- 1NHC Key Laboratory of Mental Health (Peking University), Peking University Sixth Hospital, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders (Peking University Sixth Hospital), Peking University Institute of Mental Health, Beijing, China
- 2The Second People’s Hospital of Guizhou Province, Guiyang, China
- 3Beijing Changping Hospital of Integrated Chinese and Western Medicine, Beijing, China
- 4School of Biomedical Science and Medical Engineering, Beihang University, Beijing, China
- 5National Institute on Drug Dependence and Beijing Key Laboratory of Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China
- 6School of Public Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
- 7Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences and PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing, China
Background: Dreams can be affected by recent life events and long-term life experiences. Previous evidence has shown that childhood adverse experiences are associated with sleep quality and dream experiences.
Objective: The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between childhood adverse experiences and dream content in adults.
Participants and Setting: A total of 163 participants without current or past physical or mental disorders aged between 18 and 35 were screened in the hospital. Among them, 120 subjects who completed a dream content record at home and whose anxiety and depression levels and sleep quality were within the normal range were included in the data analysis.
Methods: A cross-sectional survey was conducted from June 2017 to December 2019. Dream content for 10 consecutive days was recorded by the participants and coded by the Hall and Van de Castle coding system. Childhood adversity was assessed by the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ). In the end, 719 dreams out of 626 nights for 120 participants (44 female) were included in the data analysis, gender differences between groups were analyzed using t-tests or U tests, and Spearman’s partial correlation and multiple linear regression were used to investigate the relationship between childhood trauma and dream content.
Results: Childhood adversity was associated with characters, friendly interactions, and objects in dream content. Regression models of childhood adversity predicting characters and objects in dream content were constructed. There were no gender differences in general demographic data, sleep quality, emotional state, childhood adversity, dream recall frequency, or dream content.
Conclusion: Childhood adversity is associated with adult dream content.
Introduction
A dream is a basic physiological phenomenon that often appears during sleep and includes, for example, thoughts, emotions, perceptions, and images (Pagel et al., 2001). Dreams can also be conceived as experience of consciousness or phenomenal states during sleep (Windt, 2015). In addition, dreams are considered proof of the speculation that consciousness is encapsulated within the brain (Revonsuo, 2006). Dreaming is the subjective process of experiencing these mental activities. Among normal adults, 93.5% have nightly dream memories and 6.5% have almost no dream memories. (Pagel, 2003) Additionally, adults reported approximately 1–2.8 dream memories per week in a dream questionnaire (Schredl, 2008; Nielsen, 2012) and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary was completed (Goodenough, 1991). Dream content may be influenced by sociodemographic factors and health status. Thematic diversity in dream content has been reported to decrease with age, and the prevalence of disturbing dreams in young and middle-aged people is higher than that in older adults (Worley et al., 2021). Patients with nightmare disorder, rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder have more negative emotional content in their dreams (Phelps et al., 2018; Gieselmann et al., 2019; Antelmi et al., 2021; Stefani and Hogl, 2021).
Daytime life and childhood experiences have an impact on dream content. Dreamers’ life experiences within the week before they dream may reappear in dreams (Picard-Deland and Nielsen, 2021). Childhood adversities refer to situations or experiences that create difficulty or unpleasantness for an individual before the age of 16 (Bernstein et al., 2003). According to the WHO World Mental Health Surveys, the prevalence of exposure to at least one childhood adversity (including interpersonal loss, parental maladjustment, maltreatment, physical illness, and economic adversity) is approximately 38.8% worldwide, irrespective of a country’s level of economic development. Of those who have reported any adverse childhood event, approximately 60% have been exposed to multiple adversities (Kessler et al., 2010; Morgan and Gayer-Anderson, 2016). Childhood adversity is a well-documented risk factor for the development of clinical or subclinical psychosis in adolescence and adults (Varese et al., 2012; Rossler et al., 2014). Childhood adversity has also been linked to ultrahigh-risk status and more severe disease processes and outcomes, such as an increased risk of substance abuse, earlier illness onset, and a higher risk of comorbidity (Arseneault et al., 2011; Agnew-Blais and Danese, 2016; Hunt et al., 2017; Gaweda et al., 2020). Severe emotional abuse (EA), emotional neglect (EN), physical neglect (PN), and sexual abuse (SA) have been associated with suicidal ideation and attempts (de Araujo and Lara, 2016).
