Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Educ., 25 October 2022
Sec. Teacher Education

Plant blindness: A focus on its biological basis

  • Department of Didactics of Mathematics and Experimental and Social Sciences, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Bilbao, Spain

Plant blindness or the inability to notice plants in one’s everyday life is a complex phenomenon in the field of science education. Although plant blindness is well documented in the literature, the underlying factors, whether biological or cultural, are still under research. Here I focus on its biological basis. That is, I review and discuss how plants’ own inherent characteristics cause effects on human visual and cognitive processes. Animals versus plant differences in human attention and memory are also addressed. Grounded on that knowledge, some recommendations for effective practice in plant science education emerge. I conclude that only when we understand human-plant relationships will we know how to enhance teaching and learning about plants.

Introduction

Human inability to see or notice plants in one’s everyday life is a phenomenon known as plant blindness. It also refers to failing to recognize the role of plants on earth and believing that plants are somehow inferior to animals (Wandersee and Schussler, 1999, 2001). See some other indicators for plant blindness in Table 1. Recently, a proposal to rename plant blindness as plant awareness disparity has arisen with the objective of eliminating “blindness” due to possible ableism (Parsley, 2020). However, plant awareness disparity is not a generally agreed term today (Thomas et al., 2021) and, therefore, here I will refer to this phenomenon as plant blindness.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Indicators for plant blindness as defined by Wandersee and Schussler (1999).

Plant blindness has attracted research for over 20 years and it is currently accepted as a real thing, a real problem–at least- in the Western society (Balding and Williams, 2016; Knapp, 2019). Numerous investigations have provided evidences for the indicators defined by Wandersee and Schussler (2001; here listed in Table 1). For instance, it is well known that students and the general public (i) identify and/or recall plant species more poorly than animal ones (e.g., Bebbington, 2005; Patrick and Tunnicliffe, 2011; Kaasinen, 2019; Zani and Low, 2022); (ii) find animals more interesting and attractive than plants (e.g., Kinchin, 1999; Nyberg et al., 2021; Pedrera et al., 2021); and (iii) have a poor knowledge about plants (e.g., Kubiatko et al., 2021; Fernández-Díaz, 2022).

The potential to successfully overcome plant blindness relies to a great extent on finding factors open to change. A consensus exist that it is a complex phenomenon where biological (including human cognition) and cultural factors are major drivers (see Balding and Williams, 2016 for a short review). While other authors highlight the cultural origin, in their description of plant blindness, Wandersee and Schussler (1999, 2001) mostly focused on the biological basis. They suggested that several plant characteristics such as their uniform color and grouping together, lack of movement and lack of a face, have an effect on how humans process visual information. Note that what we see is not simply a translation of the image projected on the retina; visual perception is involved in selecting, organizing and interpreting the information. Although these authors grounded their explanations on well-known visual principles, they lacked at that time of experimental support from the field of visual perception. This came later with Balas and Momsen (2014) and Kanske et al. (2013), who demonstrated that plants indeed capture human attention less effectively than animals. It is also worth mentioning the recall study by Schussler and Olzak (2008), which provided strong evidence for a cognitive basis; either visual attention or memory were suggested as processes involved in plant blindness.

Here, I will review some of the biological factors that were suggested by Wandersee and Schussler (1999, 2001) as a cause for plant blindness. First, I focus on how an inherent morphological feature of plants, being green, has an impact in human vision. Next, I address how plants change their visual apparency and its potential implications for human visual processes. And in third place, the differences between animals and plants in human attention and memory are reviewed. All this will lead me to conclude that only when we understand human-plant relationships will we know how to enhance teaching and learning about plants. Some recommendations for an effective education practice are also given.

