Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 06 June 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology

Support Strategy for Executive Function in Children of Low-Income Families: The Marshmallow Test Has a Learning Value

  • School of Education, Kwansei Gakuin University, Nishinomiya, Japan

Executive function (EF) development is an important part of infancy and childhood, promoting success in later life. Developing targeted methods is desirable to enable EF. There is an urgent need for easily implementable proposals to ameliorate educational disparities between income groups. EF is a skill that its possessor can use to inhibit thoughts and emotions. There is a relationship between EF and the prefrontal cortex. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children’s (WISC-IV) Working Memory Index (WMI) task for thinking and the marshmallow test for emotions have been used to measure EF, but these tests themselves carry a load of inhibitions. Therefore, we can assume a natural value in learning to improve on the test. The marshmallow test does not require parental expertise in preparation or implementation, and it entails little financial burden. If there is a potential value in learning how to do better on the test, it will be easy for parents in low-income families to help their children improve. I examined whether the marshmallow test itself can support EF. Measurements of brain activity in the prefrontal cortex over 8 days showed that the marshmallow test was more active than the WMI task or the abacus task. Therefore, the results suggest that continued use of the marshmallow test can support EF even in low-income families.

Introduction

Executive Function and Family Support in Childhood

The development of executive function (EF) in childhood has become increasingly understood as important for the future success of children (Casey et al., 2011; Moffitt et al., 2011; Moriguchi, 2019). However, it is difficult to know how its development can be supported (Moriguchi, 2021). It is more difficult for children in low-income families to develop EF (Evans and Schamberg, 2009; Tough, 2012). Therefore, it is desirable to identify easier means of supporting their EF development. EF is a necessary socio-emotional skill for goal attainment and can be described as the skill of suppressing one’s thoughts and emotions (Moriguchi, 2021).

The marshmallow test is often used to test the emotional suppression function of EF (Mischel et al., 1989). Similarly, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children’s (WISC-IV) Working Memory Index (WMI) task has been used for thought suppression, and the abacus task is possible (Watanabe, 2021). Recent studies showed that EF is related to the activity of the prefrontal cortex through the measurement of brain activity (Moriguchi and Hiraki, 2013).

The marshmallow test is rooted in a psychological experiment on children’s perseverance developed by Mischel et al. (1989). The experiment results have been used to predict children’s future success (Mischel et al., 1988; Shoda et al., 1990; Michaelson and Munakata, 2020). The test activates the prefrontal cortex (Mischel, 2014), which is consistent with the observation that EF is activated in the prefrontal cortex (Moriguchi and Hiraki, 2011).

Various ways of supporting EF have been explored, but we remain in the dark overall (Moriguchi, 2021). Although much attention has been paid to the results of the marshmallow test itself, there is surprisingly little focus on the learning value of the marshmallow test. Because its design forces children to persevere, running the test itself should have a learning value. If the marshmallow test has a supportive value for EF, it can be used to support EF. In addition, the marshmallow test itself can be easily administered at home, making it usable by and for low-income families.

This study demonstrates the validity of the hypothesis that the marshmallow test itself has value for supporting EFs.

The validity of this hypothesis is determined by comparing brain activity in the prefrontal cortex. Specifically, I examine this hypothesis by comparing it with other tests of EF, such as the WMI (forward digit span and mathematics) task and the abacus task.

This study employed the case study method, useful for deriving new hypotheses (George and Bennett, 2005). Further, it has already been suggested that case studies are sufficiently valuable (Saito, 2013; Yin, 2018).

Materials and Methods

Subjects

This single case study included one 10-year-old girl (in the fourth grade) who had passed the marshmallow test.

Ethics

The procedures involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Kwansei Gakuin University Committee for Regulations for Behavioral Research with Human Participants (Approval Number: 2020-06; Approval Date: June 12, 2020). The participants’ legal guardian or next of kin provided written informed consent to participate in this study.

