Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 30 June 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Closure and Reopening of Schools and Universities During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Prevention and Control Measures, Support Strategies for Vulnerable Students and Psychosocial Needs View all 15 articles

Academic Self-Regulation, Chronotype and Personality in University Students During the Remote Learning Phase due to COVID-19

  • 1Department for Didactics of Biology, Eberhard Karls University Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 2Department for Didactics of Biology, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 3Osnabrück University, Osnabrück, Germany
  • 4University Klagenfurt, Klagenfurt, Austria

During the COVID-19 shutdown phase in Germany, universities stopped presence teaching and students had to turn to digital instruction. To examine their capability to cope with the changed learning situation, we assessed how basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, motivational regulation, vitality, and self-efficacy of 228 German biology-teaching students (75% female) relate to their chronotype and personality (Big Five). Specifically, we were interested in possible effects of chronotype and personality dimensions on variables related to successful remote learning. Since the pandemic and remote learning will accompany teaching and learning at university in 2021, predictors of successful remote learning need to be identified to support student learning optimally in digital learning environments. In our study, morning-oriented, conscientious, and open students with low neuroticism seem to better cope with the shutdown environment due to vitality, self-efficacy, and partly their self-determined motivation. Moreover, our findings implicate students might need different support depending on their chronotype and personality during the digital learning phase.

Introduction

During the COVID-19 pandemic, the German government-imposed restrictions to limit viral transmission. As a part of this strategy, universities implemented online teaching and required students to work from home. In combination with asynchronous learning arrangements, students were able to change their sleep-wake cycle to sleeping later and longer (Staller and Randler, 2020). This led to a natural approximation to the inherent biological rhythm. The chronotype as the measurable manifestation of the biological rhythm describes the time of day at which a person is best able to cope with particularly challenging tasks. It is becoming an increasingly important predictor of academic achievement (see e.g., Arbabi et al., 2015; Tonetti et al., 2015). Since students were able to live in accordance with their own biological rhythm the conditions for academic success with respect to the chronotype may have improved during the restriction phase in Germany. This is corroborated by a study of Horzum et al. (2014). These authors suggested that online teaching with free-choice time schedules diminished the achievement discrepancies between chronotypes. Besides their chronotype, students’ motivation has a crucial impact on academic achievement (Ryan and Deci, 2017). With the sleep schedule in line with the inherent biological needs rather than with social expectations, the pandemic and remote learning phase open a rare opportunity to study the relationship between chronotype dependent characteristics and motivation-related constructs such as basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, motivational regulation, vitality, and self-efficacy (see e.g., Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Richardson et al., 2012; Kirmizi, 2015; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Additionally, we examine personality dimensions (Big Five) which are related to chronotype (e.g., DeYoung et al., 2007; Tonetti et al., 2009; Randler and Saliger, 2011) as well as motivational regulation (e.g., Müller et al., 2006; Komarraju et al., 2009) to provide a more holistic picture. Our study aimed at offering a first exploratory insight into the relationships of chronotype, well-being and motivation in the situation of asynchronous learning arrangements. Our findings provide valuable guidance for the design of digital learning environments and further analyses during remote learning.

Theoretical and Empirical Background

Chronotype and Circadian Preference

Chronotype is a personality-like trait in which humans are categorized according to their daytime preference, their wake and bedtimes, or their midpoint of sleep on days off. According to the current state of research, chronotype is divided into either demarcated types (morning type, evening type, or neither type; e.g., Adan et al., 2012; Horne and Östberg, 1976) or determined by a score on a continuum (from morningness to eveningness; Roenneberg et al., 2003). As a personality trait, it refers to the preferred daytime for physical or cognitive activities, thereby indicating the particularly efficient periods. Morning-oriented people reach their peak performance in the morning while evening-oriented people show their best performance in the late afternoon (Kerkhof and Van Dongen, 1996; Roenneberg et al., 2003). Chronotype differs from sleep duration by its inherent trait of timing that is irrelevant to the length of sleep (Adan et al., 2012). In the current study, we view chronotype as a unidimensional construct with a parametric score.

Motivation in Organismic Integration Theory

In Organismic Integration Theory, a sub-theory of self-determination theory (Ryan and Deci, 2017), motivational qualities and regulations that differ in their degree of perceived self-determination during an action are described. The prototype of a self-determined action is the intrinsically motivated action (Ryan and Deci, 2002). Here, an individual only pursues the goal of performing the action itself and no contingencies outside the action (Guay et al., 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2017). The action is performed to feel an inherent satisfaction and pleasure (Ryan and Deci, 2017). Extrinsically motivated actions, on the other hand, are performed to achieve a goal that is separable from the action (Guay et al., 2000; Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). They are therefore described as instrumental (Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002). However, this does not mean that extrinsically motivated actions are solely perceived as externally determined (Reeve, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Based on the perceived degree of heteronomous control or self-determination, Ryan and Deci (2017) describe four types of motivational regulation of extrinsically motivated actions: external, introjected, identified, and integrated.

Externally regulated actions are performed to achieve a positively rated state (e.g., a reward) or to avoid a negatively rated state (e.g., a punishment) (Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). The execution of such actions is experienced as being externally determined (Ryan and Deci, 2017; Thomas et al., 2018). Actions that are based on introjected regulation are described as being rather externally determined (Ryan and Deci, 2002). With the execution of introjected regulated actions, individuals tend to avoid guilt and shame (avoidance type; Guay et al., 2000; Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002) or to strengthen or maintain their self-esteem (approach type; Assor et al., 2009). One regulation that results in a rather self-determined quality of action is the identified regulation (Ryan and Deci, 2002, 2017; Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002). An individual performs an identified regulated action when the goal and the underlying values of this action are considered valuable by the individual (Ryan and Deci, 2017). The underlying goals of such self-determined actions can be separated from the beliefs of the individual (Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002). If the beliefs of the individual and the goals of the action are no longer separable, an action is subject to integrated regulation (Vallerand and Ratelle, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). The goals and needs of the self are in line with the goals of the action while performing an integrated regulated action (Ryan and Deci, 2002). These actions already share qualities with intrinsically regulated actions such as the voluntary execution and perceived self-determination (Ryan and Deci, 2002).

Motivation in Basic Psychological Needs Theory

The motivational regulation of an action is determined, among other things, by the degree of the perceived satisfaction and frustration of the three-universal basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness (Ryan and Deci, 2017; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). The need for autonomy describes an individuals’ striving to be the origin of his/her action and having a sense of choice in actions (Reeve, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Moreover, individuals perceive themselves as being autonomous if they can execute actions voluntarily and without external pressure (Reeve, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). The need for competence entails an individuals’ desire to feel effective and be able to express and improve his/her own skills in his/her interactions with the environment (Reeve, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). The need for relatedness describes an individuals’ wish to belong to a community and to interact with significant others (Reeve, 2002; Ryan and Deci, 2017). A satisfaction of the depicted needs most likely results in a self-determined motivational regulation whereas a frustration thereof fosters controlled types of motivational regulation and negatively affects self-determined regulation (Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). Furthermore, the satisfaction of the basic needs facilitates well-being (Ryan and Deci, 2017). Vitality is regarded as one indicator of well-being and is defined by the availability of energy and feelings of enthusiasm (Ryan and Frederick, 1997; Martela et al., 2016). A satisfaction of the basic psychological needs combined with low levels of needs frustration support the facilitation of vitality (Ryan et al., 2006; Ryan et al., 2010).

