Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 14 October 2020
Sec. Eating Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Eating Behavior and Food Decision Making in Children and Adolescents View all 16 articles

New Insights Into Causal Pathways Between the Pediatric Age-Related Physical Activity Decline and Loss of Control Eating: A Narrative Review and Proposed Conceptual Model

Tyler B. Mason&#x;Tyler B. Mason1Kathryn E. Smith&#x;Kathryn E. Smith2Britni R. BelcherBritni R. Belcher1Genevieve F. Dunton,Genevieve F. Dunton1,3Shan Luo,
Shan Luo3,4*
  • 1Department of Preventive Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Research consistently suggests that loss of control (LOC) eating in children and adolescents is a key factor contributing to pediatric obesity and eating disorders. However, causes of pediatric LOC eating are yet unclear, and there is a lack of longitudinal research investigating the developmental processes contributing to LOC eating and related outcomes in youth. Physical activity is an understudied behavior that declines during middle childhood to adolescence and may exert an influence in the development of LOC eating via its impact on executive functioning. While physical activity levels and executive functioning have been linked to regulation of eating, no research has examined the mechanistic processes by which these domains may together impact LOC eating during childhood and adolescence. In the current narrative review, a model is proposed that suggests how physical activity and executive functioning influence LOC eating and related outcomes during childhood and adolescence. This model has the potential to influence future theoretical models of pediatric LOC eating and guide future prevention and intervention efforts.

Introduction

As children transition from childhood to adolescence, they become increasingly responsible for their own eating behavior – including types of food eaten, how much food is eaten, and when they eat (Bassett et al., 2008). In the obesogenic food environments that are omnipresent in modern society, ability to self-regulate eating and control responses to food are critical for prevention of negative pediatric health outcomes including obesity, type 2 diabetes, and eating disorders. Loss of control (LOC) eating is a behavior that is characterized by a subjective sense of LOC over what or how much one is eating (irrespective of quantity consumed) and is associated with elevated caloric intake particularly from snacking and intake of energy-dense foods (e.g., fast food and sweets) as well as eating disorder pathology and mood and anxiety disorders (Goldschmidt et al., 2017).

LOC eating begins to emerge across middle childhood into adolescence, with recent data showing that up to 30% of children and adolescents with overweight or obesity report LOC eating with similar prevalence across sex (He et al., 2017). Importantly, children who report LOC eating are more likely to gain weight over time and develop full syndrome eating disorders and/or mood and anxiety disorders (Goldschmidt, 2017; Byrne et al., 2019). Specifically, previous data show that LOC eating predicts sub‐ or full-threshold binge-eating disorder diagnosis and greater global eating disorder psychopathology (Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2011; Hilbert et al., 2013). Despite the prognostic relevance of LOC eating for longer-term psychological and physical health, the etiology and maintenance of LOC eating in youth remains poorly understood, as predominant theoretical models of disordered eating (e.g., affect regulation and interpersonal models) have not held up consistently in children and adolescents (Hilbert et al., 2009; Ranzenhofer et al., 2014; Goldschmidt et al., 2018b). This is a crucial problem for prevention and intervention efforts, which is further evidenced by the limited efficacy of existing weight management and eating disorder interventions in children.

Physical Activity Decline in Middle Childhood and Adolescence

Alongside observed increases in LOC eating during middle childhood and adolescence, there is a well-documented age-related decline in physical activity levels as children enter middle childhood and puberty, such that only 24.8% of youth ages 12–15 meet physical activity guidelines of daily moderate-to-vigorous physical activity for at least 60 min (Fakhouri et al., 2014). This decline is not well-understood, but may be driven by biological factors (Belcher et al., 2013; Spruijt-Metz et al., 2013), environmental and psychosocial factors (Sallis et al., 2000), or decreases in participation in organized sports (Perez et al., 2017; Kemp et al., 2019). In addition to decreasing physical activity, sedentary behaviors increase during adolescence. Taken together, middle childhood through adolescence are critical years during which LOC eating develops and physical activity levels are simultaneously declining.