Evidence has demonstrated that childhood adversities are associated with sleep problems in adolescence (April-Sanders et al., 2021). Children who suffer from interpersonal violence, such as parent to child violence, rape, or trauma, including accidents or injuries, show a higher risk of sleep disturbance and sleep disorders (Kajeepeta et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2016). Individuals with a history of emotional and physical abuse during childhood have poorer sleep quality in adulthood (Ramsawh et al., 2011). Childhood adversity may also influence dreams, and children who are separated from their mothers before 1 year of age have more nightmare experiences as adults (Csoka et al., 2011; Nielsen et al., 2019). There is also a correlation between attachment style and dream content that dreams may promote attachment in insecurely attached adults (McNamara, 1996). Compared with those classified as securely, avoidant, or dismissing attachment styles, individuals classified as preoccupied attachment style are more likely to have dreams and describe their dreams more loquaciously (McNamara et al., 2001). Anxious and avoidant attachment are associated with high levels of stress and conflict in dreams, and individuals with anxious and avoidant attachment have higher levels of anxiety and jealousy in dreams (Selterman and Drigotas, 2009). However, the relationship between childhood adversities and dream content in adulthood is not yet fully understood.
In this study, we hypothesized that adult dream content is influenced by childhood adversity. We sought to encode and quantify dream content using the text version of the Hall and Van de Castle (HVDC) coding system of dream content (Hall et al., 1966).
Materials and Methods
Participants
A total of 163 participants aged between 18 and 35 years without a history or family history of mental health disorders were screened between June 2017 and December 2019. For the screening evaluation, all participants were interviewed regarding their history and status of physical and mental disorders, assessed using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)-4 (Sheehan et al., 1998). The participants were asked to record their dreams in writing immediately after they awoke from a dream over the next 10 days. Signed informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiment, and ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the Peking University Sixth Hospital.
Measures
Demographic data (i.e., age, gender, ethnicity, residence, educational level, and dream recall frequency) were collected. Depressive and anxiety symptoms and sleep quality were assessed using the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS), Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS), and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), respectively. The severity of childhood adversities was assessed using the Chinese version of the Childhood Trauma questionnaire-28 item Short Form (CTQ-SF). Because our study focused on dream content in healthy adults, participants who presented with abnormal levels of anxiety or depression, or sleep quality problems were excluded. The SDS and SAS are self-rating scales, of which both have 20 questions scored from 1 to 4 (none or a little of the time, some of the time, a good part of the time, and most of the time, respectively). There are 10 questions each concerning increasing and decreasing depression levels in SDS, respectively (Zung, 1972). Participants with SDS scores less than 50 were included in the analysis. There were fifteen positively worded and five negatively worded questions in SAS (Zung, 1971). Participants with SAS scores that less than 45 were included in the analysis. The PSQI is a self-rated questionnaire that is widely used to assess sleep quality within 1 month. The PSQI includes 19 self-rated and five other-rated questions that can be further categorized into seven subscales (sleep duration, sleep latency, sleep disturbances, sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, and use of sleep medication). Participants who scored less than or equal to 5 on the PSQI were included in the analysis (Buysse et al., 1989). The CTQ-SF is a self-report questionnaire used to assess childhood adversity before the age of 16 years. It comprises 28 items generating a total trauma score and has five subscales: emotional abuse (EA), physical abuse (PA), sexual abuse (SA), physical neglect (PN), and emotional neglect (EN). Responses are recorded using a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never true), 2 (rarely true), 3 (sometimes true), 4 (often true), to 5 (very often true; Bernstein et al., 2003). We analyzed childhood trauma data as continuous variables. Finally, 43 of the 163 participants were excluded because they exceeded the cutoff score on the SAS, SDS, or PSQI, or because CTQ-SF data were missing.
