Skip to main content

CONCEPTUAL ANALYSIS article

Front. Educ., 11 May 2023
Sec. Special Educational Needs

Executive functions in understanding certain disabilities within an African context

  • 1School of Built Environment and Development Studies, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa
  • 2School of Applied and Human Sciences, Department of Social Work, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa
  • 3Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Department of Development Studies, University of Zululand, KwaDlangezwa, South Africa

Cognitive and social functioning are important aspects of the development of all children. In the transition from one stage of development to another, children are expected to master related skills. There is sufficient literature depicting that children with learning disabilities often experience challenges in a range of executive functions, which are necessary for the successful negotiation of their educational and life-related tasks. As a result, they may require appropriate intervention measures to enable them to address possible limitations. This paper examines the cognitive and social skills deficits that impact on the planning and decision-making outcomes of children with disabilities, using an Afrocentric approach. The Afrocentric paradigm is central in studying and understanding African experiences from an African perspective. Afrocentricity is used as an alternative knowledge system, which places the needs of Africans at the center of focus. In contrast to Eurocentric tendencies, the holistic being, in the African context, is understood and constituted as body, mind, and spirit. While there is sufficient Western literature acknowledging similar holistic views, and biopsychosocial and spiritual dimensions, this paper points out on the lack of emphasis on the latter dimension. The paper concludes that without providing relevant interventions on learner executive functions, the impairment of cognitive and social functioning is likely to continue.

1. Introduction

As they transition from one stage of development to another, children are expected to master related developmental skills, which place significance on the domains of cognitive, social, cultural and spiritual functioning. For the longest time, the injustices of inadequate access for learners with disabilities, and the consequent limitations, have remained the two main concerns with which various scholars and interest groups have grappled. This paper explores the conception and intervention of the executive functions (EFs) from the Afrocentric perspective, which emphasizes the communitarian view, in contrast to limiting such intervention to an individualist view.

This paper acknowledges the Eurocentric approach of the biological, psychological, social and spiritual dimensions as four possible sources of human disturbances but notes that the social and spiritual domains appear to lack central attention. Applicable in the African context is the conception of a unified whole, comprised of body, mind, and spirit. Fundamentally, this paper does not dispute the Western notion of “the holistic orientation that inherently recognizes the biopsychosocial-spiritual nature of human beings” (Downs, 2016, p. 31), but argues that there is less attention paid to spiritual dimensions (Mark and Lyons, 2010). As stated by García-Campos et al. (2018, p. 6):

This biopsychosocial-spiritual model is not a “dualism” in which a “soul” accidentally inhabits a body. Rather, in this model, the biological, the psychological, the social, and the spiritual are only distinct dimensions of the person, and no aspect can be disaggregated from the whole.

There is an ongoing need for a culturally affirming education, which is rooted in and reflects the African ethos. Alem (2019) argues that Africans possess a thought system which is embedded in their cultures, and that their way of life is believed to be interactive and communitarian. Eskay et al. (2012) describe culture as a pattern of behavior exhibited in response to diverse phenomena in their environment. Highlighting the significance of personhood, Nwoye (2017) asserts that human beings do not mature through birth but that they are made or constructed. In recognizing the developmental trajectory of EFs along the lifespan, De Luca and Leventer (2008) regard goal-directed behaviors and planning as the beginning of maturity in preschool.

In this context, planning is evident when a child makes a list of what must be achieved to finish prescribed tasks. However, for some of these learners, starting a given task is difficult and they are often then labelled as lazy and procrastinators. This has led them to be unable to prioritize important tasks, and face challenges in time management and meeting due dates. This has also led to these learners being more likely to battle with resisting temptation, be impulsive and thoughtless before they act (Otero and Barker, 2014).

This paper explores the need for an Afrocentric perspective to explain the agency of people experiencing limited EFs. Most people are regarded as executing their actions in a routine manner, based on the notion that most behavioral repertoire is exhibited in the same physical and social environment (Aarts and Dijksterhuis, 2000). Executive skills involve a set of higher-order cognitive processes that allow one to realize a variety of outcomes, namely to plan, organize, and successfully execute purposeful, goal-directed, and future-oriented actions (Temple, 1997; Gioia et al., 2000; Zelazo et al., 2016). Furthermore, Zelazo et al. (2016) regard neurocognitive skills as required to engage in the goal-directed control of thought, action, and emotion. While it is perceived as desirable for persons to develop the ability to achieve purposeful, goal-directed, and future-oriented behavior, it is not unusual to find many people, and children in particular, experiencing developmental difficulties in the performance of “executive tasks, lacking the capacity to plan, monitor, update, and shift their goal-directed actions” (De Luca and Leventer, 2008, p. 45).

A lack of executive skills often inhibits the management of emotions and thoughts and the subsequent regulation of their behavior. Nwoye (2015) identifies the biological, psychological, social, and spiritual dimensions as four possible sources of human disturbance and challenges. The Eurocentric tendency is to focus its aetiological explanation of mental illness to only the first three domains.

Most studies about executive functioning focus on studies of participants who reside in developed countries, mostly including European ones (Willoughby et al., 2019). In contrast, there have been few studies in Africa on the effects of diseases and infections of cognitive function. This notion stands in contrast to the African’s holistic conception of the universe and cosmology as a blending of the divine, spirit, human, animate and inanimate beings, which constantly interact with one another (Igboin, 2011).

