PISA data cycle of 0.49 indicates that there is a degree of instability among the belongingness scores over time. At face value, this result could potentially indicate an overall global shift in student sense of belongingness. However, a more plausible explanation is that the discrepancies can be attributed to changes that were made to the scales used to measure belongingness across time in each country, including but not limited to changes in translation of the scale items and response scales. This needs further exploration, for example by analyzing data from all PISA cycles between 2003 and 2018.

Another limitation is that the measurements obtained from the scales in the PISA survey are dependent on self-rated reports from students. Thus, the results may be subject to the limitations in validity and reliability that come with self-reported measurements in general as the validity of the data depends on the levels of self-reflecting, honesty, and self-awareness of the students.

Future directions

Overall, the findings of this study show that in addition to the mediation effect of perceived climate for international difference in levels of belonging, there is a potential interaction effect between perceived competition and perceived cooperation that may influence sense of belongingness in most, if not all countries. Future studies should further investigate and characterize the moderating effects of perceived cooperative climate on the effect of perceived competitive climate on belongingness and should assess whether cultural differences influence the degree to which this effect occurs, as well as examine the differences between PISA datasets distributed in countries with high levels of discrepancies between to address the issue of the instability in belongingness observed in a few countries. Additionally, future research should characterize the relationship between the constructs of perceived competitive climate and perceived cooperative climate to contribute to a more thorough understanding of the complex ways in which student perceptions of climate interact with the social context within an educational setting.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.oecd.org/pisa/data/2018database/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Health Sciences and Behavioral Sciences Institutional Review Board (IRB-HSBS) at the University of Michigan. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

SO wrote the introduction, and did the descriptive analysis. KC wrote the method and results. The discussion was written together. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ames, C., and Ames, R. (1984). Goal structures and motivation. Elem. Sch. J. 85, 39–52. doi: 10.1086/461390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderman, E. M. (2002). School effects on psychological outcomes during adolescence. J. Educ. Psychol. 94, 795–809. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arslan, G. (2021). School belongingness, well-being, and mental health among adolescents: exploring the role of loneliness. Aust. J. Psychol. 73, 70–80. doi: 10.1080/00049530.2021.1904499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskin, T. W., Wampold, B. E., Quintana, S. M., and Enright, R. D. (2010). Belongingness as a protective factor against loneliness and potential depression in a multicultural middle school. Couns. Psychol. 38, 626–651. doi: 10.1177/0011000009358459

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battistich, V., and Hom, A. (1997). The relationship between students' sense of their school as a community and their involvement in problem behaviors. Am. J. Public Health 87, 1997–2001. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.12.1997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., and Schaps, E. (1997). Caring school communities. Educ. Psychol. 32, 137–151. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3203_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergin, D. A. (1995). Effects of a mastery versus competitive motivation situation on learning. J. Exp. Educ. 63, 303–314. doi: 10.1080/00220973.1995.9943466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canning, E. A., LaCosse, J., Kroeper, K. M., and Murphy, M. C. (2019). Feeling like an imposter: the effect of perceived classroom competition on the daily psychological experiences of first-generation college students. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 11, 647–657. doi: 10.1177/1948550619882032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortina, K. S., Arel, S., and Smith-Darden, J. P. (2017). School belonging in different cultures: the effects of individualism and power distance. Front. Educ. 2:56. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2017.00056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crul, M. (2018). Culture, identity, belonging, and school success. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2018, 109–114. doi: 10.1002/cad.20234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Datu, J. A. D., and Valdez, J. P. M. (2019). Psychological capital is associated with higher levels of life satisfaction and school belongingness. Sch. Psychol. Int. 40, 331–346. doi: 10.1177/0143034319838011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 225–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00725.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fülöp, M. (2000). Teachers’ perception of the role of competition in their country: Hungary, Japan and the USA. Citizsh. Soc. Econ. Educ. 4, 142–158. doi: 10.2304/csee.2000.4.3.142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fülöp, M. (2004). “Competition as a culturally constructed concept” in Travelling facts: the social construction, distribution, and accumulation of knowledge. Eds. C. Baillie, E. Dunn, and Y. Zheng (New York: Campus), 124–148.

Google Scholar

Glass, C. R., and Westmont, C. M. (2014). Comparative effects of belongingness on the academic success and cross-cultural interactions of domestic and international students. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 38, 106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school membership among adolescents: scale development and educational correlates. Psychol. Sch. 30, 79–90. doi: 10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1<79::AID-PITS2310300113>3.0.CO;2-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosnell, C. L. (2020). Receiving quality positive event support from peers may enhance student connection and the learning environment. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Psychol. 6, 342–349. Available at: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/stl0000178

Google Scholar

Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., and Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: software of the mind. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research. Interaction Book Company.

Google Scholar

Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1994). Review of learning together and alone: cooperative, competitive, and individualistic learning (2nd ed.). Contemp. Psychol. J. Rev. 32:580. doi: 10.1037/027279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, D. R., Soldner, M., Leonard, J. B., Alvarez, P., Inkelas, K. K., Rowan-Kenyon, H. T., et al. (2007). Examining sense of belonging among first-year undergraduates from different racial/ethnic groups. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 48, 525–542. doi: 10.1353/csd.2007.0054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kathuria, T., and Pandya, A. (2020). Listen, I am too responsible! Effect of schooling on sense of trust, responsibility and belongingness to the community of children working on the street in Chandigarh City, India. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 119:105527. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keramati, M. R., and Gillies, R. M. (2021). Advantages and challenges of cooperative learning in two different cultures. Educ. Sci. 12:3. doi: 10.3390/educsci12010003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, R., and Maehr, M. L. (2010). “Schooling, cultural diversity, and student motivation” in Handbook of research on schools, schooling and human development Eds. J. L. Meece and J. S. Eccles (New York, NY: Routledge), 308–324.