Participants were asked to record their dreams in as much detail as possible immediately after waking up in the morning for 10 consecutive days. The introductory words on the dream record sheet are as follows: Please answer whether you were dreaming, and if there are dreams with content, please describe the content of your dreams in detail. We then counted the number of days involving dreaming, the total number of dreams recalled, and the total number of words in the dream content descriptions of the participants. Because some participants had more than one dream recalled on a particular day, we calculated the number of dreams recalled on the days when at least one dream was recalled. Ultimately, 719 dreams out of 626 nights for 120 participants (44 women) were included in the data analysis.
Dream content was analyzed using an HVDC coding system (Hall et al., 1966). This is the most commonly used empirical system for analyzing dream content because of its comprehensiveness. This scale has several nominal categories within dream reports: characters, social interactions, activities, success and failure, misfortune and good fortune, emotions, settings, objects, and descriptive elements (Domhoff and Schneider, 1998; Domhoff, 2003). Two judges, who were blinded to the groups and adequately trained in the use of the scale, scored these dreams. After the judges reached a consensus on the scores by consulting, they entered the scores into DreamSAT, which is a spreadsheet that facilitates the statistical analysis of dreams coded with the Hall/Van de Castle system of quantitative content analysis. More details of DreamSAT can be found on the website of Dream Bank http://www.dreamresearch.net/ (Schneider and Domhoff, 2022). Owing to the differences in the number of words and elements recorded by each person in the contents of dreams, and to reduce the bias caused by the differences, it is also necessary to explore the possibility of whether the proportion of each element in dreams change with other variables. We calculated the proportion of all categories based on the total number of categories and statistically analyzed the proportions as independent variables. To reflect the stability of the subjects over time, the data of all the dream contents recorded by each subject during these 10 days were analyzed as a whole, and not individually.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 24. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were used to test the normality of the distribution of the data, including demographic data, emotional state, sleep quality, dream memory, and dream content. Two-sample t-tests on data that conformed to the normal distribution were used to determine the differences between the sexes, and Mann–Whitney U tests were used to analyze the data that were no normally distributed. Data that conformed to a normal distribution are presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD), and data that did not conform to a normal distribution are presented as median (interquartile range). As we did not find gender differences in dream content, we combined data from all genders in subsequent analyses.
Correlations between age, years of education, anxiety level, depression level, sleep quality, and dream content were tested using Spearman’s correlations. The relationship between total childhood trauma scores and dream content was analyzed using Spearman’s partial correlations, with age, years of education, and SAS, SDS, and PSQI scores as covering variables to exclude the effects of current mood and sleep states on CTQ scores and dream content relationships. We then explored which specific negative experiences on the CTQ were associated with particular categories, and Spearman’s partial correlations were also based on age, years of education, and SAS, SDS, and PSQI scores as covariates.
We performed multiple linear regression tests to construct prediction models for the proportions of various elements in dream content. The dependent variables in the model are those elements of the dream contents that have positive results in Spearman’s correlations or Spearman’s partial correlations, and the independent variables were the demographic data or CTQ questionnaire scores on the side of the correlation with the dependent variables in the previous analysis.
Results
Demographic Information
The demographic characteristics, dream recalls, and dream contents of all participants are shown in Table 1. This study included 120 subjects, including 44 women, with a median age of 25 years and a median education level of 17 years. In this 10-day dream report, the median number of dreaming days for men and women was 5. The median numbers of dreams in men and women were 5 and 6, respectively. There were no significant differences across genders in the general demographic data, anxiety levels, depression levels, sleep quality, frequency of dream memories, or dream contents across categories.
Correlations Between Demographic Characteristics and Dream Content Assessed as Proportions
Correlations between demographic data, anxiety and depression levels, dream recall, and dream content were shown in Table 2. As age increased, the proportion of sexual interactions (r = 0.220, p = 0.016) and good fortune (r = 0.185, p = 0.043) in the dream content increased. Increased depression levels were associated with an increased proportion of dreams involving aggressive interactions (r = 0.264, p = 0.004). However, with higher SAS scores, there was a greater proportion of aggressive interactions (r = 0.210, p = 0.021) and a lower proportion of descriptive elements (r = −0.211, p = 0.021). Poor sleep quality was associated with a higher proportion of characters in dream content (r = 0.182, p = 0.047).
Table 2. Correlations between demographic characteristics, anxiety and depression levels, and sleep qualities and characters, aggression interactions, sex interactions, good fortune, and descriptive elements in the dream content.