In the human service profession, spirituality is increasingly recognized as a significant client strength that can aid a client’s well-being (Hodge, 2011). Embracing the cultural and spirituality dimensions, among others, may be essential in providing a holistic human frame of reference and it is, therefore, our conviction in this paper to regard their inclusion as bearing some significance in the Afrocentric perspective. Executive function (EF) deficits are central characteristics of many acquired and developmental disorders of childhood (Gioia et al., 2000). Zelazo et al. (2016, p. 23) concurs that “Self-regulated learning refers to an active form of learning, in which the learner is metacognitively, motivationally, and behaviorally engaged in the learning process.” In this regard, the individualistic view of EFs is regarded as posing a challenge in limiting executive skills, as foundational skills underlying the individual learner, instead of propounding the learner’s functioning and development from the communitarian perspective.

2. Executive functions

Executive functions (EFs) are described by neuropsychologists as the abilities that allow a person to engage in purposeful, self-serving behavior, such as volition, planning, the ability to translate a plan into productive behavior, and self-monitoring (Melrose et al., 2018). It represents a broad range of cognitive abilities that appear to be related but can be distinguished into distinct abilities, such as shifting between tasks, updating information in working memory, and inhibiting prepotent responses. Other scholars, including Anderson (2008), refer to EFs as executive control or cognitive control, and do not regard it as a unitary or a linear process, but rather as a collection of various domains, consisting of intellect, thought, self-control, and social interaction. He further asserts that this is regarded as a psychological construct that is composed of multiple interrelated high-level cognitive skills. The four domains of learning, memory, response time and intrinsic intelligence are used to characterize cognitive function, attendance, and academic success, to describe educational outcome indicators (Ezeamama et al., 2018). Learning disorders (LD) have also been linked to executive dysfunctions, characterized by a child having significant difficulty in specific areas, such as reading or writing (El Wafa et al., 2020).

The studies investigating the neurological and developmental impacts of malaria and anemia and soil-transmitted helminths (STH) infections regard the development of speech and motor skills as often delayed (Dellis, 2009; Zainal and Newman, 2018; Mazibuko and Chimbari, 2022). Neurological effects have been described, even in nonanemic deficits, when learning potential is decreased due to impairment of learners’ EF and recognition memory (Dellis, 2009). Most of the attempts to ascertain the nature of EFs reflect they are by no means a unitary concept, as traditionally it has only been seen from a cognitive perspective (Kouklari et al., 2019).

The idea of EFs can be thought of in numerous ways. Working memory can be viewed in relation to EFs using a cognitive science approach. The capacity to temporarily store and manipulate information is known as working memory (Melrose et al., 2018). The key elements of EF are identified as including “(a) anticipation and deployment of attention; (b) impulse control and self-regulation; (c) initiation of activity; (d) working memory; (e) mental flexibility and utilization of feedback; (f) planning ability and organization; and (g) selection of efficient problem-solving strategies” (Anderson, 2008, p. 4). The EFs provide cognitive processing deficits that enable students to have agency over various domains with a view to achieving the learning goals. Furthermore, EF deficits have been found to be linked with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), not oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Social problems are principal to the difficulties associated with ADHD. Children with ADHD struggle to take turns, and often interrupt and intrude into others’ space. The literature asserts that children with ADHD are stigmatized and fail to obtain peer acceptance, particularly in the schooling environment and at a societal level (Clark et al., 2002). ADHD symptoms, such as hyperactivity, inattention, and impaired executive function, are all identified as contributing factors to the poor school, learning barrier and social functioning often recognized in young children with ADHD (Gau et al., 2015). The literature has portrayed that ADHD, learning difficulties and behavioral problems are associated with deficits in EF (El Wafa et al., 2020). Some scholars have indicated that deficits in inhibition, working memory and cognitive flexibility have been linked with mathematical difficulties in children with normal IQ (Bull and Scerif, 2001; Altemeier et al., 2006; Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009). Therefore, early treatment of children with ADHD and other chronic conditions could potentially mitigate the memory and learning deficits, especially in preschool learners, though these chronic conditions tend to be lifelong. Recent evidence suggests that early identification of “at-risk children should take place around the age of 5 years when they enroll in a preschool because appropriate interventions and support can have a significant impact on future academic progress (Mazibuko and Chimbari, 2022).

2.1. Historical development of executive functions

While the concept can be defined differently, there is a common view in the literature that EF involves a diversity of cognitive processes, namely planning, working memory, attention, inhibition, self-monitoring, and self-regulation (Gioia et al., 2000; Goldstein et al., 2014). This paper describes self-monitoring as a process where children evaluate themselves on how they perform in a specific task. According to Goldstein et al. (2014) although the historical evidence suggests that the executive dysfunctional challenge was initially identified as a control mechanism as far back as 1840, it was only in 1970 that the term ‘executive functions’ started to be used.

In tracing the origin of the term, we must mention the case of the patient Phineas Gage, as “the earliest and best cited example of the impact of frontal lobe pathology on the executive function” (Anderson et al., 2008). Harlow (1869) narrates the story of Phineas Gage, one of his clients, on how a tamping iron penetrated his skull and damaged his frontal cortex, resulting in erratic behaviour and debilitating personality change.