Self-Efficacy

Self-efficacy in academic contexts can be described as the belief in one’s abilities to organize and execute the action(s) required to reach a given educational goal (Bandura, 1997; Elias and McDonald, 2007) and is linked to motivation (Zimmermann, 2000) and academic achievement (Valentine, Dubois, and Cooper, 2004; Zajacova et al., 2004). Self-efficacy is related to the perception of competence. Since one’s own belief about mastering tasks affects the balance between one’s own ability and the requirements of the task it is a central prerequisite of perceiving competence. At the same time, events that resulted in a high or low perception of competence affect self-efficacy positively or negatively. Self-efficacy might therefore play an important role in coping with new and potentially challenging situations such as the remote learning phase.

Academic Achievement and Personality Characteristics

Academic achievement is determined by ability factors (e.g., cognitive abilities; Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997) as well as non-ability factors (e.g., personality characteristics; Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2006). For example, achieving academic goals requires the cognitive ability to understand the content, the ability to control distracting emotions as well as to work and learn in an appropriate manner. These, among other factors, must be properly fulfilled to accomplish academic achievement. In this context, personality characteristics need to be considered as important predictors for academic achievement because 1) certain personality traits affect behavior which, in turn, can have an influence on academic achievement (e.g., conscientiousness; Rothstein et al., 1994), 2) personality traits reflect behavior which a person will show rather than what a person is theoretically capable of (Goff and Ackerman, 1992; Furnham and Chamorro-Premuzic, 2004), and 3) in an university setting, personality traits show more predictive power than cognitive ability for academic achievement (Ackerman et al., 2001; Furnham et al., 2003; O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007). For example, conscientiousness has been consistently related positively to academic achievement prior to (O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007; Poropat, 2009) and during the COVID-19 pandemic (Corazzini et al., 2020). As a personality dimension of the big five, it determines self-regulation and impulse control (John et al., 2008), which proved to be important in utilizing emotions to achieve academic goals (Pekrun, 1992; Pekrun et al., 2002). Since academic achievement belongs to the most important influencing factors on educational and professional careers in modern society, students are confronted with both their actual academic performance and their expectations thereof.

The expectation of their academic performance triggers a variety of personal and task-related emotions as well as different motivational regulations, which, in turn, influence cognitive processes and performance (e.g., Ryan and Deci, 2017). Emotions that are directly linked to academic achievement are called academic emotions (e.g., anxiety and motivation to learn) (Pekrun et al., 2002). These modulate a student’s behavior by triggering positive or negative directed intentions. Taken together, personality traits predefine how emotions influence behavioral tendencies and in consequence academic achievement.

Bridging Academic Achievement and Circadian Preference

Another important dimension of a personality trait-like characteristic affecting academic achievement is the circadian preference. Evening-oriented students show significantly worse grades in elementary school (Arbabi et al., 2015), middle school (Kolomeichuk et al., 2016), high school (Randler and Frech, 2006), and university (although this correlation weakens depending on the degree of free time allocation; Tonetti et al., 2015). Reasons given for this relationship are early school schedules (Goldstein et al., 2007) and the resulting lack of sleep for evening-oriented students (Roberts et al., 2009). These conclusions are further underlined by the findings of Jovanovski and Bassili (2007) who reported evening-oriented students prefer watching lectures online instead of attending them. Moreover, no correlation of chronotype with course performance was found. Horzum et al. (2014) reported similar results: the disadvantages evening-oriented students face in classroom teaching disappear with the switch to online teaching, because the students could adapt the lecture time to their personal needs. Additionally, various personality traits which favor academic achievement could be linked to morning orientation (e.g., conscientiousness; Adan et al., 2012; O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007; Önder et al., 2014; Poropat, 2009), while those which negatively affect advantageous academic behavior can be associated with evening orientation (e.g., extraversion; Adan et al., 2012; Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2005; Furnham and Chamorro-Premuzic, 2004; Furnham et al., 2003; Goff and Ackerman, 1992). However, the negative relationship between extraversion and academic achievement has yet to be validated, since some research shows no correlation or even suggests a positive correlation (e.g., Rothstein et al., 1994). Furthermore, evening orientation relates to the use of external stimuli (caffeine; Fleig and Randler, 2009), smoking and soft drinks (Gariépy et al., 2019), excessive cell phone use (Randler et al., 2016a; Demirhan et al., 2016), and long screen times (Kauderer and Randler, 2013; Shimura et al., 2018; Gariépy et al., 2019). According to Ryan and Frederick (1997), these factors may affect vitality negatively. Overall, the relationship of circadian preference with self-regulation and academic achievement builds on a small but growing body of literature. Work in this domain suggests that evening orientation is associated with characteristics and behaviors that hinder academic achievement.

Remote Working, Chronotype, and Motivation

Previous research shows that the change in students’ sleep-wake cycle caused by working from home resulted in positive alterations of sleep parameters for many people in different countries (Cellini et al., 2020; Gao and Scullin, 2020; Leone et al., 2020; Sinha et al., 2020; Staller and Randler, 2020). These findings support a modern approach to work environments called “New Ways of Working” (NWW; Baane et al., 2011). This concept tries to create temporal and spatial flexibility for employees while focusing on innovation and productivity with simultaneously reduced costs for employers (Nijp et al., 2016). It is proposed to adjust work to private life (Gajendran and Harrison, 2007; Nijp et al., 2015) and the employees’ biological needs such as chronotype (Wittmann et al., 2006). The remote working situation that the students found themselves in during the COVID-19 restriction phase in Germany reflects the temporal and spatial flexibility NWW tries to create. Positive effects of this working approach are assumed to be e.g., employees’ improved motivation due to gaining autonomy (Pritchard and Payne, 2003) and increased efficiency (Demerouti et al., 2014). By contrast, the lack of collegial support and exchange, which is considered a negative aspect of NWW (Halford, 2005), also applies to the university students’ current situation. There is also evidence that NWW might lead to exhaustion at the end of the workday (Ten Brummelhuis et al., 2012). Thus, vitality might be undermined. In line with self-determination theory (Ryan and Deci, 2017), these characteristics of remote working may affect the relationship between academic self-regulation and academic achievement.

Taken together, our study aimed at providing insight into the relationship between different related personality and motivational variables that affect academic achievement. Some interactions between these variables have already been shown in previous studies. Our study takes a more holistic approach to the relationship between these variables. Moreover, as shown, these relationships may be influenced by the remote learning situation. However, knowing these relationships is significant for designing learning environments that enable students to learn successfully in times of remote learning.

Research Question

Our study aims to investigate the effects of personality variables on various variables related to successful learning in an unprecedented situation, lockdown, and digital teaching. The identification of such predictors of successful remote learning can help to support student learning optimally in digital learning environments. As to the unprecedented situation we opted to derive an exploratory research agenda for A) the personality traits and B) chronotype based on findings of literature and previous studies in face-to-face teaching that are specified hereafter in more detail.

A) In respect to the personality traits we derive:

• the big five personality variables have an impact on the satisfaction and frustration of the students’ basic needs (Deniz and Satici, 2017)

• the big five personality variables have an impact on the students’ motivational regulation (Müller et al., 2006; Komarraju et al., 2009).

• the big five personality variables have an impact on the students’ vitality (Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016).

• the big five personality variables have an impact on the students’ self-efficacy (Şahin and Çetin, 2017).