Participants with higher total childhood trauma scores had a lower proportion of life-like characters (r = −0.186, p = 0.047) and a lower proportion of friendly interactions between characters (r = −0.212, p = 0.023) but an increase in the proportion of dreams with inanimate objects (r = 0.216, p = 0.020). Of all the subscales of the CTQ, only emotional neglect was associated with the above three categories. As the emotional neglect scores increased, there was a lower proportion of participants’ dreams with life-like characters (r = −0.251, p = 0.007) and friendly interactions between characters (r = −0.217, p = 0.020), but an increase in the proportion of dreams involving inanimate objects (r = 0.299, p = 0.001). The correlations between the total childhood trauma scores, emotional neglect scores, and dream content were shown in Table 3.
Table 3. Correlations between total childhood trauma and emotional neglect scores and characters, friendly interactions, and objects in the dream content.
Predictive Models of the Content of Dreams
We constructed a multilinear regression model for the dependent variables based on dream content that was related to two or more indicators, such as demographic data, anxiety and depression levels, sleep quality, and CTQ scores. First, we used PSQI, total childhood trauma, and emotional neglect scores as independent variables; the proportion of dreams including characters as the dependent variable; and the method chosen used a backward selection procedure. This model was significant (R2 = 0.069, F = 4.358, p = 0.015), with a significant contribution from the PSQI scores (B = 0.009, t = 2.296, p = 0.023), emotional neglect scores (B = −0.004, t = −2.086, p = 0.039), and the intercept of this model (B = 0.159, t = 9.722, p < 0.001; see Table 4).
We then built a model with the proportion of dreams involving objects as the dependent variable, which was also statistically significant (R2 = 0.073, F = 9.316, p = 0.003), in which the total childhood trauma scores contributed (B = 0.004, t = 3.052, p = 0.003), as did that the intercept (B = 0.109, t = 2.655, p = 0.009; see Table 5).
Discussion
This study illustrated that adverse childhood experiences, especially emotional neglect experiences, were related to changes in the proportion of characters, friendly interactions, and objects in adults’ dreams. The proportion of characters in the dream content was related to sleep quality, total childhood trauma scores, and emotional neglect scores. The interactions between the characters in dreams were related to the current emotional state or adversities experienced in childhood. With increasing age, sexual interactions and good fortune of the characters in dreams increased. Sexual interactions in dreams also increased with education. As the level of anxiety increased, the descriptive components of the dream reports decreased.
Although we found a correlation between sexual interaction and age and years of education, we were unable to establish an effective linear model. This result showed that the proportion of dreams containing the content of sexual interactions may increase with age and years of education, but the correlations between age, years of education, and sexual interactions in dreams may not constitute a causal relationship. Early studies found that dream content involving sexual interactions is associated with sexual fantasy, sexual daydreaming, and orgasmic experiences in real life (King et al., 2009). Inconsistent with our research, according to an extensive study, there is a negative correlation between age and sexual dreams. Although the age span (16–92 years old) was large, the study did not conduct subgroup analyses of different ages (Schredl et al., 2019). Most of the participants in our study were young college students or graduates, who sexual experiences gradually increased with age. According to the continuity assumption (Schredl and Hofmann, 2003), this may be one of the reasons why sexual contents in dreams gradually increase with age. Therefore, we assumed that the sexual content in dreams presents an inverted U-shaped change; that is, the sexual content in dreams gradually increases from adolescence but gradually decreases after middle age.