The executive skills are regarded as subservient to the supervisory role that involves integrating information stored elsewhere in the brain (Stuss and Alexander, 2000; Shallice, 2004). Morgenstern and Bates (1999) and Otero and Barker (2014) regard that, at the clinical level, this challenge mainly results from the damage to the frontal lobes, which subsequently affect different domains of neuropsychological functioning. The paper asserts that some children are born with weak EFs, injury to the frontal lobe of the brain, ADHD, depression, and stroke act. Zelazo et al. (2016) provide an understanding of normative brain development — that the frontal lobes undergo a prolonged period of development spanning from infancy to early adulthood. As a life course construct, it is worth noting that the environmental stimuli could be instrumental during the development, particularly as children transition. In the African context, families and communities are regarded as essential systems in the development of the child.

Zelazo et al. (2016) identify three skills serving as ways to actively and intentionally control attention to accomplish a goal, namely cognitive flexibility, working memory and inhibitory control. First, cognitive flexibility focuses on thinking about something in diverse ways and being able to adjust to changing demands or priorities and perceptions. Some of these children with a lack of cognitive flexibility struggle to change and may not know how to react when a change is required; this normally leads to anxiety, anger, frustration and giving up (Otero and Barker, 2014). Conversely, this suggests that one should have adaptable thinking and the ability to adjust and solve problems. Second, the working memory includes both keeping information in mind and, usually, mentally working with it in some way. This differs from short-term memory, which is about “holding information in mind” (Diamond, 2013, p. 8). Third, inhibitory control is regarded as the process of deliberately suppressing attention to something, such as ignoring a distraction, stopping an impulsive utterance, or overcoming a highly learned response. Diamond (2013) regards inhibitory control as the ability to control one’s holistic functioning, including attention, behavior, thoughts, and/or emotions that are acceptable and appropriate in society. Generally, these skills also help learners and people to stay focused and disciplined when one must finish certain tasks. When one lacks this kind of function, one is likely to be driven by impulses, habits, and external stimuli.

The multidimensional assessment of EF is evident in the Delis rating of EF (D-REF), which is a set of rating scales designed to assess executive functions manifested through constituent subprocesses in children and adolescents between the ages of 5 and 18 (Warnick et al., 2015). The D-REF is described as a:

supplemental assessment of children and adolescents demonstrating behavioral or cognitive difficulties often associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), traumatic brain injury (TBI), learning disorders/disability, and other developmental, psychiatric, neurological, or medical conditions known to affect executive functions. (ibid: 597).

From this review, it is apparent that there are diverse causes of EFs. The Western culture seems easy to diagnose a particular behavior as being abnormal. For instance, the 5th proposition of the person-centered approach, describes all behavior as consisting of purposeful endeavors to satisfy the needs as experienced (Glober et al., 2013). Nwoye (2017) advocates for the deconstruction of psychopathology and highlights the need to address the gap observed in the Western paradigm to psychopathological practice and treatment that makes it unable to deal decisively with the certain presenting illness of the Black African client. It is therefore paramount to guard against possible misdiagnosis and misplacement of clients in the process.

2.2. Executive functions and learning

There is evidence depicting EFs as providing a foundation for learning and achievement, in that it provides both direct and indirect roles in classroom learning (Zelazo et al., 2016). In relation to this view, these skills do influence students to sit still, pay attention, remember and follow rules, and flexibly adopt new perspectives, among other things. Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2000) are of the view that behaviors that are carried out less often are more likely to be accurately predicted by intentions towards the behavior. In explaining the relationship between past and future behavioral patterns, this assertion confirms the assumption that if the behavior that has been performed many times in the past, the related future behavior becomes increasingly associated with the automated process. This assertion is further defined by Bay and Daniel (2003), who argue that past behavior makes a substantive contribution to understanding future behavior. Goldstein et al. (2014) discuss the EFs in education in relation to Luria’s neurodevelopmental model, which postulates specific developmental stages related to stages of higher cortical maturation. The time dimension is one of the crucial aspects of learning. As Barkley (2010) asserts, past experience, and the related prior learning experience, are essential in the development of new learning.

Alternatively, empirical evidence, particularly from epidemiological studies, supports the relationship between executive functioning and illnesses and infections. Studies have linked cognitive functions and educational loss to a lack of deworming or schistosomiasis infection (Terer et al., 2013; Ezeamama et al., 2018; Sacolo-Gwebu et al., 2019). Child development may be compromised by diseases such as schistosomiasis and STH and contingent responses to infections, such as fever and hypertrophy of the muscles (Mazibuko and Chimbari, 2022). Schistosomiasis and STH may impair cognitive function because of one, or a combination, of parasitic infection symptoms, such as iron deficiency anaemia (Zainal and Newman, 2018). This has a negative impact on children’s learning, memory, school attendance and academic performance. Though few studies in Africa have investigated the effects of EFs in relation to learning and language, there is a knowledge gap regarding whether diseases and infections have the same impact on expressive language skills as they may have on other cognitive skills.

Nazel et al. (1999) show how parasitic infection detrimentally affects cognitive functions of school children aged 9 to 12 years. This study reported that although the infected children’s overall IQ (intelligence quotient) scores were not significantly lower than those of the controls, they performed significantly worse on the comprehension, vocabulary, and image completion subtests (Nazel et al., 1999). Reading comprehension is a difficult process that demands the integration of skills needed to process information at the word and sentence levels to comprehend what is being read. EF is likely associated with reading comprehension through its relationship with decoding and oral language, and this finding provides additional support for the role of EF in reading comprehension as a potentially crucial precursor to skilled reading. Another study that examined the relationships between oral language, decoding, and two components of EF (cognitive flexibility and working memory) and reading comprehension found that these relationships exist (Spencer et al., 2020). Learners with specific language impairment (SLI) are at high risk for developing subsequent social, emotional, and behavioral issues, as well as delayed literacy skills, such as reading and spelling (Prathanee et al., 2007).