Physical Activity and Pediatric LOC Eating

Although physical activity and LOC eating share intriguingly similar developmental timeframes during which significant changes in these behaviors occur, they are almost entirely studied apart from one another. Consistently, recent reviews of the literature on LOC eating in youth did not discuss physical activity as relevant risk factor for LOC eating (Byrne et al., 2019; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2020). Nevertheless, mounting evidence suggests that higher overall physical activity may exert beneficial effects on eating behavior, and this is supported by studies in adults showing that higher levels of physical activity are related to better appetite regulation, reduced food cue responsivity, and less binge eating (Joseph et al., 2011; Luo et al., 2018). Similarly, among children and adolescents, higher accelerometer-assessed physical activity was negatively correlated with naturalistically-assessed LOC eating, overeating, stress‐ and emotion-related eating, and hunger (Smith et al., 2020a,b). Given such data, physical activity has been termed a “gateway behavior” that may facilitate improvements in related health behaviors, including eating. These findings are especially relevant for youth with overweight or obesity given that LOC eating and physical inactivity are more prevalent in this group compared to peers of lower weight (Harriger and Thompson, 2012; Prentice-Dunn and Prentice-Dunn, 2012; He et al., 2017). Thus, it is possible that higher physical activity levels could have beneficial effects on eating patterns that in turn mitigate poor long-term outcomes among children.

Importantly, the influence of physical activity on eating may occur both at the momentary level (e.g., minutes to hours) and over extended time periods (e.g., months to years). At the momentary level, in children and adults, acute bouts of activity have been shown to attenuate appetite and urges to consume palatable food and have been linked to decreases in energy intake in children and adults (Thayer et al., 1993; Maraki et al., 2005; Taylor and Oliver, 2009; Thivel and Chaput, 2014). Further, prior naturalistic research among adults with obesity found that dietary lapses and temptations were less likely to occur after exercising (Carels et al., 2004). Also, elevated momentary moderate-to-vigorous physical activity predicted less stress-related eating in adolescents with higher BMI-z and predicted less positive emotional eating in adolescents with lower BMI-z (Smith et al., 2020b). Conversely, physical inactivity may have detrimental short-term effects on eating regulation. While directionality cannot be inferred, an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study of high school adolescents found that consumption of sweet snacks was concurrently associated with sedentary activities such as watching television and using electronic media at the same prompt (Grenard et al., 2013).

In addition to these momentary associations, longitudinal research in adults has shown that adults participating in exercise interventions experience greater increases in healthy eating patterns (i.e., increased fruit and vegetable intake and decreased junk food consumption) relative to non-intervention conditions (Oaten and Cheng, 2006; Fleig et al., 2011). Among adults with overweight and obesity, higher lifestyle physical activity, measured at the end of a 12-month behavioral weight loss program, was also related to greater flexible dietary restraint, less disinhibited eating, and less perceived hunger at 12‐ and 36‐ month follow-up assessments (Carraça et al., 2013). In sum, there is evidence that physical activity may have both short‐ and long-term beneficial effects on eating. However, there remains a dearth of literature that has examined such relationships in children and adolescents, particularly with respect to key behaviors (i.e., LOC eating) that are linked to current and future physical and mental health problems.

Executive Functioning as a Mechanism Linking Activity and LOC Eating

Moreover, the mechanisms underlying associations between physical activity and eating regulation have yet to be elucidated. While several factors have been posited to contribute to these relationships, burgeoning evidence indicates acute and long-term physical activity behavior enhance executive functioning, and poor executive functioning increases risk for the development of LOC eating (Verburgh et al., 2014; Goldschmidt et al., 2015; Alvarez-Bueno et al., 2017; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2020). Executive functions refer to “top-down” cognitive processes that guide goal-directed behavior and allow for adaptations to changing circumstances, and which are rooted in circuitry within the prefrontal cortex (Diamond, 2013). These executive functions develop throughout adolescence and are critically important for adaptive self-regulatory processes, including eating and physical activity behaviors (Hofmann et al., 2012; Dohle et al., 2018). In particular, inhibitory control deficits (i.e., reduced ability to suppress or interrupt prepotent responses) can interfere with self-regulation processes, including the ability to modulate the types and amount of food consumed (Liang et al., 2014) and are a specific facet of executive functioning that may be related to LOC eating. In fact, a recent study found that inhibitory control deficits, assessed with the stop-signal task, were the only executive functioning measure associated with caloric consumption during a laboratory test meal – an objective measure of LOC eating (Kelly et al., 2020).