We found that as the severity of childhood adversity increased, the proportion of dreams involving characters and friendly interactions decreased, whereas the proportion of dreams involving objects increased. The correlations between these three dream content items and total childhood trauma scores were attributed to their correlations with emotional neglect. Although there has been no direct evidence prior to this study that childhood adversity affects the content of dreams in adults, the effects of childhood adversity on the onset of mental symptoms or mental illness in adults have long been demonstrated. Individuals who had experiences of being emotionally neglected or abused were more likely to have diagnoses of several Axis I and Axis II mental disorders and the onset of psychosis symptoms (Rossler et al., 2014; Mandelli et al., 2015; Taillieu et al., 2016; Nelson et al., 2017). The effects of childhood experiences on the mental state of adults may be based on irreversible changes in brain function after experiencing adversities, such as trauma. As children with self-reported childhood adversities have elevated emotional reactivity to negative stimuli (Heleniak et al., 2016), the amygdala’s response to negative stimuli is similarly elevated (McCrory et al., 2013). This elevated amygdala response may be attributed to amygdala volume changes after exposure to childhood adversities (Tottenham et al., 2010; Edmiston et al., 2011; Hanson et al., 2015). If these adversities continue to exist as chronic stress, the amygdala is hyperactivated (Rosenkranz et al., 2010). In addition to volume changes in the amygdala, increasing exposure to emotional neglect also leads to hyperactivity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and dysfunctional attitudes (Peng et al., 2014; Roth et al., 2018). Studies have shown that emotional experiences in a state of awakening are more likely to occur in dreams (Malinowski and Horton, 2014). This may reflect childhood emotional neglect playing an important role not only in psychological growth, but also in dream formation, and these experiences could influence dreams in adults.
Emotional neglect has been associated with difficulties in emotional processing and the development of secure attachment (Kessler et al., 1997). Earlier studies have shown that adversity in childhood, especially emotional neglect, is associated with the onset of anxiety disorder (SAD; Kessler et al., 1997). Greater emotional neglect predicts significantly greater severity of SAD, lower quality of life, and higher levels of self-reported disability (Bruce et al., 2012). This may explain why people with higher levels of emotional neglect have fewer characters and friendly interactions in their dreams. A history of emotional neglect has been positively associated with loneliness, and this loneliness could increase their emotional attachment to objects (Kyrios et al., 2018; Yap et al., 2020). People with higher levels of emotional neglect were less likely to stick with smaller rewards and more likely to make choices to avoid harm (De Carvalho et al., 2015). This result may support the social simulation theory of dreaming (SST; Revonsuo et al., 2016), which suggests that dream contents serve as simulations for real-life social events. This might be the reason our participants who experienced more emotional neglect had more objects in their dreams. Fewer characters, friendly interactions, and more objects in dreams reflect of the strategies adopted by people with higher levels of emotional neglect to avoid harm. Interestingly, fewer strangers and more familiar characters in dreams have been found following short social isolation (Tuominen et al., 2022).
Childhood trauma and emotional neglect can disrupt the relationships between individuals and objects. To reduce fear, disappointment, sadness, and abandonment, individuals shift their needs from human interactions to object interactions, such as eating more and satisfying material needs to meet their psychological needs. This could lead to reduced interactions with people in dreams and increased attention to objects. Similarly, as it has been illustrated that the impact of childhood adversities on psychological development extends until middle age in adults, the findings of this study show that the impact of childhood adversity on dream content can extend to adulthood as well.
Although this study provided important information on dream content associated with childhood adversities, especially emotional neglect, several limitations should be considered. First, the best study design to illustrate the relationship between childhood adversities and dream content in adults is a prospective study. Second, qualitative interviews are needed to identify the exact causality between childhood adversities and dream content. Third, the dream content written by the participants was diverse and subjective. Fourth, the sample size was relatively small, and the purpose of this study was to explore the effects of childhood trauma on dream content in healthy adults, without including a wider range of participants who had experienced childhood adversities; therefore, our study is not sufficient to represent all situations. In the future, research on the relationship between childhood experiences and dream content should be based on longitudinal cohort studies in which the effects of different adversity experiences dream content can be continuously observed. Therefore, we should also simultaneously explore the intermediary or regulatory factors between childhood adversity and dreams, and design appropriate interventions to reduce the negative effects of childhood adversity on adult dreams.
Conclusion
Childhood adversities may influence adult dream content, and this influence may consist of the association of emotional neglect with characters, friendly interactions, and objects in dream content.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Peking University Sixth Hospital Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
YM designed the study, analyzed the data, drafted the manuscript, conducted the literature review, and revised and finalized the manuscript. YM, XF, DW, XZ, and ZY carried out the subject recruitment, questionnaire screening, dream content collection, and data entry. XZ and ZY performed the electronic entry and coding of dream content. YB, RZ, and JD guided the statistical analysis of the data and reviewed and revised the manuscript. QS helped with the literature review and revised the manuscript. LL provided the platform and support for the research and reviewed and revised the manuscript. HS supervised the study design and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was supported in part by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (nos. 81771429, 82071490, and 81761128036).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank all of the participants for their willingness to participate in the study and the time that they devoted to the study. We thank Kangqing Zhao, Nana Xiong, and Qian Ying for their valuable comments in writing the paper.