Factors found to be influencing cognitive function have been explored in several studies on autism spectrum disorder (ASD). ASD is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by difficulties in social communication and interaction, as well as restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities (Demetriou et al., 2019). The findings from ASD studies discovered a link between executive dysfunction and ASD symptoms and may be connected to anatomical and functional differences in the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Chen et al., 2019; Johnston et al., 2019; Ameis et al., 2020; Andreou et al., 2020). The assessment of EF and activities of daily living (ADL) is critical in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (Martyr and Clare, 2012).

In a study conducted by Guarino et al. (2019), EFs are found to be impaired in Alzheimer’s disease from the beginning, primarily due to prefrontal cortex degeneration, inhibitory abilities, attentional and visuospatial functions are compromised. Alzheimer’s disease is known as a severe, irreversible illness marked by a gradual deterioration in cognitive function that interferes with the standard instrumental, necessary daily activities (Martyr and Clare, 2012). There is a need for neuropsychological assessment of EFs in preclinical and clinical phases of Alzheimer’s disease (Allain et al., 2013).

2.3. Executive functions and psychological assessment

In this section, it is worth acknowledging the view that, while children and adults with learning and attention are found to be struggling profoundly with many of the skills that most of us take for granted, literature on psychological assessments warns against possible faulty claims regarding test performance, particularly in as far as incumbents from other cultural backgrounds are concerned. Kim and Zabelina (2015:130) regard it as “inappropriate to use the same assessments with individuals of various racial/ethnic minority groups without norming the instrument to reflect those groups.” Shuttleworth-Edwards et al. (2013) assert that cross-cultural literature is of the view that focusing on ethnicity or race differences alone is likely to lead to faulty claims regarding test performance, due to cultural influence, such as acculturation to the predominant culture, amongst other factors. Acculturation is multidimensional. Acculturation in the South African context could be described as mostly multidimensional in the sense that individuals find themselves having to negotiate their identities within these plural cultural contexts (Adams and van de Vijver, 2017). García-Campos et al. (2018) emphasize that the success in promoting EFs for all students is manifested through considering their diversity. It is on similar basis that the challenges of inclusive education proactively promote conducive teaching environments that accommodate human differences and overcome existing exclusions. It is interesting to note that Adams and Abubakar (2016) are of the view that, even in these multidimensional considerations of acculturation, outcomes of acculturation seem to be somewhat associated with Western cultural values, norms, and attitudes.

Laher and Cockcroft (2013, p. 6) assert that “cognitive tests are still largely viewed with suspicion in South Africa as a result of their past misuse to enforce and support divisive racial apartheid practices.” In line with this view, made almost two decades after the advent of the new democracy, there is a notion that the value of psychological testing is not without contestation. In this regard, this assertion does not suggest that the benefit of psychological testing on society should be rejected, but that they need to be undertaken with consideration of cultural diversity. Laher and Cockcroft (2013) further highlight that the neuropsychological assessment in South Africa, with its focus on the assessment of brain-behavior relationships, also faces major challenges because of the complexity and diversity of the country’s population, varying levels and qualities of education, socio-economic status discrepancies and rapid acculturation. In African countries, measuring EF skills in children has been hindered by the lack of assessments that are both easy to deploy and scalable (Willoughby et al., 2019).

An assessment of the capacity to understand mental functions, such as beliefs, intentions, deception, and emotions, among others, can be measured using a developmental neuropsychological assessment test such as the ‘theory of mind’ test (ToM; Berenguer et al., 2018). ToM is also known as mentalizing skills, which is the ability to ascribe cognitive states, such as beliefs, intentions and emotions, to both oneself and others, as well as to predict subsequent behaviors (Andreou et al., 2020). A model of executive dysfunction, which includes defective ToM was put out to explain behavioral issues in ASD, including impaired ToM. They concluded that discrete EFs (set shifting, response inhibition, and working memory) may serve as useful cognitive indicators for differentiating between ASD and ADHD after reviewing research findings on a variety of neurodevelopmental diseases (Demetriou et al., 2019). However, it should be noted that a study to better understand the nature of EFs itself is necessary before determining the specific nature of the EFs developmental pathway and its impact on ToM in ASD (Kouklari et al., 2019).

EFs can be assessed by the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF). “The BRI consists of three scales, including skills such as adjusting to changes in routine (Shift), suppressing impulsive responses (Inhibit), and moderating one’s emotions (Emotional Control)” (Bertollo and Yerys, 2019, p. 5). In a study examining whether early childhood EFs predict new onset or worsening of ADHD and/or depression, the BRIEF has been shown to differentiate ADHD subtypes and to be a useful tool for assessing EFs deficits in ADHD (Hawkey et al., 2018). The type of self-regulation deficits assessed by the BRIEF in children may be linked to disorders such as ADHD and depression as early as preschool (Hawkey et al., 2018). Examination of everyday behavior is a complementary approach to assessing EFs in preschool children. The child’s everyday environments, both at home and at school or daycare, are important venues for observing routine manifestations of the EFs. Historically, examining daily behavior is an additional method for psychological assessing executive skills in preschoolers (Isquith et al., 2004). The child’s regular contexts, such as home, school, or daycare, are crucial places to observe the EFs in action on a regular basis.