While studies of inhibitory control in children and adolescents have most commonly utilized self-report measures and behavioral tasks, cognitive neuroscience research has begun identifying brain pathways associated with inhibitory control deficits. Neuroimaging studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine inhibitory control reliably implicate frontostriatal circuitry, including areas of the lateral prefrontal cortex (Dias et al., 1997; Aron et al., 2004). In adolescents with obesity, disinhibited eating has been linked to reduced integrity of frontal lobe, specifically lower orbitofrontal cortex volume (Maayan et al., 2011). Another recent study found that after completion of a food-specific inhibitory control task, overweight adolescents showed reduced activation in frontal inhibitory regions, including the superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex, compared to adolescents of lower weight (Batterink et al., 2010). Thus, it appears that less activation in the prefrontal cortex, important for inhibition control when trying to inhibit response to palatable food, is associated with greater weight and as an extension more dysregulated eating behaviors, such as LOC eating.

In addition to inhibitory control predicting LOC eating, research suggests that physical activity improves inhibitory control in children and adolescents. Several studies of children or adolescents found that physical activity was associated with acute improvements in inhibitory control (Chang et al., 2014; Browne et al., 2016; Franco-Alvarenga et al., 2019). Further, adolescents who completed an 8-week exercise program had increased inhibitory control following the intervention compared to a control group (Ludyga et al., 2018). While cognitive neuroscience research is limited (Belcher et al., 2020), one study found that physical activity may enhance prefrontal cortex functioning in children (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2013). In addition, in a separate fMRI study, children with higher fitness level had more efficient brain networks associated with inhibitory control compared to children with lower fitness level (Voss et al., 2011).

Model of Physical Activity Decline, Executive Functioning, and Pediatric LOC Eating

Given current limitations, we introduce a hypothesized model (Figure 1) that posits that developmental changes in physical activity patterns and executive functioning together influence self-regulation from childhood into adolescence, such that declines in physical activity have negative short‐ and long-term effects on behavioral and neural markers of executive functioning, which in turn increases risk for subsequent LOC eating and poor health outcomes (e.g., obesity and eating disorder pathology) over time. While there is likely a bi-directional relationship between executive functioning and physical activity such that executive functioning also precipitates regular physical activity, there is a more substantial literature on the predictive association from physical activity to executive functioning in children (Hillman et al., 2011; Verburgh et al., 2014; de Greeff et al., 2018). In addition, studies using accelerometers to obtain objective measurements of physical activity have shown that children who engaged in more physical activity had better executive functioning (Syväoja et al., 2014; van der Niet et al., 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model of activity, executive functioning, loss of control eating, and long-term outcomes.

Physical activity has also been shown to alter neurobiological processes associated with inhibitory control. For example, children with overweight and obesity who underwent a 3-month aerobic exercise intervention, compared to a control group, evidenced increased recruitment of the bilateral prefrontal cortex during an inhibitory control (i.e., antisaccade) task that was completed at baseline and post-intervention (Davis et al., 2011). Further, physical activity has been shown to be particularly effective at improving executive functioning in children with obesity (Logan et al., 2020), whom are more at-risk for LOC eating (He et al., 2017).

The proposed model has not been extensively studied and stems from connecting the available literatures on physical activity and LOC eating in children and adults. Investigation of this model has several potential theoretical and clinical implications. Research has implicated a number of factors in relation to LOC eating in children; however, little is known about the developmental origins of LOC eating (Byrne et al., 2019). That is, much more is known about how children who exhibit LOC eating differ from children who do not exhibit LOC eating opposed to the mechanisms that explain the initial onset of LOC eating. Although, research has yet to study differences in physical activity between children with vs. without LOC eating. One of the risk factors associated with LOC eating in children is impairments in general and food-specific aspects of executive functioning (Allen et al., 2013; Goldschmidt et al., 2015; Goldschmidt et al., 2018a; Stojek et al., 2018; Van Malderen et al., 2018). This proposed model is the first to suggest that a decline in physical activity that occur in middle childhood may be a biobehavioral mechanism that explains the onset of LOC eating in childhood.