B) Regarding chronotype, it can be assumed that:

• chronotype has an impact on the satisfaction and frustration of the students’ basic needs (Tavernier et al., 2019)

• chronotype has an impact on the students’ motivational regulation (Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, 2020)

• chronotype has an impact on the students’ vitality (Randler and Schaal, 2010).

• chronotype has an impact on the students’ self-efficacy (Przepiórka et al., 2019).

Methods

Participants and Data Collection

We investigated biology-teaching students (N = 228; MAge = 23.36 years, SDAge = 4.24 years, range = 18–43 years; 75% female, n = 171) in their bachelor or master studies participating in an one-time online survey. The study took place during the first lockdown in Germany in June 2020. Participants were invited via email distribution lists. These students gave their permission to use their anonymous data for scientific purposes and were included in our evaluation. Their participation in the survey was voluntary. After filling out the questionnaire, all participating students could take part in a raffle to win gift cards/vouchers. All participants studied in an online environment and took very different courses (e.g., lecture series or seminars). Furthermore, they all had access to a learning platform (e.g., Lernraum or studIP). 48 subjects were not included in the calculations because they did not complete the questionnaire. The dropouts are similar to the sample in demographic data, gender (dropouts: 69% female/sample: 75% female), age (dropouts: Ø 23 years (youngest: 19 years/oldest: 34 years)/sample: Ø 23 years (youngest: 18 years/oldest: 43 years)) and origin. In conclusion, the aforementioned 228 students were included in the statistical analyses. Together with the questionnaires, participants’ time spent on other commitments per week was assessed. Participants spent on average 17.66 h (SD = 19.30 h) on other commitments beyond their study. Here, participants (N = 228) reported these commitments mainly in the categories (part-time) job (46.5%), nursing/caregiving activities (4.4%), family (7%) and household activities (11.4%). 16% of the investigated students lived alone at the time of the survey, while 83% lived in a shared apartment with roommates, their partner and/or children. 1% of the students did not specify their situation at home.

Big Five Personality

We followed the big five-dimensional concepts of personality (e.g., Costa and McCrae, 1995). To measure personality, we used a German translation of the short version of the big five inventory (Rammstedt and John 2007; Rammstedt et al., 2013). This scale was based on the BFI-44 (Benet-Martínez and John, 1998) and was shortened to a 10-item questionnaire with two items for each personality dimension (extraversion, agreeableness, openness, neuroticism, and conscientiousness). The items were rated on a seven-point rating scale (see 3.3). The BFI-10 always showed a clear five factor structure and correlations with peer-ratings showed good external validity (Rammstedt and John, 2007). Due to its brevity, the scale can be used when personality assessment is only one aspect of a study design and when time is short. We used a confirmatory factor analysis to test the model structure of the BFI. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was 0.057 (CI 0.028–0.083). The comparative fit index CFI was 0.954. This suggests a good fit of the scale.

Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (Reduced)

To assess circadian preference, we used the Adan and Almirall (1991) short Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (rMEQ). This scale is based on five different questions regarding wake and bedtime preferences, peak performance, morning affect and self-classification. The scale ranges from 4 to 25 (4–11: evening type; 12–17: neither type; 18–25: morning type). The rMEQ is a time efficient questionnaire that has received a lot of support for its convergent validity (Di Milia et al., 2013). For example, the reduced form correlates between 0.87 and 0.90 with the full scale containing 19 questions (Di Milia et al., 2013). The questionnaire scores have been validated against biologically measured variables, such as objectively assessed sleep-wake variables based on actigraphy (Thun et al., 2012). The German version of the rMEQ has been established and validated (Cronbach’s α = 0.72; Randler, 2013).

Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale

To assess the satisfaction and frustration of the students’ basic psychological needs during the online semester, Heissel et al. (2018) validated German scales were used. The dimensions for satisfaction and frustration of the respective needs are the following: need for autonomy (satisfaction: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.74; frustration: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.84), need for competence (satisfaction: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.85; frustration: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.83), and the need for social relatedness (satisfaction: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.74; frustration: four items, Cronbach’s α = 0.72). A five-point rating scale (“1 = not true at all” to “5 = absolutely true”) was applied.

Scales for Motivational Regulation in Learning

To assess the students’ motivational regulation during the online semester, the scales for motivational regulation in learning (Thomas et al., 2018), a translated and adapted version of the Academic Self-Regulation Questionnaire (Ryan and Connell, 1989), were used. The instrument contains four subscales: intrinsic motivational regulation (three items, Cronbach’s α = 0.88); identified motivational regulation (three Items, Cronbach’s α = 0.72); introjected motivational regulation (six items), and external motivational regulation (three items, Cronbach’s α = 0.72) (Thomas et al., 2018). In this study, the subscale introjected motivational regulation was assessed separately as approach type (three items, Cronbach’s α = 0.78) and avoidance type (three items, Cronbach’s α = 0.83). The items of all subscales were rated on a seven-point rating scale (“1 = not true at all” to “7 = absolutely true”).

Vitality

Students’ vitality during the online semester was assessed with a translated version of Ryan and Frederick (1997) Subjective Vitality Scale. Analysis of the factorial validity was carried out with a principal axes factor analysis (PFA; Moosbrugger and Kelava, 2012). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin criterium (KMO = 0.90) was found to be good (Hutcheson and Sofroniou, 1999) and showed that the sample was entitled for analysis. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant with a p < 0.001. PFA showed one factor (eigenvalue of 4.58) and 65.40% of explained variance. The items had satisfactory factor loadings with values of 0.57–0.90 (Stevens, 2002). The seven items were rated on a seven-point rating scale as well (see 3.3). The internal consistency of the items was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.91).

Self-efficacy

To examine students’ self-efficacy during the online semester, seven items by Jerusalem and Schwarzer (1986) were applied. The items were again rated on a seven-point rating scale (see 3.3). The internal consistency of the items was satisfactory (Cronbach’s α = 0.82).

Statistical Analyses

No specific assumptions were made in advance for the situation in which the study group found itself during the lockdown. We therefore analyzed the data in an exploratory manner based on related previous research (see Research Question) and looked for relevant models. To determine internal consistency as Cronbach’s α, we used IBM SPSS Statistics 26. Afterward, we ran a series of multiple regressions with all 14 dependent variables. Independent predictors were personality, chronotype and the demographics age and gender. Only the significant total models were inspected for further analyses. We set a p = 0.01 as a threshold to accept a model as significant. For the confirmatory factor analysis, AMOS 26 was used. Correlations between personality dimensions were tested and intercorrelations were below 0.28 showing below medium effects in all cases.

Results

We calculated the distribution of the datasets and found them neither to be substantially skewed nor was a distinct kurtosis visible in any variable (the values for both, skewness and kurtosis did not exceed/fall below ±1). Table 1 summarizes the means, standard deviations, and ranges of all investigated variables.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Means, standard deviations and ranges of the independent (personality, chronotype) and dependent variables (basic psychological needs, motivational regulation, vitality, self-efficacy).

The mean rMEQ score was 14.63 (SD = 4.04) and ranged from 5–23. Men had a lower score compared to women (men: 13.65, SD = 4.38; women: 14.95, SD = 3.88; F = 4.52, p = 0.035, η2 = 0.020). Age was unrelated to the rMEQ score (r = 0.027, p = 0.685). Table 2 gives an overview of the correlations between the big five dimensions/chronotype and all investigated dependent variables. Furthermore, we calculated the correlations of the big five personality dimensions and the rMEQ score which showed mostly no significant correlations. Extraversion (r = 0.076, p = 0.253), Neuroticism (r = 0.010, p = 0.882), Openness (r = -0.109, p = 0.101), Agreeableness (r = 0.103, p = 0.122) with the exception of conscientiousness (r = 0.283, p < 0.001) which correlated with morningness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Correlation matrix between the independent (personality, chronotype) and dependent variables (basic psychological needs, motivational regulation, vitality, self-efficacy).