3. Afrocentric paradigm

Alem (2019, p. 17) argues that the worry for most African scholars springs from the fact that Africa continues to be the greatest consumer of Western scientific and technological products, but which bear little relevance to Africa. It is worth noting that there is evidence suggesting that disability activism has grown transnationally in Africa, but there are still unanswered questions regarding the appropriateness of universalizing disability knowledge and principles (Haang’andu, 2019). This challenge could be mainly attributed to having African experiences perceived and written from Eurocentric perspectives. In relation to this view, there is a need to explore resolutions that embrace the Afrocentric ethos.

For some time, the misunderstanding of disability in the African context has led to the negative perception and treatment of children with disabilities (Eskay et al., 2012). In the process, Africans deprived themselves the prime opportunity to develop African epistemologies that were essential in developing understanding of disability and knowledge creation (Sefotho, 2021).

Two aspects worth noting in respect to the Afrocentric perspective, are the relationship between people and inanimate objects, including the visible and the invisible, and the multidimensionality of time, namely the past, present and future. For instance, it suffices to indicate that, from the Afrocentric perspective, it is not all social ills that originate from visible sources. Taking the example of ancestry, most Africans share a belief of the active role of ancestry and the link between the living and non-living. According to Nwoye (2017, p. 47), “most African people many a time act from the understanding and assumption that various types of misfortune, illness, death, and failure arise from the activities of unseen forces, unknown and unseen infuriated spiritual agencies, and revengeful ancestors.” Despite the political systems, “Sub-Saharan Africa has myriad beliefs, rites and representations of both the living and the dead, which are also viewed with prejudice by westerners” (Bellucci, 2010, p. 14). This dialectical inter-phase between the European African practices could be viewed as having impacted on the psychological, economic, cultural, political, and other levels.

3.1. Goal-directed behavior

Recognizing the applicability of cultural construction in Afrocentric perspective implies that all thoughts, feelings, and human activities are the result of historical experiences that become an integral part of the culture (Eskay et al., 2012). As indicated in Glober et al. (2013), every behavior is purposeful and goal-directed on the experiencing individual, no matter how odd it may appear to another person. Wit and Dickinson (2009) state that for an action to be considered goal-directed, it must be instrumental, meaning that it has to be characterized by two properties: learned and controlled, by the causal relationship between the action or response, and its consequences or outcomes.

3.2. Personhood and cultural construction

Many writers observe that, despite Africa’s ethnic pluralism and diverse nature of cultures, underlying affinities in many areas of African life is the notion of communitarian, with its emphasis on the relationship between the self and the community (Gyekye, 2010; Kalumba, 2020). Furthermore, Ikeke (2014, p. 146) construes the nature of personhood by explaining the significance of the “self in the community” in the African society. Kalumba (2020, p. 137) perceives the communitarians in relation to individualists and regards the latter’s notion of a person as an “atomic, self-sufficient individual whose relationship to the community is contingent and voluntary.” In contrast, communitarians view the people as social beings who owe their development on community. This philosophical analysis of personhood does not necessarily deny the individual existence but views the individual as incomplete without the community. This thinking is manifested through shared features of the moral life and thought found in various African societies. This section also focuses on the recognition of the role of social interactions in the shaping of identity. In this regard, self-identity is viewed as not made in isolation, but that it is formed through negotiated relationship with others.

In explaining the place of rights in the African context, Molefe (2017) refers to the idea of personhood and conceives it as representing “an African approach to secure a life of dignity.” In this regard, Molefe (2017) cites Menkiti’s conception of personhood as defined through three different ideas. The first idea is the view of personhood as a claim about personal identity; second, by a claim about moral status and third, a claim about human beings who are morally upright or virtuous. Key to this view is the notion that, in the African context, rights are considered to be secondary to duties “because they will have a tendency to clash with the fundamental moral-social goal posited by a morality of duties of securing the well-being of all human beings entailed by this idea of personhood” (Ibid:1). Zelazo et al. (2016) acknowledge the individual differences in EFs and primarily attribute them to aspects of experience in childhood, such as the socioeconomic conditions in which children are reared, the differences in parental and out-of-home care that children receive, and the differences in cultural beliefs and practices in families and communities.

The conception of EFs along the lifespan suggests that at the elderly stage, the cognitive functioning finds itself to follow a declining trajectory. This may be viewed in contrast to the African notion of the elderly, which is mostly associated with an elderly person being the much respected repository of a society’s values and wisdom, which have remained unchanged over the ages (Mbele, 2004). The author articulates this assertion with emphasis on the value of intergenerational relationships. From the Afrocentric perspective, this kind of cultural construction could be viewed as implying that not all thoughts, feelings and human activities develop naturally but that most of them are the result of historical experiences that become an integral part of culture (Eskay et al., 2012). Zelazo et al. (2004) protect this view and state that it is by no means suggesting that the elderly are not capable of high levels of conscious reflection, but rather that their abilities are likely to be resource-demanding and effortful.

4. Implications

The current study examined the significance of the integration of African and Eurocentric perspectives in understanding and enhancing executive functioning skills in the African context. African philosophies and interventions have received little explicit attention in the design and evaluation of school readiness interventions for children with difficulties in their executive functioning skills, which poses threats to recognizing African knowledge as a proven science. Furthermore, the paper demonstrated that there is limited literature on empirical research published on EF using African philosophies as a theoretical component. Therefore, the dearth of empirical research in this studied subject calls for more measures to undertake empirical studies to close the gap between a theoretical perspective, and its application in the real world, to determine the relevance of EF using African philosophies to address the subject of disability.