How being green affects human vision

As noted by Wandersee and Schussler (1999), plants usually grow in populations and blend together, which creates a chromatic and spatial continuity for the human eye. In other words, at some places and to a given distance, the consequence to the human vision is a more or less homogeneous green scene (Figure 1). Moreover, other morphological features in addition to color contribute to the homogeneity of the environment. For example, plants living in the same habitat usually have a number of similar morphological characteristics (Prokopy and Owens, 1983). In this regard, Givnish (1979) found that, in particular habitats, the form of the leaves (small vs. large size, entire vs. toothed margins, and simple leaves vs. compound leaves) depends on specific factors such as moist, nutrient quantity, geographical area or successional stage of the habitat. As a result, different species of plants living in the same habitat may share same morphological characteristics (making plants less prominent to the human eye).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of green scenes that could lead to plant blindness. (A) A green scene where all vegetation (grasses and trees) show a similar shade of green. (B) A vast pine forest from a distance forming a more or less homogeneous green view. (C) An arid ecosystem with soil and vegetation being hard to differentiate at a given distance. (D) A winter view with green, brown and white patches from a distance. All photos were taken by the author in Spain.

What happens when we look at a homogeneous visual environment? According to the eye movement research, when an individual explores a scene, two phases are distinguished. The initial viewing consists of a fast registration of the spatial layout (2 sec); the subsequent phase is a longer and more in depth and element centered analysis (Pannasch et al., 2008). The initial phase is characterized by short fixations and long saccadic movements1. Then, the scanning strategy changes: fixation time increases and more fixations are given in certain regions of the scene; this implies that saccadic movements are shorter (Pannasch et al., 2008). Back to the topic, one could think that a plant scene with a predominantly monochromatic green color would have a poor second phase, as there are not many elements for the eye to fixate (Figure 1). Note that fixations are known to be related to uptake of visual information. In addition, when the eye finds no fixations (as in our green scene), the amplitude of saccadic movements increase, resulting in uncomfortable perception (Filin, 2006). As a result, the individual may well change the focus to another scene. Therefore, less fixations carries a reduction in the collected visual data and a likely change of focus to other target element/scene.

With respect to the selection of the target in a scene, selection is governed not only by the saliency of the elements in the scene (this was already pointed out by Wandersee and Schussler, 2001), but also by the tasks to be carried out by the observer and the emotional aspects transmitted by the scene (Pannasch et al., 2008). Then, one could also think that an individual that suffers from plant blindness would have less elements to focus on than an individual that appreciates plants, as no task or emotion is leading the former.

Plants’ abilities to change their visual signal

Plants adapt their visual signal values to the environment by modifying their apparency. This can be done in two ways: becoming more or less visible. This last one was proposed by Wandersee and Schussler (2001) as a factor for plant blindness. I discuss this topic in more detail below.

Decreasing their visual apparency, plants avoid recognition and, thus, predation. It is well-known that plants can conceal themselves from animals such as birds (Fadzly et al., 2009) and butterflies (Niu et al., 2017). For decreasing their apparency, two main strategies are distinguished: hiding in refuges and crypsis (Mortensen, 2013). Regarding the first strategy, some plants grow in spatial refuges, that is, in areas where herbivores cannot reach such as abrupt slopes and areas above or below the browse line for the herbivores (Milchunas and Noy-Meir, 2002). Other plants hide in temporal refuges, which consists of growing or flowering when herbivores are not active (Mortensen, 2013). With regard to the second strategy, some plants can camouflage themselves (crypsis) among other similar colored plants, which is known as background matching (Niu et al., 2018). And some other plants resemble natural elements, e.g., stones, for avoiding recognition (Mortensen, 2013).

Do plants become less apparent for humans when they reduce their visual signal to hide from herbivores? A priori, the answer seems to be yes. However, as far as I know, no specific investigation has been made (no comparisons with cryptic animals either).

On the other hand, becoming more visible is usually a reproduction strategy for plants to attract pollinators and seed dispersers. Bird-pollinated flowers tend to be red and insect-pollinated ones, blue (Caro and William, 2017). Similarly, mammals tend to disperse yellow or green fruits and birds, white, black, blue, or red ones (Caro and William, 2017). Moreover, trichromacy in primates is thought to help them detect fruits against a background of green leaves, young leaves against mature leaves, etc. (Regan et al., 2001). Becoming more visible is also a way to emit warning signals to avoid a predator attack (aposematism). Yellow, orange, red, brown, black, and white are often used by spiny and poisonous plants as defense (Lev-Yadun, 2009).