Due to the many correlations, we ran a series of multiple linear simultaneous regressions with each of the motivation-related scales and subscales as dependent variables. Table 3 presents the results of the full models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Results of the full models (linear regression) with the dependent variable (left column) and gender, age, rMEQ score, and personality as predictor variables of the basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, motivational regulation, vitality, and self-efficacy. The corrected R-squared is only given for the models with a p < 0.01.

In the following section, only the significant models with a p < 0.01 for the full model were analyzed (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Results of the multiple regressions. Full models are presented in Table 3. Standardized coefficient beta for the predictor variables is given. Predictors were gender, age, personality and rMEQ score. Dependent variables were basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, motivational regulation, vitality, and self-efficacy.

A significant impact of gender on motivational aspects were found, with men reporting a higher degree of self-efficacy and women being more intrinsically motivated. Age showed a negative relationship with introjected avoidance motivational regulation. Extraversion related negatively to intrinsic motivational regulation. Neuroticism related negatively to self-efficacy, vitality, and need satisfaction competence while it related positively to introjected approach and avoidance motivational regulation, as well as need frustration competence and relatedness. Openness correlated positively with self-efficacy, intrinsic, identified, and introjected approach motivational regulation, as well as need satisfaction competence. Conscientiousness was related positively to self-efficacy, vitality, identified motivational regulation, need satisfaction competence, and negatively to introjected avoidance motivational regulation and need frustration competence. For the rMEQ, positive correlations were found with self-efficacy, vitality, and need satisfaction competence.

Discussion

In our sample of biology-teaching students, the mean rMEQ score did not differ significantly from other German study samples (Randler 2013; Randler et al., 2016b). This is an expected result because chronotype remained stable during the COVID-19 shutdown phase in Germany while only sleep-wake timing changed (Staller and Randler, 2020). Gender differences in line with previous studies were found, with men being more evening-oriented (e.g., Randler and Engelke, 2019). Age effects were absent, most likely due to the low age variation (see e.g., Randler et al., 2016b, for a larger sample with the rMEQ). The relationship between morningness and vitality in our sample might have a biological reason: Morningness was linked to the cortisol awakening response in previous studies (CAR; see e.g., Randler and Schaal 2010), which may take account of this correlation as it reflects the theoretical connection to the diurnal cycle. Overall, personality and chronotype had a significant impact on online learning during the COVID-19 pandemic in these biology-teaching student sample.

Effects of Gender on Self-Efficacy

Our results are in line with previous findings on gender differences regarding self-efficacy (e.g., Fallan and Opstad, 2016). However, in a meta-analysis, Huang (2013) showed that such gender effects vary depending on the investigated subject domain. Whereas female students seem to have a higher self-efficacy in language arts, male students express a higher degree of self-efficacy in mathematics, social sciences, and computers (Huang, 2013).

Effects of Gender on Intrinsic Motivational Regulation

Biology as a school subject is assumed to be a female domain (e.g., Budde, 2008). Thus, females exhibit both more interest (Dietze et al., 2005) and a higher intrinsic motivational regulation than males in the school subject biology (Renaud-Dubé et al., 2010; Großmann et al., 2019). As a scientific field, biology might show the same underlying gender-related effects as described by Huang (2013) as well. However, since the study sample only consisted of biology-teaching students, the interest and intrinsic motivational regulation of all participants might have been above average, which could argue against the former conclusion.

Effects of Age on Introjected Approach Motivational Regulation

Our results show that younger students reported a higher level of introjected approach motivational regulation than older students. However, the correlation is small. One possible explanation might be that younger students feel more obligated to prove their abilities to others than older students do. They might have a stronger desire to manage what others think about them. However, we did not find such age-related effects for the avoidance type of introjected motivational regulation. Acting to avoid negative feelings such as guilt and shame seems to be independent of students’ age. To test the reliability of the current findings more research is needed.

Effects of Extraversion on Intrinsic Motivational Regulation

In our sample, extraversion related negatively to intrinsic motivational regulation. This result is contrary to the findings of Komarraju et al. (2009) and Müller et al. (2006), who found a positive relationship between these variables. When it comes to the teacher profession, positive correlations between extraversion and intrinsic motivation should become particularly apparent, since extraversion predicts satisfaction and success in teacher training programs as well as in the teaching profession (Mayr, 2014). A possible explanation for our result might be that other people and external stimuli play a more important role to extroverts than to introverts. Specifically, extroverts’ decision-making and behavior may be significantly influenced by what others think of them, suggesting a more externally determined rather than self-determined motivational regulation. A situation in which extrinsic motivational factors are largely absent, such as the COVID-19 shutdown, might lead to a lower level of both intrinsic motivation for learning and self-efficacy regarding extroverts. A lack of social exchange with peers and lecturers may therefore have a stronger effect on extroverts’ motivation and might (at least partially) explain the contradiction to what Komarraju et al. (2009) and Müller et al. (2006) reported.

Effects of Neuroticism

The results of our sample replicated previous findings concerning the negative relationship between neuroticism and self-efficacy as well as vitality (Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016; Deniz and Satici, 2017). Neurotic people are less open toward new and unpredictable situations (Borkenau and Ostendorf, 2008). The unpredictable situation resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic and the unexpected move to online learning constitutes a major challenge for neurotic people. The significant correlations between neuroticism and the tested motivation-related variables (positive correlation with introjected approach and avoidance motivational regulation, need frustration competence and relatedness; negative correlation with need satisfaction competence) are in line with previous studies (Müller et al., 2006; Komarraju et al., 2009; Önder et al., 2014; Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016).

Effects of Openness

As was the case in the sample in Şahin and Çetin’s (2017) study, our sample also yielded a positive correlation between openness and self-efficacy. This is contrary to the results of Judge et al. (2007), who found no impact of openness on self-efficacy. Openness as a predictor of self-efficacy might be explained by one’s inherent openness to situations and experiences. More “open” students may face more challenging and difficult situations that allow them to perceive more self-efficacy than students with a more “reserved” character. This conjecture is backed up by the result of Corazzini et al. (2020) who found high levels of openness to new experiences correlating with better student scores during the COVID-19 pandemic. Moreover, openness correlated positively to intrinsic motivation, replicating the work of Komarraju et al. (2009) and Önder et al. (2014). Furthermore, it related to the other self-determined types of motivational regulation, namely identified and introjected approach. Self-determined motivational regulation indicates perceived competence. Therefore, the positive correlation of openness and need satisfaction competence fits into this line of reasoning. Moreover, openness and need satisfaction competence were shown to correlate positively in previous work as well (Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016). Regarding the remote learning phase during COVID-19 shutdown, we reason that openness to new experiences might be beneficial when new methods of learning are implemented, even though more research is needed to test the reliability of the current findings.