References
Agnew-Blais, J., and Danese, A. (2016). Childhood maltreatment and unfavourable clinical outcomes in bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 342–349. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00544-1
Antelmi, E., Lippolis, M., Biscarini, F., Tinazzi, M., and Plazzi, G. (2021). REM sleep behavior disorder: mimics and variants. Sleep Med. Rev. 60:101515. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101515
April-Sanders, A., Duarte, C. S., Wang, S., McGlinchey, E., Alcantara, C., Bird, H., et al. (2021). Childhood adversity and sleep disturbances: longitudinal results in Puerto Rican children. Int. J. Behav. Med. 28, 107–115. doi: 10.1007/s12529-020-09873-w
Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Fisher, H. L., Polanczyk, G., Moffitt, T. E., and Caspi, A. (2011). Childhood trauma and children's emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 65–72. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10040567
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., et al. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0
Bruce, L. C., Heimberg, R. G., Blanco, C., Schneier, F. R., and Liebowitz, M. R. (2012). Childhood maltreatment and social anxiety disorder: implications for symptom severity and response to pharmacotherapy. Depress. Anxiety 29, 132–139. doi: 10.1002/da.20909
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F. 3rd, Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4
Csoka, S., Simor, P., Szabo, G., Kopp, M. S., and Bodizs, R. (2011). Early maternal separation, nightmares, and bad dreams: results from the Hungarostudy epidemiological panel. Attach Hum. Dev. 13, 125–140. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2011.553991
De Araujo, R. M. F., and Lara, D. R. (2016). More than words: The association of childhood emotional abuse and suicidal behavior. Eur. Psychiatry 37, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.04.002
de Carvalho, H. W., Pereira, R., Frozi, J., Bisol, L. W., Ottoni, G. L., and Lara, D. R. (2015). Childhood trauma is associated with maladaptive personality traits. Child Abuse Negl. 44, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.10.013
Domhoff, G. W. (2003). The Scientific Study of Dreams: Neural Networks, Cognitive Development, and Content Analysis. Washington, DC, United States: American Psychological Association.
Domhoff, G. W., and Schneider, A. (1998). New rationales and methods for quantitative dream research outside the laboratory. Sleep 21, 398–404. doi: 10.1093/sleep/21.4.398
Edmiston, E. E., Wang, F., Mazure, C. M., Guiney, J., Sinha, R., Mayes, L. C., et al. (2011). Corticostriatal-limbic gray matter morphology in adolescents with self-reported exposure to childhood maltreatment. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 165, 1069–1077. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.565
Gaweda, L., Pionke, R., Krezolek, M., Frydecka, D., Nelson, B., and Cechnicki, A. (2020). The interplay between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, psychotic-like experiences and depression and their additive impact on predicting lifetime suicidal behavior in young adults. Psychol. Med. 50, 116–124. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718004026
Gieselmann, A., Ait Aoudia, M., Carr, M., Germain, A., Gorzka, R., Holzinger, B., et al. (2019). Aetiology and treatment of nightmare disorder: state of the art and future perspectives. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12820. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12820
Goodenough, D. R. (1991). Dream Recall: History and Current Status of the Field. The Mind in Sleep: Psychology and Psychophysiology. 2nd Edn. Oxford, England: John Wiley and Sons.