A number of studies have analyzed how prominent features (colorful flowers and fruits) enhance human interest, attraction, and willingness to protect plants (e.g., Tunnicliffe, 2001; Adamo et al., 2021; Hùla and Flegr, 2021). If one applies the knowledge reviewed in the previous section (How being green affects human vision), fixations in flowers and other salient morphological characteristics would be expected to be longer and more abundant. That is, more visual data are captured, and plant blindness could be minimized.

Visual attention and memory: Animals versus plants

Visual attention and memory were essential parts of the explanations for plant blindness by Wandersee and Schussler (2001). I next review how humans detect and memorize animals compared to plants according to the advances in the literature.

First, one should consider how the conceptual knowledge is organized in our brain. That is, if animals and plants fall into the same “compartment.” Caramazza and Shelton (1998) suggested that our brain is organized within the distinct categories of animals, plants, and artifacts. These were posteriorly condensed in the literature into animates and inanimates.

New et al. (2007) found that changes to animates (including animals and among them, humans) were visually detected by people more frequently and more quickly than changes to inanimates (including plants, vehicles, tools, and others). It was then concluded that humans give preferential visual attention to animates. Regarding memory, Nairne et al. (2013) found that animates are better remembered than inanimates, but the study did not include plants. In fact, studies on animates versus inanimates do not specifically compare plants versus animals. Moreover, most of them use fruits (e.g., Jackson and Calvillo, 2013) and flowers (e.g., Guerrero and Calvillo, 2016) as the only images of plants.

Interestingly, these authors (New et al., 2007; Nairne et al., 2013) rely their explanations on an evolutionary hypothesis. From a fitness perspective, animals have survival and reproductive value for humans; animals are potential predators and food and other humans are potential mating partners and competitors for resources. Therefore, rapid and successful identification of animates would have been a selective pressure for adaptation of visual and neural mechanisms in humans (New et al., 2007; Nairne et al., 2013). Following that hypothesis, one could think that plants would also have been a selective pressure (as they are potential food, sometimes poisonous or stinging, shelter, tooling material, etc.); however, questions remain open.

Following Nairne et al. (2013), it is fundamental to deconstruct the animacy dimension, because still we do not know which the necessary and sufficient properties for animates to produce the effect in our brain are. According to the authors, “cognitive biases may require agency (the ability to initiate causal action), movement, mental states (such as knowing or emotion), or even the ability to communicate in order for something to be considered animate.” Interestingly, Kinchin (1999) found that the animal feature that generated most interest in a sample of 162 students was movement. Motion is exhibited in plants at all scales, from stomatal opening and closing on the leaf to branch movements in trees. However, most movements of plants are unnoticeable for the human eye because they are too slow for us to perceive them (Forterre, 2013). And, therefore, they are not expected to attract people’s attention. In any case, future advances in this field seem indispensable to understand the phenomenon of plant blindness.

Recommendations for effective education practice

Some science instruction time could be devoted to observing green homogeneous plant scenes. Giving a task (observing) would give the eye elements to focus on (fixations). That could be made during a field trip to the forest, for example. The scientific practice of observation could be accompanied by taking data and making inferences and/or classification and description practices. Note that all those scientific practices accord with inquiry based learning. To finish, students could reflect about what they saw in the forest under the guidance of educators (reflective learning). Making students aware of their own plant blindness would be the ultimate goal of the reflection.

Prior training on plant blindness seems necessary in order for educators to be able to carry out interventions in relation to this phenomenon. This can be done during the university teacher training stage. Plant blindness fits well in the area of science didactics, as an obstacle to learning about plants. Pre-service teachers should be aware of the phenomenon, its “symptoms” and its potential biological and cultural causes. Furthermore, when advances in the cognitive area allow us to understand how attention and memory biases lead to better identification and recalling of animals vs. plants, we will be able to advance in the teaching of plants.