Effects of Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness showed a strong positive correlation with self-efficacy and vitality, thereby replicating previous findings (Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016; Deniz and Satici, 2017). This was a somewhat expected result, as conscientiousness is one of the most important influencing factors on learning and academic achievement (O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007; Poropat, 2009). Also, conscientiousness was found to be highly correlated to student scores during the COVID-19 pandemic (Corazzini et al., 2020). Our results are in line with Komarraju et al. (2009), Önder et al. (2014), and Müller et al. (2006), who found that conscientiousness is a positive predictor of intrinsic motivation. Komarraju and others (2009) also found that there is a positive correlation between identified as well as introjected motivational regulation and conscientiousness. Moreover, conscientiousness was a positive predictor of extrinsic motivation (measured as identified, introjected and external motivational regulation) in their study. In our study, we replicated the positive relationship between conscientiousness and identified motivational regulation, but our data showed a negative correlation between conscientiousness and introjected avoidance motivation. This diverging result might be explained by the fact that Komarraju et al. (2009) did not differentiate between approach and avoidance introjection. Furthermore, self-determined motivational regulation indicates perceived competence, which relates to conscientiousness (see Nishimura and Suzuki, 2016). This positive correlation between conscientiousness and need satisfaction competence was evident in our data as well. Since it correlates negatively with introjected avoidance regulation, the connection to the need frustration competence meets expectations.

Effects of Chronotype on NWW

Morningness was related to self-efficacy and need satisfaction competence, which, in turn, were shown to correlate with conscientiousness, thus supporting the findings of previous work (Komarraju et al., 2009). Furthermore, morningness has been shown to correlate with conscientiousness (Adan et al., 2012) which could be replicated in this sample. Eveningness relates positively to extraversion (Adan et al., 2012, which could not be replicated in this sample) as well as negatively to intrinsic motivational regulation (in our sample). Our data indicate that the NWW approach might be more suitable for morning types, though this research question should be examined in more detail. The negative effects of NWW discussed in the literature (e.g., missing collegial support and a structured working environment; see theoretical background) might affect evening types more because they are less intrinsically motivated. The absence of extrinsic motivational factors may therefore have a stronger effect on evening types’ motivation and on their work and learning success. By contrast, morning types, may benefit more from the opportunities which NWW present (temporal and spatial flexibility) because of the relationship between morningness and characteristics such as self-efficacy and conscientiousness.

Conclusion

In this study, we found correlations indicating that the changeover to a remote or distant learning setting during the COVID-19 shutdown phase in Germany affects student teachers’ motivational regulation depending on their chronotype and big five personality characteristics. These effects on motivation have implications for students’ learning success in these new and probably challenging learning environments. The morning-oriented students dealt with the digital semester better and were more vital during the restrictions than evening-oriented students. Morning orientation further correlated with the personality traits in a distinct pattern. It correlated positively to personality characteristics that strengthen the relationship to intrinsic motivational regulation such as self-efficacy and need satisfaction competence and negatively to characteristics that weaken this connection such as extraversion (Adan et al., 2012). This study could replicate some prior findings in the field of motivational research such as the correlation between conscientiousness and intrinsic motivational regulation. Furthermore, some new findings emerged: 1) Extraversion was a negative predictor of intrinsic motivational regulation. This finding is contrary to that of Komarraju et al. (2009). 2) Whereas the introjected approach motivational regulation seems to be dependent on the students’ age, this dependency was not found for the avoidance type of introjected motivational regulation. We nevertheless recommend more testing for reliability which would give a stronger basis for the conclusions.

Strengths and Limitations

In this study, we revealed opportunities and obstacles in terms of remote learning following the restriction measures in Germany. This situation will accompany university teaching and learning further on. Even when the pandemic is over, digital elements may remain present in university teaching as blended learning. Therefore, identifying important predictors of successful learning in digital learning environments might help instructors to redesign these in a beneficial way. We did not limit the data collection to a single theoretical perspective but rather examined many covariables to ensure the results we conclude from this study are not directionally biased. This allowed for a broad perspective at the current motivational characteristics in relation to well-being and personality traits. Nevertheless, the explanatory power of this study is limited due to its exploratory cross-sectional nature. The ongoing pandemic prevented appropriate pre-testing from being carried out. Furthermore, it was not possible to use measurement methods that would complement the self-reports as the data access is restricted by the data protection act and in addition other non-self-report measures could not be applied due to the lockdown situation. We researched a small and narrow sample that refers exclusively to biology-teaching students. With our results, we are able to offer an insight into the relationships of personality dimensions, chronotype, motivational regulation and vitality of biology-teaching students during the first lockdown, even though the results may be less transferable to other groups. In this respect, future studies should expand the sample under consideration. Although this study provided information regarding the life situation of the participants, the situation of the online studies as well as study circumstances should be focused in more detail in future studies as they offer valuable insight and influence the perception of the digital study itself. We discussed conceivable relationships of the variables under consideration with academic achievement which should be investigated in further projects, as we have not included a measure of academic achievement here. Although the measurements used in this study are widely applied in the literature and are validated, the validity of the vitality measurement is limited due to the German translation used here. Moreover, our findings offer a valuable steppingstone for further research such as longitudinal studies that focus on the long-term effects of the lockdown on students’ learning processes.

Implications for Further Research

Future cross-sectional and longitudinal studies might take the subject matter into account since it can be assumed that personality traits can have different effects on experience and behavior (see Mayr, 2014). The present study showed that it could be a worthwhile research desideratum to clarify the connection between NWW and chronotype as well as to identify possible moderators between the two variables. In such studies, students’ temporal and spatial flexibility that is offered in their university courses might be surveyed. This flexibility most likely has an impact on students’ perception of autonomy and, in turn, their motivation. Students’ use of learning strategies has not been assessed in this study. As the use of learning strategies could very well influence the time invested in a course and as such be directly connected to the perception of workload (Kember, 2004; Kember and Leung, 2006) this aspect could be interesting for future studies. Moreover, we believe that it is necessary to investigate whether students have developed more appropriate coping strategies than at the beginning of the COVID-19 crisis which might result in a more self-determined motivation. Such changes and relationships can be clarified by longitudinal or cohort designs.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the authors still use this dataset for further studies. The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to bmFkaW5lLmdyb3NzbWFubkB1bmktYmllbGVmZWxkLmRl.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the ethics comission of Universität Bielefeld, Antrag-Nr.: 2020-200; Az.: 1266. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

AE, NG, MW, and FM, CR conceptualized and designed the study, AE, NG, MW, and FM performed the data collection, CR, NS, and NG made the statistical calculations, CR, NG, and NS wrote the first draft of the manuscript, AE, NG, MW, FM, CR, and NS agreed on the final submission of the study, FM managed and overlooked the whole project.

Funding

This project is part of the “Qualitätsoffensive Lehrerbildung”, a joint initiative of the Federal Government and the Länder, which aims to improve the quality of teacher training. The programme is funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (funding code: 01JA1908). The authors are responsible for the content of this publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge support by Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen.