Hall, C. S., Van Castle, D., and Robert, L. (1966). The content analysis of dreams. Am. J. Psychol. 80, 156–161. doi: 10.2307/1420563
Hanson, J. L., Nacewicz, B. M., Sutterer, M. J., Cayo, A. A., Schaefer, S. M., Rudolph, K. D., et al. (2015). Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 314–323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.020
Heleniak, C., Jenness, J. L., Stoep, A. V., McCauley, E., and McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Childhood maltreatment exposure and disruptions in emotion regulation: a transdiagnostic pathway to adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Cogn. Ther. Res. 40, 394–415. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9735-z
Hunt, T. K. A., Slack, K. S., and Berger, L. M. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and behavioral problems in middle childhood. Child Abuse Negl. 67, 391–402. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.005
Kajeepeta, S., Gelaye, B., Jackson, C. L., and Williams, M. A. (2015). Adverse childhood experiences are associated with adult sleep disorders: a systematic review. Sleep Med. 16, 320–330. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.12.013
Kessler, R. C., Davis, C. G., and Kendler, K. S. (1997). Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychol. Med. 27, 1101–1119. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005588
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO world mental health surveys. Br. J. Psychiatry 197, 378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499
King, D. B., DeCicco, T. L., and Humphreys, T. P. (2009). Investigating sexual dream imagery in relation to daytime sexual behaviours and fantasies among Canadian university students. Can. J. Hum. Sex. 18, 135–146.
Kyrios, M., Mogan, C., Moulding, R., Frost, R. O., Yap, K., and Fassnacht, D. B. (2018). The cognitive-behavioural model of hoarding disorder: evidence from clinical and non-clinical cohorts. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 25, 311–321. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2164
Malinowski, J., and Horton, C. L. (2014). Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams. Dreaming 24, 18–31. doi: 10.1037/a0036017
Mandelli, L., Petrelli, C., and Serretti, A. (2015). The role of specific early trauma in adult depression: a meta-analysis of published literature. Childhood trauma and adult depression. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 665–680. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.007
McCrory, E. J., De Brito, S. A., Kelly, P. A., Bird, G., Sebastian, C. L., Mechelli, A., et al. (2013). Amygdala activation in maltreated children during pre-attentive emotional processing. Br. J. Psychiatry 202, 269–276. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116624
McNamara, P. (1996). REM sleep: A social bonding mechanism. New Ideas Psychol. 14, 35–46. doi: 10.1016/0732-118x(95)00023-a
McNamara, P., Andresen, J., Clark, J., Zborowski, M., and Duffy, C. A. (2001). Impact of attachment styles on dream recall and dream content: a test of the attachment hypothesis of REM sleep. J. Sleep Res. 10, 117–127. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00244.x
Morgan, C., and Gayer-Anderson, C. (2016). Childhood adversities and psychosis: evidence, challenges, implications. World Psychiatry 15, 93–102. doi: 10.1002/wps.20330
Nelson, J., Klumparendt, A., Doebler, P., and Ehring, T. (2017). Childhood maltreatment and characteristics of adult depression: meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 210, 96–104. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.180752
Nielsen, T. (2012). Variations in dream recall frequency and dream theme diversity by age and sex. Front. Neurol. 3:106. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00106
Nielsen, T., Carr, M., Picard-Deland, C., Marquis, L. P., Saint-Onge, K., Blanchette-Carriere, C., et al. (2019). Early childhood adversity associations with nightmare severity and sleep spindles. Sleep Med. 56, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.03.004
Pagel, J. F., Blagrove, M., Levin, R., States, B., Stickgold, B., and White, S. (2001). Definitions of dream: a paradigm for comparing field descriptive specific studies of dream. Dreaming 11, 195–202. doi: 10.1023/A:1012240307661
Peng, H., Long, Y., Li, J., Guo, Y., Wu, H., Yang, Y., et al. (2014). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning and dysfunctional attitude in depressed patients with and without childhood neglect. BMC Psychiatry 14:45. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-14-45
Phelps, A. J., Kanaan, R. A. A., Worsnop, C., Redston, S., Ralph, N., and Forbes, D. (2018). An ambulatory polysomnography study of the post-traumatic nightmares of post-traumatic stress disorder. Sleep 41:zsx188. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx188
Picard-Deland, C., and Nielsen, T. (2021). Targeted memory reactivation has a sleep stage-specific delayed effect on dream content. J. Sleep Res. 31:e13391. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13391
Ramsawh, H. J., Ancoli-Israel, S., Sullivan, S. G., Hitchcock, C. A., and Stein, M. B. (2011). Neuroticism mediates the relationship between childhood adversity and adult sleep quality. Behav. Sleep Med. 9, 130–143. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2011.583895
Revonsuo, A. (2006). Inner Presence: Consciousness as a Biological Phenomenon. Cambridge, MA, United States: MIT Press.