Google Scholar

Lam, S.-F., Yim, P.-S., Law, J., and Cheung, R. (2001). The effects of classroom competition on achievement motivation Quality of Education Fund of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region.

Google Scholar

Lardier, D. T., Opara, I., Bergeson, C., Herrera, A., Garcia-Reid, P., and Reid, R. J. (2019). A study of psychological sense of community as a mediator between supportive social systems, school belongingness, and outcome behaviors among urban high school students of color. J. Community Psychol. 47, 1131–1150. doi: 10.1002/jcop.22182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lätsch, A. (2017). Do perceived helpfulness and competition in classroom contexts moderate or mediate the association between perceived stress and socio-emotional strengths and difficulties from early to middle adolescence? Learn. Individ. Differ. 58, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maehr, M. L., and Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school cultures. Boulder: Westview.

Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., and Eccles, J. S. (1989). Change in teacher efficacy and student self- and task-related beliefs in mathematics during the transition to junior high school. J. Educ. Psychol. 81, 247–258. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.81.2.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moeller, R. W., Seehuus, M., and Peisch, V. (2020). Emotional intelligence, belongingness, and mental health in college students. Front. Psychol. 11:93. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Napoli, M., Marsiglia, F. F., and Kulis, S. (2003). Sense of belonging in school as a protective factor against drug abuse among native American urban adolescents. J. Soc. Work. Pract. Addict. 3, 25–41. doi: 10.1300/j160v03n02_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD . (2004). Learning for tomorrow's world: first results from PISA 2003, PISA. OECD Publishing, Paris.

Google Scholar

OECD . (2018). PISA 2018 results: what students know and can do (Vol. 1), Paris, France: OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

Osterman, K. F. (2000). Students' need for belonging in the school community. Rev. Educ. Res. 70, 323–367. doi: 10.3102/00346543070003323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posselt, J. R., and Lipson, S. K. (2016). Competition, anxiety, and depression in the college classroom: variations by student identity and field of study. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 57, 973–989. doi: 10.1353/csd.2016.0094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.55.1.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryzin, M. J., Gravely, A. A., and Roseth, C. J. (2007). Autonomy, belongingness, and engagement in school as contributors to adolescent psychological well-being. J. Youth Adolesc. 38, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9257-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryzin, M. J., and Roseth, C. J. (2019). Effects of cooperative learning on peer relations, empathy, and bullying in middle school. Aggress. Behav. 45, 643–651. doi: 10.1002/ab.21858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryzin, M. J., Roseth, C. J., and McClure, H. (2020). The effects of cooperative learning on peer relations, academic support, and engagement in learning among students of color. J. Educ. Res. 113, 283–291. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2020.1806016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, J.-W. (2019). Thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness in Korean college students: psychometric properties and cultural considerations for the interpersonal needs questionnaire-10. Death Stud. 44, 261–269. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2018.1541938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slavin, R. E. (2000). Cooperative learning: theory, research, and practice Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Slavin, R. E. (2014). Cooperative learning in elementary schools. Education 3–13 43, 5–14. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2015.963370

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., Cortina, K. S., Miller, K. F., and Ning, H. (2019). Willpower as cultural construct: do Chinese students believe less in its depletion? Front. Psychol. 10:988. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tellhed, U., Bäckström, M., and Björklund, F. (2016). Will I fit in and do well? The importance of social belongingness and self-efficacy for explaining gender differences in interest in STEM and heed majors. Sex Roles 77, 86–96. doi: 10.1007/s11199-016-0694-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, V. D., Loc Nguyen, T. M., Van De, N., Soryaly, C., and Doan, M. N. (2019). Does cooperative learning may enhance the use of students’ learning strategies? Int. J. High. Educ. 8:79. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v8n4p79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivekananda-Schmidt, P., and Sandars, J. (2018). Belongingness and its implications for undergraduate health professions education: a scoping review. Educ. Prim. Care 29, 268–275. doi: 10.1080/14739879.2018.1478677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, K. J., and Thrasher, S. S. (2023). The Impact of Community Belongingness on Mental Health and Well-Being Among Black LGBTQ. Adults. Race Soc. Probl. doi: 10.1007/s12552-023-09396-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wentzel, K. R. (2015). Socialization in school settings. In: Grusec, J. E. & P. D. Hastings , Handbook of socialization, theory and research, 251–275. New Yorck, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Williams, P., and Sheridan, S. (2010). Conditions for collaborative learning and constructive competition in school. Educ. Res. 52, 335–350. doi: 10.1080/00131881.2010.524748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, K., Vongkulluksn, V. W., Cheng, S.-L., and Jiang, Z. (2022). Examining high-school students’ motivation change through a person-centered approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 114:89. doi: 10.1037/edu0000507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: school belonging, intercultural comparison, learning climate, competitive & collaborative learning, trend analyses

Citation: Ooi SX and Cortina KS (2023) Cooperative and competitive school climate: their impact on sense of belonging across cultures. Front. Educ. 8:1113996. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1113996

Received: 02 December 2022; Accepted: 30 August 2023;
Published: 15 September 2023.

Edited by:

Verónica López, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso, Chile

Reviewed by:

Yung-Ji Sher, National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan
Paula Molinero González, University of Valladolid, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Ooi and Cortina. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shi Xin Ooi, sxooi@umich.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.