References

Ackerman, P. L., Bowen, K. R., Beier, M. E., and Kanfer, R. (2001). Determinants of Individual Differences and Gender Differences in Knowledge. J. Educ. Psychol. 93 (4), 797–825. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.93.4.79710.1037/0022-0663.93.4.797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ackerman, P. L., and Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, Personality, and Interests: Evidence for Overlapping Traits. Psychol. Bull. 121 (2), 219–245. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adan, A., and Almirall, H. (1991). Horne & Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire: A Reduced Scale. Personal. Individual Differences 12 (3), 241–253. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adan, A., Archer, S. N., Hidalgo, M. P., Di Milia, L., Natale, V., and Randler, C. (2012). Circadian Typology: A Comprehensive Review. Chronobiology Int. 29 (9), 1153–1175. doi:10.3109/07420528.2012.719971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbabi, T., Vollmer, C., Dörfler, T., and Randler, C. (2015). The Influence of Chronotype and Intelligence on Academic Achievement in Primary School Is Mediated by Conscientiousness, Midpoint of Sleep and Motivation. Chronobiology Int. 32 (3), 349–357. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.980508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assor, A., Vansteenkiste, M., and Kaplan, A. (2009). Identified versus Introjected Approach and Introjected Avoidance Motivations in School and in Sports: The Limited Benefits of Self-worth Strivings. J. Educ. Psychol. 101 (2), 482–497. doi:10.1037/a0014236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baane, R., Houtkamp, P., and Knotter, M. (2011). Het Nieuwe Werken Ontrafeld: Over Bricks, Bytes & Behavior (New Ways of Working Unraveled: About Bricks, Bytes and Behavior). Tijdschr. Voor HRM 1, 7–23.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman and Company.

Benet-Martínez, V., and John, O. P. (1998). Los Cinco Grandes across Cultures and Ethnic Groups: Multitrait-Multimethod Analyses of the Big Five in Spanish and English. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75 (3), 729–750. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borkenau, P., and Ostendorf, F. (2008). NEO-FFI: NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar nach Costa und McCrae. 2nd ed.. Göttingen, Germany: Manual, Hogrefe.

Budde, J. (2008). Jungen, Männlichkeit und Schule [Boys, masculinity, and school]. In Bildungsministerium für Bildung und Forschung Bildungs(miss)erfolge von Jungen und Berufswahlverhalten bei Jungen/männlichen Jugendlichen, (pp. 39–45). Bonn, Berlin, Germany.

Google Scholar

Cellini, N., Canale, N., Mioni, G., and Costa, S. (2020). Changes in Sleep Pattern, Sense of Time and Digital media Use during COVID‐19 Lockdown in Italy. J. Sleep Res. 29. doi:10.1111/jsr.13074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2005). Intellectual Competence. Psychol. 18 (6), 352–354.

Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2006). Intellectual Competence and the Intelligent Personality: A Third Way in Differential Psychology. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 10 (3), 251–267. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corazzini, L., D'Arrigo, S., Millemaci, E., and Navarra, P. (2020). The Influence of Personality Traits on University Performance: Evidence from Italian Freshmen Students. Venice, Italy: University Ca'Foscari of Venice, Dept. Of Economics Research Paper Series No, 19. doi:10.2139/ssrn.3764500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and Facets: Hierarchical Personality Assessment Using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. J. Pers Assess. 64, 21–50. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6401_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demerouti, E., Derks, D., Ten Brummelhuis, L. L., and Bakker, A. B. (2014). “New Ways of Working: Impact on Working Conditions, Work-Family Balance, and Well-Being,” in The Impact of ICT on Quality of Working Life. Editors C. Karunka, and P. Hoonakker (Berlin, Germany: Springer), 123–141. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-8854-0_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demirhan, E., Randler, C., and Horzum, M. B. (2016). Is Problematic mobile Phone Use Explained by Chronotype and Personality?. Chronobiology Int. 33 (7), 821–831. doi:10.3109/07420528.2016.1171232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deniz, M. E., and Satici, S. A. (2017). The Relationships between Big Five Personality Traits and Subjective Vitality. An Psicol-spain 33 (2), 218–224. doi:10.6018/analesps.33.2.261911

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G., Hasher, L., Djikic, M., Criger, B., and Peterson, J. B. (2007). Morning People Are Stable People: Circadian Rhythm and the Higher-Order Factors of the Big Five. Personal. Individual Differences 43, 267–276. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Milia, L., Adan, A., Natale, V., and Randler, C. (2013). Reviewing the Psychometric Properties of Contemporary Circadian Typology Measures. Chronobiology Int. 30 (10), 1261–1271. doi:10.3109/07420528.2013.817415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietze, J., Gehlhaar, K.-H., and Klepel, G. (2005). “Untersuchungen zum Entwicklungsstand von Biologieinteressen bei Schülerinnen und Schülern der Sekundarstufe II [Investigations on the state of development of student interest in biology at the secondary level],” in Lehr- und Lernforschung in der Biologiedidaktik. Editors R. Klee, A. Sandmann, and H. Vogt (Innsbruck, Austria: Studien Verlag), 133–145.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational Beliefs, Values, and Goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53 (1), 109–132. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallan, L., and Opstad, L. (2016). Student Self-Efficacy and Gender-Personality Interactions. Ijhe 5 (3), 32–44. doi:10.5430/ijhe.v5n3p32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleig, D., and Randler, C. (2009). Association between Chronotype and Diet in Adolescents Based on Food Logs. Eat. Behaviors 10 (2), 115–118. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and McDougall, F. (2003). Personality, Cognitive Ability, and Beliefs about Intelligence as Predictors of Academic Performance. Learn. Individual Differences 14 (1), 47–64. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2003.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2004). Personality and Intelligence as Predictors of Statistics Examination Grades. Personal. Individual Differences 37 (5), 943–955. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajendran, R. S., and Harrison, D. A. (2007). The Good, the Bad, and the Unknown about Telecommuting: Meta-Analysis of Psychological Mediators and Individual Consequences. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 1524–1541. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.6.1524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, C., and Scullin, M. K. (2020). Sleep Health Early in the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Outbreak in the United States: Integrating Longitudinal, Cross-Sectional, and Retrospective Recall Data. Sleep Med. 73, 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2020.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gariépy, G., Doré, I., Whitehead, R. D., and Elgar, F. J. (2019). More Than Just Sleeping in: A Late Timing of Sleep Is Associated with Health Problems and Unhealthy Behaviours in Adolescents. Sleep Med. 56, 66–72. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2018.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goff, M., and Ackerman, P. L. (1992). Personality-intelligence Relations: Assessment of Typical Intellectual Engagement. J. Educ. Psychol. 84 (4), 537–552. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.537

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, D., Hahn, C. S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U. J., and Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Time of Day, Intellectual Performance, and Behavioral Problems in Morning versus Evening Type Adolescents: Is There a Synchrony Effect?. Personal. Individual Differences 42 (3), 431–440. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Großmann, N., Hofferber, N., Wilde, M., and Basten, M. (2019). Students’ Flow-Experience and Quality of Motivation in Biology Lessons – Can the Gender gap Be Reduced by Teaching Behavior?. Manuscript submitted for publication in: Motivation and Emotion.

Google Scholar

Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., and Blanchard, C. (2000). On the Assessment of Situational Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS). Motiv. Emot. 24 (3), 175–213. doi:10.1023/A:1005614228250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halford, S. (2005). Hybrid Workspace: Re-spatialisations of Work, Organisation and Management. New Tech. Work Empl 20, 19–33. doi:10.1111/j.1468-005X.2005.00141.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heissel, A., Pietrek, A., Flunger, B., Fydrich, T., Rapp, M. A., Heinzel, S., et al. (2018). The Validation of the German Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale in the Context of Mental Health. Eur. J. Health Psychol. 25 (4), 119–132. doi:10.1027/2512-8442/a000017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, J. A., and Östberg, O. (1976). A Self-Assessment Questionnaire to Determine Morningness-Eveningness in Human Circadian Rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol 4, 97–110. doi:10.1111/j.1468-005X.2005.00141.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horzum, M. B., Önder, İ., and Beşoluk, Ş. (2014). Chronotype and Academic Achievement Among Online Learning Students. Learn. Individual Differences 30, 106–111. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. (2013). Gender Differences in Academic Self-Efficacy: A Meta-Analysis. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 28 (1), 1–35. doi:10.1007/s10212-011-0097-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutcheson, G., and Sofroniou, N. (1999). The Multivariate Social Scientist: Introductory Statistics Using Generalized Linear Models. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. doi:10.4135/9780857028075

CrossRef Full Text

Jerusalem, M., and Schwarzer, R. (1986). “Selbstwirksamkeit [Self-Efficacy],” in Skalen zur Befindlichkeit und Persönlichkeit (Forschungsbericht 5. Editor R. Schwarzer (Freie Universität, Institut für Psychologie), 15–28.