Conclusion

In a sustainable future world, plant blindness has no place (Thomas et al., 2021). From this reflection, I conclude that human-plant relationships in plant blindness are poorly known, and that research progresses on how human brain processes plants is a fundamental aspect to be up-to-date with from science education. Only that knowledge will guide us on how to enhance teaching and learning about plants. It is my hope that this perspective captures the attention of research aimed at deepening into the biological factors in plant blindness.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was funded by the University of the Basque Country (GIU21/031).

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adamo, M., Chialva, M., Calevo, J., Bertoni, F., Dixon, K., and Mammola, S. (2021). Plant scientists’ research attention is skewed towards colourful, conspicuous and broadly distributed flowers. Nat. Plants 7, 574–578. doi: 10.1038/s41477-021-00912-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balas, B., and Momsen, J. L. (2014). Attention “blinks” differently for plants and animals. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 13, 437–443. doi: 10.1187/cbe.14-05-0080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balding, M., and Williams, K. J. (2016). Plant blindness and the implications for plant conservation. Conserv. Biol. 30, 1192–1199. doi: 10.1111/cobi.12738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bebbington, A. (2005). The ability of A-level students to name plants. J. Biol. Educ. 39, 63–67. doi: 10.1080/00219266.2005.9655963

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., and Shelton, J. (1998). Domain specific knowledge system in the brain: The animate-inanimate distinction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 1–34. doi: 10.1162/089892998563752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caro, T., and William, L. A. (2017). Interspecific visual signaling in animals and plants: A functional classification. Philos. Trans. B 372:20160344. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fadzly, N., Jack, C., Schaefer, H. M., and Burns, K. C. (2009). Ontogenetic colour changes in an insular tree species: Signaling to extinct browsing birds? New Phytol. 184, 495–501. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2009.02926.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Díaz, M. (2022). Pre-service teachers’ ideas and misconceptions about the nutrition, reproduction and importance of plants: A case study in Spain. J. Biomed. Res. Environ. Sci. 3, 930–933. doi: 10.37871/jbres1534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filin, V. A. (2006). Videoecology: What is good for the eye, and what is bad. Moscow: MC “Videoecology”.

Google Scholar

Forterre, Y. (2013). Slow, fast and furious: Understanding the physics of plant movements. J. Exp. Bot. 64, 4745–4760. doi: 10.1093/jxb/ert230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Givnish, T. J. (1979). “On the adaptive significance of leaf form,” in Topics in plant population biology, eds O. T. Solbrig, S. Jain, G. B. Johnson, and P. H. Raven (New York, NY: Columbia University Press), 375–407. doi: 10.1007/978-1-349-04627-0_17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerrero, G., and Calvillo, D. P. (2016). Animacy increases second target reporting in a rapid serial visual presentation task. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1832–1838. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1040-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hùla, M., and Flegr, J. (2021). Habitat selection and human aesthetic responses to flowers. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3:E5. doi: 10.1017/ehs.2020.66

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, R. E., and Calvillo, D. P. (2013). Evolutionary relevance facilitates visual information processing. Evol. Psychol. 11, 1011–1026. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaasinen, A. (2019). Plant species recognition skills in Finnish students and teachers. Educ. Sci. 9:85. doi: 10.3390/educsci9020085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., Schönfelder, S., and Wessa, M. (2013). Emotional modulation of the attentional blink and the relation to interpersonal reactivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:641. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinchin, I. (1999). Investigating secondary-school girls’ preferences for animals or plants: A Simple ‘head-to-head’ comparison using two unfamiliar organisms. J. Biol. Educ. 33, 95–99.

Google Scholar

Knapp, S. (2019). Are humans really blind to plants? Plants People Planet 1, 164–168. doi: 10.1002/ppp3.36

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubiatko, M., Fanèovièová, J., and Prokop, J. (2021). Factual knowledge of students about plants is associated with attitudes and interest in botany. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 43, 1426–1440. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2021.1917790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lev-Yadun, S. (2009). “Aposematic (warning) coloration in plants,” in Plant-environment interactions. From sensory plant biology to active plant behavior, ed. F. Baluska (Berlin: Springer), 167–202. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-89230-4_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milchunas, D. G., and Noy-Meir, I. (2002). Grazing refuges, external avoidance of herbivory and plant diversity. Oikos 99, 113–130. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2002.990112.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortensen, B. (2013). Plant resistance against herbivory. Nat. Educ. Knowl. 4:5.