Revonsuo, A., Tuominen, J., and Valli, K. (2016). Avatars in the Machine: Dreaming as a Simulation of Social Reality. Cambridge, MA, United States: MIT Press.
Rosenkranz, J. A., Venheim, E. R., and Padival, M. (2010). Chronic stress causes amygdala hyperexcitability in rodents. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1128–1136. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.008
Rossler, W., Hengartner, M. P., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Haker, H., and Angst, J. (2014). Impact of childhood adversity on the onset and course of subclinical psychosis symptoms–results from a 30-year prospective community study. Schizophr. Res. 153, 189–195. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.040
Roth, M. C., Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., and Gotlib, I. H. (2018). Self-reported neglect, amygdala volume, and symptoms of anxiety in adolescent boys. Child Abuse Negl. 80, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.016
Schneider, A., and Domhoff, G. W. (2022). The Quantitative Study of Dreams. Available at: http://dreamresearch.net/ (Accessed January 30, 2022).
Schredl, M. (2008). Dream recall frequency in a representative German sample. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 699–702. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.3.699-702
Schredl, M., Geissler, C., and Goritz, A. S. (2019). Factors influencing the frequency of erotic dreams: an online study. Psychol. Sex 10, 316–324. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2019.1638297
Schredl, M., and Hofmann, F. (2003). Continuity between waking activities and dream activities. Conscious. Cogn. 12, 298–308. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00072-7
Selterman, D., and Drigotas, S. (2009). Attachment styles and emotional content, stress, and conflict in dreams of romantic partners. Dreaming 19, 135–151. doi: 10.1037/a0017087
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59, 22–33.
Stefani, A., and Hogl, B. (2021). Nightmare disorder and isolated sleep paralysis. Neurotherapeutics 18, 100–106. doi: 10.1007/s13311-020-00966-8
Taillieu, T. L., Brownridge, D. A., Sareen, J., and Afifi, T. O. (2016). Childhood emotional maltreatment and mental disorders: results from a nationally representative adult sample from the United States. Child Abuse Negl. 59, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.07.005
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., et al. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev. Sci. 13, 46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x
Tuominen, J., Olkoniemi, H., Revonsuo, A., and Valli, K. (2022). “No man is an Island”: effects of social seclusion on social dream content and REM sleep. Br. J. Psychol. 113, 84–104. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12515
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., et al. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 661–671. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs050
Wang, Y., Raffeld, M. R., Slopen, N., Hale, L., and Dunn, E. C. (2016). Childhood adversity and insomnia in adolescence. Sleep Med. 21, 12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.01.011
Windt, J. M. (2015). Dreaming: A Conceptual Framework for Philosophy of Mind and Empirical Research. United States: MIT Press.
Worley, C. B., Bolstad, C. J., and Nadorff, M. R. (2021). Epidemiology of disturbing dreams in a diverse US sample. Sleep Med. 83, 5–12. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.04.026
Yap, K., Eppingstall, J., Brennan, C., Le, B., and Grisham, J. R. (2020). Emotional attachment to objects mediates the relationship between loneliness and hoarding symptoms. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 24:100487. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2019.100487
Zung, W. W. (1971). A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 12, 371–379. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0
Keywords: childhood adversities, emotional neglect, dream recall, dream content, Hall and Van de castle coding system
Citation: Ma Y, Feng X, Wang D, Zhao X, Yan Z, Bao Y, Zhu R, Sun Q, Deng J, Lu L and Sun H (2022) Adverse Childhood Experiences Are Associated With Adult Dream Content: A Cross-Sectional Survey. Front. Psychol. 13:837347. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.837347
Edited by:
Antonino Raffone, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyReviewed by:
Pınar Guzel Ozdemir, Yüzüncü Yıl University, TurkeyJarno Tuominen, University of Turku, Finland
Copyright © 2022 Ma, Feng, Wang, Zhao, Yan, Bao, Zhu, Sun, Deng, Lu and Sun. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jiahui Deng, jiahuideng2012@bjmu.edu.cn; Lin Lu, linlu@bjmu.edu.cn; Hongqiang Sun, sunhq@bjmu.edu.cn