Google Scholar

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Soto, C. J. (2008). “Paradigm Shift to the Integrative Big Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Conceptual Issues,” in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Editors O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 114–158.

Google Scholar

Jovanovski, D., and Bassili, J. N. (2007). The Relationship between Morningness - Eveningness Preference and Online Learning. Biol. Rhythm Res. 38 (5), 355–365. doi:10.1080/09291010600950149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, T. A., Jackson, C. L., Shaw, J. C., Scott, B. A., and Rich, B. L. (2007). Self-efficacy and Work-Related Performance: The Integral Role of Individual Differences. J. Appl. Psychol. 92 (1), 107–127. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, R. (2020). Insufficient Sleep Among Adolescents: the Role of Bedtime Procrastination, Chronotype and Autonomous vs. Controlled Motivational Regulations. Curr. Psychol. 39 (3), 1031–1040. doi:10.1007/s12144-018-9825-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauderer, S., and Randler, C. (2013). Differences in Time Use Among Chronotypes in Adolescents. Biol. Rhythm Res. 44 (4), 601–608. doi:10.1080/09291016.2012.721687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerkhof, G. A., and Van Dongen, H. P. A. (1996). Morning-type and Evening-type Individuals Differ in the Phase Position of Their Endogenous Circadian Oscillator. Neurosci. Lett. 218 (3), 153–156. doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(96)13140-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirmizi, O. (2015). The Interplay Among Academic Self-Concept, Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation and Academic Achievement of Higher Education L2 Learners. Hig 5 (1), 32–40. doi:10.5961/jhes.2015.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolomeichuk, S. N., Randler, C., Shabalina, I., Fradkova, L., and Borisenkov, M. (2016). The Influence of Chronotype on the Academic Achievement of Children and Adolescents - Evidence from Russian Karelia. Biol. Rhythm Res. 47 (6), 873–883. doi:10.1080/09291016.2016.1207352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komarraju, M., Karau, S. J., and Schmeck, R. R. (2009). Role of the Big Five Personality Traits in Predicting College Students' Academic Motivation and Achievement. Learn. Individual Differences 19 (1), 47–52. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leone, M. J., Sigman, M., and Golombek, D. A. (2020). Effects of Lockdown on Human Sleep and Chronotype during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Curr. Biol. 30 (16), R930–R931. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martela, F., DeHaan, C. R., and Ryan, R. M. (2016). “On Enhancing and Diminishing Energy through Psychological Means,” in Self-regulation and Ego Control. Editors E. R. Hirt, J. J. Clarkson, and L. Jia (Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Academic Press), 67–85. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-801850-7.00004-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, J. (2014). “Der Persönlichkeitsansatz in der Forschung zum Lehrerberuf – Konzepte, Befunde und Folgerungen [The personality approach in research on the teaching profession – Concepts, findings and conclusions],” in Handbuch der Forschung zum Lehrerberuf. Editors E. Terhart, H. Bennewitz, and M. Rothland (Waxmann), Münster, Germany 189–215.

Google Scholar

Moosbrugger, H., and Kelava, A. (2012). Testtheorie und Fragebogenkonstruktion [Test theory and test instrument design]. 2nd ed. Berlin: Springer.

Müller, F. H., Palekčić, M., Beck, M., and Wanninger, S. (2006). Personality, Motives and Learning Environment as Predictors of Self-Determined Learning Motivation. Rev. Psychol. 12 (2), 75–86.

Google Scholar

Nijp, H. H., Beckers, D. G. J., van de Voorde, K., Geurts, S. A. E., and Kompier, M. A. J. (2016). Effects of New Ways of Working on Work Hours and Work Location, Health and Job-Related Outcomes. Chronobiology Int. 33 (6), 604–618. doi:10.3109/07420528.2016.1167731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijp, H. H., Beckers, D. G., Kompier, M. A., van den Bossche, S. N., and Geurts, S. A. (2015). Worktime Control Access, Need and Use in Relation to Work-home Interference, Fatigue, and Job Motivation. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 41 (4), 347–355. doi:10.5271/sjweh.3504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishimura, T., and Suzuki, T. (2016). Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration in Japan: Controlling for the Big Five Personality Traits. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 58 (4), 320–331. doi:10.1111/jpr.12131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, M. C., and Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big Five Personality Predictors of post-secondary Academic Performance. Personal. Individual Differences 43 (5), 971–990. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Önder, İ., Beşoluk, Ş., İskender, M., Masal, E., and Demirhan, E. (2014). Circadian Preferences, Sleep Quality and Sleep Patterns, Personality, Academic Motivation and Academic Achievement of university Students. Learn. Individual Differences 32, 184–192. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2014.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., and Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic Emotions in Students' Self-Regulated Learning and Achievement: A Program of Qualitative and Quantitative Research. Educ. Psychol. 37 (2), 91–105. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (1992). Kognition und Emotion in studienbezogenen Lern-und Leistungssituationen: Explorative Analysen [Cognition and emotion in study-related learning and performance situations: Explorative analyses]. Unterrichtswissenschaft 20 (4), 308–324.

Google Scholar

Poropat, A. E. (2009). A Meta-Analysis of the Five-Factor Model of Personality and Academic Performance. Psychol. Bull. 135 (2), 322–338. doi:10.1037/a0014996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pritchard, R. D., and Payne, S. C. (2003). “Performance Management Practices and Motivation,” in The New Workplace: A Guide to the Human Impact of Modern Working Practices. Editors D. Holman, T. D. Wall, C. W. Clegg, P. Sparrow, and A. Howard (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons), 219–244.

Google Scholar

Przepiórka, A., Błachnio, A., and Siu, N. Y.-F. (2019). The Relationships between Self-Efficacy, Self-Control, Chronotype, Procrastination and Sleep Problems in Young Adults. Chronobiology Int. 36 (8), 1025–1035. doi:10.1080/07420528.2019.1607370