Methods

Prefrontal cortex activity was measured during each task [the marshmallow test, WISC-IV WMI (forward digit span and mathematics) task, and abacus task].

Brain Activity Measurement

Each task was performed eight times, once each day for 1 week and once more after 1 day’s rest. Brain activity was measured during task performance. Data were omitted in cases of clearly abnormal results or where data collection was impossible. A relaxation period was provided wherein one participant sat in a chair with the brain activity device attached for several minutes or more before the task activity began. The rest period was defined as a time when the total hemoglobin (Hb) of the left and right hemispheres was less than 0.1 mMol × mm for 10 s. If there was no rest period, the measurement was deleted from the data. A two-channel fNIRS instrument took the measurements. Total Hb in mMol × mm was measured for the left and right sides.

Task/Protocol

WISC-IV WMI (Forward Digit Span, Mathematics) Task

This task was performed according to the procedure described in the general manual by Wechsler (2010). The forward digit span task was performed first, and the mathematics task second. The tasks were completed when the cancel condition was met, which entailed varying execution times for the tasks. The contents of each task are given below.

Forward Digit Span Task

In this task, the experimenter reads out a series of numbers to the subject who repeats then the numbers verbatim in the given order. There are eight questions in total; each question is further divided into two sub-questions, each of which comprises two sequences of equal length. Each sequence comprises 2–9 numbers, beginning with two-digit numbers and increasing by one digit for each number. If both sub-questions are answered incorrectly, the task is aborted.

Mathematics Task

The experimenter reads an arithmetic question to the subject in this task. There are 34 questions, and if the subject answers four consecutive questions incorrectly, the task is terminated.

Abacus Task

This activity used question sheets from the Japan Chamber of Commerce and Industry’s Level 5 addition and subtraction materials. There was a 5-min time limit for each task, and the task contents were as follows.

One question comprises 10 sets of four-digit integer values (mixed addition and subtraction).

Marshmallow Test

This test was conducted using the common marshmallow test method. A time limit of 15 min is set. A desk and a chair are in a room. A marshmallow is placed on a plate on the desk, and a bell is placed on the table. The subject sits in the chair. The experimenter says, “If you can wait for 15 min, you will get another marshmallow. If you cannot wait, you will not get another marshmallow. If you can’t wait, you can ring the bell. If you do, I will come back, but you will not get another marshmallow. Do not leave your chair.” If the subject understood, the experimenter left the room and began the experiment.

Calculation

fNIRS was used to measure brain activity in the prefrontal cortex for each task in the subject girl. The mean value for each task was calculated. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to test for significant differences. These values were calculated using IBM SPSS Statistics ver. 27.0.1.0.

Cerebral blood flow in the prefrontal cortex was measured using the HOT-2000, a two-channel fNIRS device manufactured in Japan (price: 198,000 yen), which is a wearable device and can be used outside the laboratory and at home.

The HOT-2000 takes measurements as follows. Changes in blood flow associated with brain activity are monitored using near-infrared light. Blood flow increases where the brain is most active. The measurement is performed by implanting two sensor blocks in the brain’s target area of the prefrontal cortex. To obtain the measurement results, I first set the baseline correction to 0 and examined the average brain activity during the task {[Σ(f(x) – min f(x))/total milliseconds], x: time, f(x): total hemoglobin}. Total hemoglobin was acquired every millisecond.

Results

This was a case study of a single child, which included a within-individual comparison based on repeated experimental data.

Figure 1 and Table 1 show the mean values for the marshmallow test, forward digit span task, math task, and abacus task. Note that for comparison with the abacus task, the marshmallow test was divided into three periods (first, second, and third section) of 5 min each.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Averages for marshmallow test, forward digit span task, math task, and abacus task.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Averages for marshmallow test, forward digit span task, math task, and abacus task.

The mean values for the left-side brain activity were in an ascending order of forward digit span task, math task, abacus task, and marshmallow test, whereas that of the right-side brain activity were in an ascending order of forward digit span task, abacus task, math task, and marshmallow test.