This study also contributes to the international evidence linking Afrocentricity and the executive functioning skills of children. The discussion derived from the lens of Afrocentricity did not appear to be plagued by the issues that are often found with many other Eurocentric perspectives. Afrocentricity was able to address limitations on executive functioning skills in African children that were a particular concern in the African context. Considering, the absence of African epistemologies used to gain insight into predicting and exploring the subject of disability, no conclusions can be made about affected individuals in the African context. This suggests that avoiding broad generalizations about the causes and effects of disabilities could be achieved by making African epistemologies the foundation of any analysis that involves studying African people.

Sufficient literature regard moving towards understanding EFs in an African context using African philosophies to provide evidence of its appropriateness to fulfil a fundamental right of all children or learners despite their diversity in this context. The main finding of this paper suggests that it is not the mere promotion of EFs that is essential in the success of quality education, but that there is a need to acknowledge several contextual and interconnected dynamics which impact on education systems, namely environmental, cultural, and systemic.

5. Conclusion

It is essential to provide understanding, analyze and design solutions that are embraced within the Afrocentric ethos, instead of looking at African challenges using the Eurocentric perspective as a lens. The paper confirms that using African epistemologies to measure executive functioning skills in young African children has the potential to make fundamental contributions to the scientific body of knowledge. It is evident in this paper that the EFs that serve as a source and path to direct the thoughts and actions of individuals and that these need not be viewed in isolation from the spiritual dimension. Executive functions remain crucial for the skills that are necessary for purposeful, goal-directed, and future oriented activity. As a result, the process of addressing related impairments needs to be viewed as a collective responsibility.

Despite the diverse and lack of universal definitions, this paper also provides a communitarian view of EFs, emphasizing the collective conception and intervention in line with the African perspective. In this process, there was an acknowledgement of the significance of EFs along the developmental path beginning in early infancy, but reservations are raised regarding the view that such development plateaus at early adulthood and then declines thereafter. The significant assertion is made that, despite the societal evolution, the elderly are still regarded as the part of society that wield knowledge and wisdom. Emphasized in this paper is also the notion that, despite the numerous benefits of transnational activism, disability activism in Africa would be more effective if built around an Afrocentric ethos that considers the existential factors of parental care, families, society, and the experience of disability. The paper concludes by rejecting the individualist conception and interventions on learner EFs and advances the communitarian view that affords the holistic human functioning and development in the communal existence.

Author contributions

MM led all aspects of the development of the paper from conceptualization and designing the study, to identification of the study via databases to finalization. MS and ZM played a leading role in conceptualizing Afrocentricity as a lens to describe EFs and made contributions to other sections of the paper. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Smidts, D. P., Oosterlaan, J., Duivenvoorden, H. J., and Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2009). Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 981–993. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9327-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aarts, H., and Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 53–63. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, B. G., and Abubakar, A. (2016). “Acculturation in sub-Saharan Africa” in The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology. eds. D. L. Sam and J. W. Berry. 2nd ed (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 355–374.

Google Scholar

Adams, B. G., and van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2017). Identity and acculturation: the case for Africa. J. Psychol. Afr. 27, 115–121. doi: 10.1080/14330237.2017.1301699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alem, A. (2019). African systems of thought: whether they fit scientific knowledge. J. Philos. Cult. 7, 14–25. doi: 10.5897/JPC2018.0024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allain, P., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., and Verny, C. (2013). Executive functions in clinical and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Rev. Neurol. 169, 695–708. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2013.07.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altemeier, L., Jones, J., Abbott, R. D., and Berninger, V. W. (2006). Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Dev. Neuropsychol. 29, 161–173. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ameis, S. H., Blumberger, D. M., Croarkin, P. E., Mabbott, D. J., Lai, M.-C., Desarkar, P., et al. (2020). Treatment of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: a double-blind, sham-controlled, pilot trial. Brain Stimul. 13, 539–547. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. J. (2008). “Towards a developmental model of executive function” in Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective. eds. J. Anderson, R. Jacobs, and P. J. Anderson (New York: Taylor and Francis), 3–22.

Google Scholar

Anderson, J., Jacobs, R., and Anderson, P.J. (Eds.). (2008). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective. New York: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Andreou, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Durrleman, S., and Peristeri, E. (2020). Theory of mind, executive functions, and syntax in bilingual children with autism Spectrum disorder. Language 5, 1–24. doi: 10.3390/languages5040067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, E. F. (2010). Student engagement techniques: A handbook for college faculty. San Francisco: The Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Bay, D., and Daniel, H. (2003). The theory of trying and goal-directed behavior: the effect of moving up the hierarchy of goals. Psychol. Mark. 20, 669–684. doi: 10.1002/mar.10091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellucci, B. (2010). The state in Africa. Persp. World Rev. 2, 1–42.

Google Scholar

Berenguer, C., Miranda, A., Colomer, C., Baixauli, I., and Roselló, B. (2018). Contribution of theory of mind, executive functioning, and pragmatics to socialization behaviors of children with high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 430–441. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3349-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertollo, J. R., and Yerys, B. E. (2019). More than IQ: Executive function explains adaptive behavior above and beyond nonverbal IQ in youth with autism and lower IQ. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 124, 191–205.