Google Scholar

Nairne, J. S., VanArsdall, J. E., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Cogdill, M., and LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of animacy. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2099–2105. doi: 10.1177/0956797613480803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

New, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 16598–16603. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703913104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, Y., Chen, Z., Stevens, M., and Sun, H. (2017). Divergence in cryptic leaf colour provides local camouflage in an alpine plant. Proc. R. Soc. B 284:20171654. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, Y., Sun, H., and Stevens, M. (2018). Plant camouflage: Ecology, evolution, and implications. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 608–618. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, E., Brkovic, I., and Sanders, D. (2021). Beauty, memories and symbolic meaning: Swedish student teachers views of their favourite plant and animal. J. Biol. Educ. 55, 31–44. doi: 10.1080/00219266.2019.1643761

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pannasch, S., Helmert, J. R., Roth, K., Herbold, A. K., and Walter, H. (2008). Visual fixation durations and saccade amplitudes: Shifting relationship in a variety of conditions. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2, 1–19. doi: 10.16910/jemr.2.2.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsley, K. M. (2020). Plant awareness disparity: A case for renaming plant blindness. Plants People Planet 2, 598–601. doi: 10.1002/ppp3.10153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, P., and Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2011). What plants and animals do early childhood and primary students’ name? Where do they see them? J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 20, 630–642. doi: 10.1007/s10956-011-9290-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedrera, O., Ortega, U., Ruiz-González, A., Díez, J. R., and Barrutia, O. (2021). Branches of Plant blindness and their relationship with biodiversity conceptualisation among secondary students. J. Biol. Educ. 1–26. doi: 10.1080/00219266.2021.1933133 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokopy, R. J., and Owens, E. D. (1983). Visual detection of plants by herbivorous insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 28, 337–364. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.28.010183.002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regan, B. C., Julliot, C., Simmen, B., Viénot, F., Charles–Dominique, P., and Mollon, J. D. (2001). Fruits, foliage and the evolution of primate colour vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 229–283. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schussler, E. E., and Olzak, L. A. (2008). It’s not easy being green: Student recall of plant and animal images. J. Biol. Educ. 42, 112–119. doi: 10.1080/00219266.2008.9656123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, H., Ougham, H., and Sanders, D. (2021). Plant blindness and sustainability. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 23, 41–57. doi: 10.1108/IJSHE-09-2020-0335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2001). Talking about plants–comments of primary school groups looking at plant exhibits in a botanical garden. J. Biol. Educ. 36, 27–34. doi: 10.1080/00219266.2001.9655792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandersee, J. H., and Schussler, E. E. (1999). Preventing plant blindness. Am. Biol. Teach. 61, 82–86. doi: 10.2307/4450624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandersee, J. H., and Schussler, E. E. (2001). Towards a theory of plant blindness. Plant Sci. Bull. 27, 2–9.

Google Scholar

Zani, G., and Low, J. (2022). Botanical priming helps overcome plant blindness on a memory task. J. Environ. Psychol. 81:101808. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: plant blindness, plant awareness disparity, science education, effective teaching, cognitive science

Citation: Achurra A (2022) Plant blindness: A focus on its biological basis. Front. Educ. 7:963448. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.963448

Received: 07 June 2022; Accepted: 10 October 2022;
Published: 25 October 2022.

Edited by:

Paco Calvo, University of Murcia, Spain

Reviewed by:

Pinelopi Papadopoulou, University of Western Macedonia, Greece
Alexandros Amprazis, University of Western Macedonia, Greece

Copyright © 2022 Achurra. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ainara Achurra, ainara.achurra@ehu.eus

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.