Table 2 shows the results of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test significant differences between left and right brain activity for each task.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The p-values for each task of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

The first section of the marshmallow test was significantly different in left-side brain activity from the results for the forward digit span task, mathematics task, and abacus task, while the second and third sections of the marshmallow test results were significantly different from those for the forward digit span task and mathematics task. On the right side of the brain, the results from the first and third sections of the marshmallow test were significantly different from those for the forward digit span task, mathematics task, and abacus task, while the results for the second section was significantly different from those for the forward digit span task and abacus task.

These results indicate that the marshmallow test activates the prefrontal cortex on the right, left, or both more than other approaches. This occurs even though it is performed almost every day.

Discussion

The Value of Learning the Marshmallow Test

I examined whether the marshmallow test itself has any learning value. To recall, the marshmallow test is a test to determine whether a child can persevere over a short period in the hope of a reward (Mischel et al., 1989). Thus, this test places a burden of patience on its participants, although not beyond the scope needed for a psychological experiment. The tests were not conducted consecutively to avoid an undue burden. The present results, obtained through 1 week of study, indicate that the prefrontal cortex was significantly activated during the test even in children whose cortex was previously cleared of major activity. Brain activity stabilizes or declines as understanding deepens (Watanabe, 2008). The abacus produces significant activation (Watanabe, 2021). It should be noted that brain activity is more active than abacus activity. In the marshmallow test, they are just waiting with optimism, but they are not exactly doing anything. Additionally, this time around, a child who had already passed the marshmallow test was targeted. Therefore, the activity in the brain should be important. Experimental results reveal that “just being patient with hope” is more active than specific activities, such as the abacus task or WMI task. This perspective is groundbreaking.

EF is low in children of low-income families (Evans and Schamberg, 2009; Tough, 2012). Attachment is an important condition for support (Cicchetti and Blender, 2006; Dozier et al., 2006; Gunnar and Fisher, 2006). In addition, it is difficult for low-income families to provide advanced knowledge to their children or find the funding or time to instruct them. The marshmallow test requires only two marshmallows, a clock, a room, a desk, a chair, a plate, and a bell. Further, the test administrator need not monitor the child for the full 15 min, and in the end, praise is all that is needed. The object need not be marshmallows but cookies, stickers, or any number of other things. There are many test variations, which can easily be done every day. Low-income families often do not know how to support EF in their children or have insufficient resources, even when they are aware. Even for low-income families, the marshmallow test may be an easy first step.

This time the child is one who passed the marshmallow test. That is, she is a child who was able to endure for 15 min; however, if one cannot stand it for 15 min, I do not think it will be a problem. This is because family support thus requires only a little parental and autonomous support for the child (Bernier et al., 2010). Children adapt to their environments (Moriguchi, 2021). Experiments have shown that reliable experimenters can wait three times longer than trustworthy ones (Michaelson and Munakata, 2016). The suggestion of a casual implementation of the marshmallow test provides effective autonomy support for parents and children.

It is less important whether a child completes the marshmallow test, but it is hoped that by continuously conducting the marshmallow test, a relationship of trust between parents and children can be established. Using this as advantage, EF can gradually be fostered by learning patience during the marshmallow test.

Although the present study was conducted on a single subject to show the activation of brain activity, it is a significant first step to show the value of implementing the marshmallow test in EF development.

Limitations

This study had some limitations. Only one child was included in the study. Larger populations should be tested in the future. Moreover, showing the superiority of the test compared to other tests is necessary.

The 2-channel fNIRS device has the advantage of being easily measurable. However, only the overall measurement of the prefrontal cortex is possible. Therefore, it is difficult to clarify the detailed relationship between EF and marshmallow test and other tasks; thus, using a multi-channel fNIRS device, such as one with 10 or 16 channels, for measurement and examining them in more detail is necessary.