Google Scholar

Bull, R., and Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C.-P., Gau, S. S.-F., and Lee, C.-C. (2019). Toward differential diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder using multimodal behavior descriptors and executive functions. Comput. Speech Lang. 56, 17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.csl.2018.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C., Prior, M., and Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 43, 785–796. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

C.R. De Luca and Leventer, R.J. (2008). Developmental trajectories of executive functions across the lifespan. (p. 24–56). In J. Anderson, R. Jacobs, and P.J. Anderson (eds.). (2008). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective. New York: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Dellis, A. M. (2009). The behaviour and development of infants with iron deficiency anaemia: Systematic observation of 9-month-old Pemban caregiver-infant dyads. [PhD thesis].

Google Scholar

Demetriou, E. A., DeMayo, M. M., and Guastella, A. J. (2019). Executive function in autism spectrum disorder: history, theoretical models, empirical findings, and potential as an endophenotype. Front. Psychol. 10:753. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology 64, 135–168.

Google Scholar

Downs, S. D. (2016). Restoring Ubuntu: Ecosystemic, biopsychosocial, Afrocentric networks for the trauma-healing of sexual violence survivors in eastern Congo. Hon. Projects 116, 1–91.

Google Scholar

El Wafa, H. E. A., Ghobashy, S. A. E. L., and Hamza, A. M. (2020). A comparative study of executive functions among children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and those with learning disabilities. MECPsych. 27, 1–9.

Google Scholar

Eskay, M., Onu, V. C., Igbo, J. N., Obiyo, N., and Ugwuanyi, L. (2012). Disability within the African culture. US-China Educ. Rev. B4, 473–484.

Google Scholar

Ezeamama, A. E., Bustinduy, A. L., Nkwata, A. K., Martinez, L., Pabalan, N., Boivin, M. J., et al. (2018). Cognitive deficits and educational loss in children with schistosome infection—a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0005524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Campos, M. D., Canabal, C., and Alba-Pastor, C. (2018). Executive functions in universal design for learning: moving towards inclusive education. Int. J. Incl. Educ. 24, 660–674. doi: 10.1080/13603116.2018.1474955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gau, S. S., Tseng, W. L., Tseng, W. I., Wu, Y. H., and Lo, Y. C. (2015). Association between microstructural integrity of frontostriatal tracts and school functioning: ADHD symptoms and executive function as mediators. Psychol. Med. 45, 529–543. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., and Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating 66 inventory of executive function professional manual. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Glober, H., Schenck, R., and Mbedzi, P. (2013). Person centred facilitation: Process, Theory & Practice. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J. A., Princiotta, D., and Otero, T. M. (2014). “Introduction: a history of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct” in Handbook of executive functioning. eds. S. Goldstein and J. A. Naglieri (New York: Springer)

Google Scholar

Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., and Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: A systematic review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:437. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gyekye, K. (2010). African ethics. In E. N. Zalta (ed.). The Stanford Encyclopaedia of philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/ [Accessed January 16, 2020].

Google Scholar

Haang’andu, P. (2019). “Towards an Afrocentric disability activism: Opportunities and challenges of transnationalizing disability advocacy in Africa” in Global perspectives on disability activism and advocacy. eds. B. K. Soldatic and K. Johnson (Routledge), 281–295.

Google Scholar

Harlow, J.M. (1869). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Boston: David Clapp & Son. Medical and Surgical Journal Office, 1–21. http://resource.nlm.nih.gov/66210360R.

Google Scholar

Hawkey, E. J., Tillman, R., Luby, J. L., and Barch, D. M. (2018). Preschool executive function predicts childhood resting-state functional connectivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and depression. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 927–936. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodge, D. R. (2011). Using spiritual interventions in practice: developing some guidelines from evidenced based practice. Soc. Work 56, 149–158. doi: 10.1093/sw/56.2.149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igboin, B. O. (2011). Colonialism and African cultural values. Afr. J. Hist. Cult. 3, 96–103.

Google Scholar

Ikeke, M. O. (2014). “Kami” and African Panpsychism: An Ecophilosophical Appraisal. Perspectives in the Arts and Humanities 4, 63–75.

Google Scholar

Isquith, P. K., Gioia, G. A., and Espy, K. A. (2004). Executive function in preschool children: Examination through everyday behavior. in Develop Neuropsychol. 26, 403–422. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, K., Murray, K., Spain, D., Walker, I., and Russell, A. (2019). Executive function: cognition and behaviour in adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 4181–4192. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04133-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalumba, K. M. (2020). A defense of Kwame Gyekye’s moderate communitarianism. Philos. Pap. 49, 137–158. doi: 10.1080/05568641.2019.1684840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H., and Zabelina, D. (2015). Cultural bias in assessment: can creativity assessment help? Int. J. Crit. Pedagogy 6, 129–148.

Google Scholar

Kouklari, E.-C., Tsermentseli, S., and Monks, C. P. (2019). Developmental trends of hot and cool executive function in school-aged children with and without autism spectrum disorder: links with theory of mind. Dev. Psychopathol. 31, 541–556. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laher, S., and Cockcroft, K. (ed.). (2013). Psychological assessment in South Africa. Johannesburg: Wits University Press.