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rammstedt, B., and John, O. P. (2007). Measuring Personality in One Minute or Less: A 10-item Short Version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J. Res. Personal. 41 (1), 203–212. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rammstedt, B., Kemper, C., Klein, M. C., Beierlein, C., and Kovaleva, A. (2013). Eine kurze Skala zur Messung der fünf Dimensionen der Persönlichkeit: big-five-inventory-10 (BFI-10). Methoden, Daten, Analysen (mda) 7 (2), 233–249. doi:10.12758/mda.2013.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., and Engelke, J. (2019). Gender Differences in Chronotype Diminish with Age: a Meta-Analysis Based on Morningness/chronotype Questionnaires. Chronobiology Int. 36 (7), 888–905. doi:10.1080/07420528.2019.1585867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., and Frech, D. (2006). Correlation between Morningness - Eveningness and Final School Leaving Exams. Biol. Rhythm Res. 37 (3), 233–239. doi:10.1080/09291010600645780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., Freyth-Weber, K., Rahafar, A., Florez Jurado, A., and Kriegs, J. O. (2016a). Morningness-eveningness in a Large Sample of German Adolescents and Adults. Heliyon 2 (11), e00200. doi:10.1016/j.heliyon.2016.e00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C. (2013). German Version of the Reduced Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (rMEQ). Biol. Rhythm Res. 44 (5), 730–736. doi:10.1080/09291016.2012.739930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., and Saliger, L. (2011). Relationship between Morningness-Eveningness and Temperament and Character Dimensions in Adolescents. Personal. Individual Differences 50, 148–152. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., and Schaal, S. (2010). Morningness-eveningness, Habitual Sleep-Wake Variables and Cortisol Level. Biol. Psychol. 85 (1), 14–18. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randler, C., Wolfgang, L., Matt, K., Demirhan, E., Horzum, M. B., and Beşoluk, Ş. (2016b). Smartphone Addiction Proneness in Relation to Sleep and Morningness-Eveningness in German Adolescents. J. Behav. Addict. 5 (3), 465–473. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J. (2002). “Self-determination Theory Applied to Educational Settings,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Editors E. L. Deci, and R. M. Ryan (Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press), 183–203.

Google Scholar

Renaud-Dubé, A., Taylor, G., Lekes, N., Koestner, R., and Guay, F. (2010). Adolescents' Motivation toward the Environment: Age-Related Trends and Correlates. Can. J. Behav. Sci./Revue canadienne des Sci. du comportement 42 (3), 194–199. doi:10.1037/a0018596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Abraham, C., and Bond, R. (2012). Psychological Correlates of university Students' Academic Performance: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138 (2), 353–387. doi:10.1037/a0026838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., and Duong, H. T. (2009). Sleepless in Adolescence: Prospective Data on Sleep Deprivation, Health and Functioning. J. Adolescence 32 (5), 1045–1057. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., and Merrow, M. (2003). Life between Clocks: Daily Temporal Patterns of Human Chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms 18 (1), 80–90. doi:10.1177/0748730402239679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothstein, M. G., Paunonen, S. V., Rush, J. C., and King, G. A. (1994). Personality and Cognitive Ability Predictors of Performance in Graduate Business School. J. Educ. Psychol. 86 (4), 516–530. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.4.516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Bernstein, J. H., and Brown, K. W. (2010). Weekends, Work, and Well-Being: Psychological Need Satisfactions and Day of the Week Effects on Mood, Vitality, and Physical Symptoms. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 29 (1), 95–122. doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.1.95

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived Locus of Causality and Internalization: Examining Reasons for Acting in Two Domains. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 57 (5), 749–761. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2002). “Overview of Self-Determination Theory. An Organismic Dialectical Perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Editors E. L. Deci, and R. M. Ryan (Rochester, NY: University Of Rochester Press), 3–33.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination Theory – Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Ryan, R. M., and Frederick, C. (1997). On Energy, Personality, and Health: Subjective Vitality as a Dynamic Reflection of Well-Being. J. Personal. 65 (3), 529–565. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., and Przybylski, A. (2006). The Motivational Pull of Video Games: A Self-Determination Theory Approach. Motiv. Emot. 30 (4), 344–360. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Şahin, F., and Çetin, F. (2017). The Mediating Role of General Self-Efficacy in the Relationship between the Big Five Personality Traits and Perceived Stress: A Weekly Assessment Study. Psychol. Stud. 62 (1), 35–46. doi:10.1007/s12646-016-0382-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimura, A., Hideo, S., Takaesu, Y., Nomura, R., Komada, Y., and Inoue, T. (2018). Comprehensive Assessment of the Impact of Life Habits on Sleep Disturbance, Chronotype, and Daytime Sleepiness Among High-School Students. Sleep Med. 44, 12–18. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2017.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinha, M., Pande, B., and Sinha, R. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 Lockdown on Sleep-Wake Schedule and Associated Lifestyle Related Behavior: A National Survey. J. Public Health Res. 9 (3). doi:10.4081/jphr.2020.1826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staller, N., and Randler, C. (2020). Changes in Sleep Schedule and Chronotype Due to CoVID-19 Restrictions and home Office. Somnologie 25, 131–137. doi:10.1007/s11818-020-00277-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. P. (2002). Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. 4th ed. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Tavernier, R., Hill, G. C., and Adrien, T. V. (2019). Be Well, Sleep Well: An Examination of Directionality between Basic Psychological Needs and Subjective Sleep Among Emerging Adults at university. Sleep health 5 (3), 288–297. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2019.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ten Brummelhuis, L. L., Bakker, A. B., Hetland, J., and Keulemans, L. (2012). Do new Ways of Working foster Work Engagement?. Psicothema 24, 113–120.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Thomas, A. E., Müller, F. H., and Bieg, S. (2018). Entwicklung und Validierung der Skalen zur motivationalen Regulation beim Lernen im Studium (SMR-LS). Diagnostica 64 (3), 145–155. doi:10.1026/0012-1924/a000201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thun, E., Bjorvatn, B., Osland, T., Martin Steen, V., Sivertsen, B., Johansen, T., et al. (2012). An Actigraphic Validation Study of Seven Morningness-Eveningness Inventories. Eur. Psychol. 17 (3), 222–230. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonetti, L., Fabbri, M., and Natale, V. (2009). Relationship between Circadian Typology and Big Five Personality Domains. Chronobiology Int. 26, 337–347. doi:10.1080/07420520902750995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonetti, L., Natale, V., and Randler, C. (2015). Association between Circadian Preference and Academic Achievement: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Chronobiology Int. 32 (6), 792–801. doi:10.3109/07420528.2015.1049271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, J. C., Dubois, D. L., and Cooper, H. (2004). The Relation between Self-Beliefs and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review. Educ. Psychol. 39, 111–133. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3902_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J., and Ratelle, C. F. (2002). “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation. A Hierarchical Model,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Editors E. L. Deci, and R. M. Ryan (Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press), 37–63.

Google Scholar

Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., and Soenens, B. (2020). Basic Psychological Need Theory: Advancements, Critical Themes, and Future Directions. Motiv. Emot. 44 (1), 1–31. doi:10.1007/s11031-019-09818-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittmann, M., Dinich, J., Merrow, M., and Roenneberg, T. (2006). Social Jetlag: Misalignment of Biological and Social Time. Chronobiology Int. 23 (1-2), 497–509. doi:10.1080/07420520500545979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajacova, A., Lynch, S. M., and Espenshade, T. J. (2005). Self-efficacy, Stress, and Academic success in College. Res. High Educ. 46, 677–706. doi:10.1007/s11162-004-4139-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: chronotype (morningness-eveningness), big five personality, motivation, self-efficacy, vitality, basic psychological needs (BPN), remote learning (distance)

Citation: Staller N, Großmann N, Eckes A, Wilde M, Müller FH and Randler C (2021) Academic Self-Regulation, Chronotype and Personality in University Students During the Remote Learning Phase due to COVID-19. Front. Educ. 6:681840. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2021.681840

Received: 17 March 2021; Accepted: 15 June 2021;
Published: 30 June 2021.

Edited by:

Daniela Conti, Sheffield Hallam University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Luca Corazzini, Ca’ Foscari University of Venice, Italy
Concetta De Pasquale, University of Catania, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Staller, Großmann, Eckes, Wilde, Müller and Randler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Naomi Staller, bmFvbWkuc3RhbGxlckB1bmktdHVlYmluZ2VuLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.