Ripple Effects

This study is expected to impact research fields related to family support for FE significantly. It will also be valuable in providing new perspectives on educational psychology, cognitive psychology, and neuropsychology.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available owing to privacy and ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to NW, bm9idWtpQGt3YW5zZWkuYWMuanA=.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Kwansei Gakuin University Committee for Regulations for Behavioral Research with Human Participants. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

The author made substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work or the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data for the work.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI under Grant Number 22K02535.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Enago (http://www.enago.jp/) for the English language review.

References

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Somerville, L. H., Gotlib, I. H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N. T., Askren, M. K., et al. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. PNAS. 108, 14998–15003. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108561108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Blender, J. A. (2006). A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on resilience: implications for the developing brain, neural plasticity, and preventive interventions. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 248–258. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lindhiem, O., Gordon, M. K., Manni, M., Sepulveda, S., et al. (2006). Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: an example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. J. Soc. Issues. 62, 767–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00486.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Schamberg, M. A. (2009). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. PNAS. 106, 6545–6549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811910106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, A. L., and Bennett, A. (2005). Case Studies and Theory Development in the Social Sciences. Massachusetts, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Gunnar, M. R., and Fisher, P. A. (2006). Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 651–677. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michaelson, L. E., and Munakata, Y. (2016). Trust matters: seeing how an adult treats another person influences preschoolers’ willingness to delay gratification. Dev. Sci. 19, 1011–1019. doi: 10.1111/desc.12388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michaelson, L. E., and Munakata, Y. (2020). Same data set, different conclusions: preschool delay of gratification predicts later behavioral outcomes in a preregistered study. Psychol. Sci. 31, 193–201. doi: 10.1177/0956797619896270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmallow Test: Understanding Self-control and How to Master it. New York, NY: Random House.

Google Scholar

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., and Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 687. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., and Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science 244, 933–938. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS. 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y. (2019). Zibun wo Control Suru Chikara. [The power to control oneself]. Tokyo: Kodansha Ltd.

Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y. (2021). Kodomo no Hattatsu Kakusa. [Child Development Disparities]. Kyoto: PHP Institute, Inc.

Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y., and Hiraki, K. (2011). Longitudinal development of prefrontal function during early childhood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y., and Hiraki, K. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: a review of NIRS studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:867. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saito, S. (2013). Jireikenkyu Toiu Paradaimu. [The Paradigm of Case Studies]. Tokyo: Iwasaki Gakujutu Shuppansya.

Google Scholar

Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., and Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions. Dev. Psychol. 26, 978. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tough, P. (2012). How Children Succeed: Grit, Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character. San Francisco, SF: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Google Scholar

Watanabe, M. (2008). “Kodo no ninchikagaku [Cognitive science of behavior],” in Ninshiki to Kodo no Nokagaku [Brain Science of Cognition and Behavior], eds K. Tanaka (Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press), 203–263.

Google Scholar

Watanabe, N. (2021). Easy abacus calculation in early childhood to support executive function: an educational pilot case study of comparing brain activity in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Educ. 6:757588. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2021.757588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2010). (Translated and Edited by the Japanese WISC-IV Publication Committee). Nihonban WISC-IV Zisshi⋅Saiten Manual [Japanese Version of WICS-IV Implementation and Scoring Manual]. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha Co., Ltd.

Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (2018). Case Study Research and Applications. California, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Keywords: executive function, prefrontal cortex, low-income, marshmallow test, fNIRS

Citation: Watanabe N (2022) Support Strategy for Executive Function in Children of Low-Income Families: The Marshmallow Test Has a Learning Value. Front. Educ. 7:875254. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.875254

Received: 14 February 2022; Accepted: 04 April 2022;
Published: 06 June 2022.

Edited by:

Douglas F. Kauffman, Medical University of the Americas – Nevis, United States

Reviewed by:

Agnese Capodieci, University of Padua, Italy
Sonia Brito-Costa, Instituto Politécnico de Coimbra, Portugal

Copyright © 2022 Watanabe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nobuki Watanabe, bm9idWtpQGt3YW5zZWkuYWMuanA=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.