Google Scholar

Mark, G. T., and Lyons, A. C. (2010). Maori healers' views on wellbeing: the importance of mind, body, spirit, family and land. Soc. Sci. Med. 70, 1756–1764. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martyr, A., and Clare, L. (2012). Executive function and activities of daily living in Alzheimer’s disease: a correlational meta-analysis. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 33, 189–203. doi: 10.1159/000338233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazibuko, X. I., and Chimbari, M. (2022). The effect of schistosomiasis and soil-transmitted helminths on expressive language skills among African preschool children. BMC Infect. Dis. 22, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12879-022-07260-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mbele, J. (2004). The elder in African society. J. Intergenerational Relatsh. 2, 53–61. doi: 10.1300/J194v02n03_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melrose, R. J., Jimenez, A. M., Riskin-Jones, H., Weissberger, G., Veliz, J., Hasratian, A. S., et al. (2018). Alterations to task positive and task negative networks during executive functioning in mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage Clin. 19, 970–981. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.06.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molefe, M. (2017). Personhood and rights in an African tradition. Politikon, 45, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/02589346.2017.1339176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgenstern, J., and Bates, M. E. (1999). Effects of executive function impairment on change processes and substance use outcomes in 12-step treatment. J. Stud. Alcohol 60, 846–855. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nazel, M. W., El-Morshedy, H., Farghaly, A., Shatat, H., and Barakat, R. (1999). Schistosoma mansoni infection and cognitive functions of primary school children, in Kafr El Sheikh, Egypt. J. Egypt. Public Health Assoc. 74, 97–119.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nwoye, A. (2015). African psychology and the Afrocentric paradigm to clinical diagnosis and treatment. S. Afr. J. Psychol. 45, 305–317. doi: 10.1177/0081246315570960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nwoye, A. (2017). An Afrocentric theory of human personhood. Psychol. Soc. 54, 42–66.

Google Scholar

Otero, T. M., and Barker, L. A. (2014). “The frontal lobes and executive functioning” in Handbook of executive functioning. eds. S. Goldstein and J. A. Naglieri (New York: Springer), 29–44.

Google Scholar

Prathanee, B., Thinkhamrop, B., and Dechongkit, S. (2007). Factors associated with specific language impairment and later language development during early life: a literature review. Clin. Pediatr. 46, 22–29. doi: 10.1177/0009922806297153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacolo-Gwebu, H., Chimbari, M., and Kalinda, C. (2019). Prevalence and risk factors of schistosomiasis and soil-transmitted helminthiases among preschool aged children (1–5 years) in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa: a cross-sectional study. Infect. Dis. Poverty 8, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s40249-019-0561-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sefotho, M. M. (2021). Basotho ontology of disability: an afrocentric onto-epistemology. Heliyon 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e06540

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shallice, T. (2004). “The fractionation of supervisory control” in The cognitive neurosciences. ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (London: MIT Press), 943–956.

Google Scholar

Shuttleworth-Edwards, A. B., Van Der Merwe, A. S., van Tonder, P., and Radloff, S. E. (2013). “WISC-IV test performance in the south African context: a collation of cross-cultural norms” in Psychological assessment in South Africa. eds. S. Laher and K. Cockcroft (Johannesburg: Wits University Press), 33–47.

Google Scholar

Spencer, M., Richmond, M. C., and Cutting, L. E. (2020). Considering the role of executive function in reading comprehension: a structural equation modeling approach. Sci. Stud. Read. 24, 179–199. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2019.1643868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T., and Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol. Res. 63, 289–298. doi: 10.1007/s004269900007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temple, C. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuropsychology. East Sussex: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Terer, C. C., Bustinduy, A. L., Magtanong, R. V., Muhoho, N., Mungai, P. L., Muchiri, E. M., et al. (2013). Evaluation of the health-related quality of life of children in Schistosoma haematobium-endemic communities in Kenya: a cross-sectional study. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 7:e2106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0002106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warnick, B., Drake, M., and Vidrine, S. (2015). Test review. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 33, 597–601. doi: 10.1177/0734282914564346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willoughby, M. T., Piper, B., Kwayumba, D., and McCune, M. (2019). Measuring executive function skills in young children in Kenya. Child Neuropsychol. 25, 425–444. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2018.1486395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wit, S. A., and Dickinson, A. (2009). Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal–human translational models. Psychol. Res. 73, 463–476. doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0230-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zainal, N. H., and Newman, M. G. (2018). Executive function and other cognitive deficits are distal risk factors of generalized anxiety disorder 9 years later. Psychol. Med. 48, 2045–2053. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P.D., Blair, C.B., and Willoughby, M.T. (2016). Executive function: Implications for education (NCER 2017–2000). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences.

Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., and Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol. 115, 167–183. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Afrocentric perspective, executive functions, goal-directed behavior, culture, communitarian

Citation: Masuku MM, Sithole MS and Mthembu Z (2023) Executive functions in understanding certain disabilities within an African context. Front. Educ. 8:1022453. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1022453

Received: 18 August 2022; Accepted: 21 April 2023;
Published: 11 May 2023.

Edited by:

Angela Page, The University of Newcastle, Australia

Reviewed by:

Paseka Andrew Mosia, National University of Lesotho, Lesotho
Zethembe Mseleku, University of Mpumalanga, South Africa

Copyright © 2023 Masuku, Sithole and Mthembu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mfundo Mandla Masuku, bWFzdWt1bUB1a3puLmFjLnph; Mbongeni Shadrack Sithole, U0lUSE9MRU0zQHVrem4uYWMuemE=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.