In addition to theoretical implications, the proposed biobehavioral model could have high clinical significance. There have been limited studies investigating treatments for LOC eating in children. Those that have been conducted have studied psychological therapies (e.g., cognitive-behavioral therapy) as a treatment for LOC eating and have reported these therapies to be successful (Byrne et al., 2019). Examples of possible clinical implications of our theoretical model might be using physical activity as a stand-alone intervention or as part of psychotherapy to treat LOC eating in children. However, empirical research will be needed to determine appropriate clinical recommendations – including types of activity, duration, and frequency that are needed to change executive functioning and behavior. Further, from a preventive standpoint, while we know that it is crucial for children to remain physically active throughout childhood and adolescence to reduce negative physical and psychological outcomes, testing of the hypothesized model can provide information about physical activity as a preventive measure for LOC eating.

This model also may inform the combination and sequencing of prevention and intervention components, particularly if strategies that promote physical activity exert a transfer effect on eating regulation via enhancing executive functions. Furthermore, consistent with precision medicine initiatives, analysis of momentary, real-time data will be crucially important to inform tailored treatments. New preventions or treatments could target certain types of children or traits (e.g., children high vs. low in inhibitory control) or target the specific moments at which a child is most prone to engage in LOC eating (e.g., states of physical inactivity and reduced inhibitory control). Further, it is critical for pediatricians to screen for children’s adherence to physical activity recommendations in early childhood and utilize behavior change techniques with children and parents to increase adherence.

It is important to acknowledge limitations and other considerations. The key limitation of this review and proposed conceptual model is that it is based on a small number of studies, and we draw on some studies from the adult literature given the comparatively sparse pediatric literature base. In addition, this is a proposed conceptual model that is intended to guide further research direction and has not yet been tested, and thus, empirical research will be needed in order to make clinical recommendations. The model described is intentionally parsimonious to guide initial research in this area, yet there is a plethora of other variables that should be considered in the context of this model moving forward. For example, emotion regulation is an important factor related to physical activity and LOC eating (Goldschmidt et al., 2017; Bernstein and McNally, 2018), and emotion regulation abilities are modulated by executive functioning (Calkins and Marcovitch, 2010; Sudikoff et al., 2015). Therefore, emotion regulation abilities likely play an important role in this model. Further, other trait and dispositional variables are key to examine as moderators and mediators within the context of this model including personality (e.g., health consciousness and impulsivity), familial factors (e.g., parenting practices), and environment (e.g., proximity to fast food outlets or parks).

Finally, while reviews of physical activity and executive functioning (Hillman et al., 2011; Verburgh et al., 2014; de Greeff et al., 2018) have all shown evidence for relationships between physical activity and executive functioning, there have been inconsistent findings regarding acute vs. chronic activity effects on executive functioning, depending upon measure used. Verburgh et al. (2014) concluded that acute physical activity (i.e., single bout of activity) predicted improved executive functioning using task-based measures but chronic physical activity (i.e., long-term exercise programs) did not; though, there were a limited number of chronic physical activity studies. Conversely, Hillman et al. (2011) reported that acute (i.e., single bout of activity) and chronic (i.e., fitness level) physical activity both predicted improved executive functioning using task and event-related potential measures. de Greeff et al. (2018) also found that acute (i.e., single bout of activity) and chronic (i.e., long-term exercise program) physical activity both predicted improved executive functioning using task measures, but results differed across tasks. These reviews demonstrate the importance for studying possible effects of both acute and chronic activity. Importantly, future studies testing this model should use objectively measured accelerometer physical activity, which measures children’s total volume of activity and can account for all activity that children perform.

In sum, establishing causal pathways and micro-temporal associations among physical activity, executive functioning, and LOC eating in youth has the potential to inform new prevention and intervention strategies for a host of pediatric outcomes. Future studies using multi-method designs, including psychological interviews, ambulatory assessment, and cognitive assessment, across middle childhood and adolescence will be needed to test the proposed model. Research on moderators and facets of executive functioning will also be needed to refine the model.

Author Contributions

TM and KS: conceptualized the idea and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. BB, GD, and SL: revised subsequent drafts and provided critical feedback. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work is in part supported by NIH K01DK115638 (P.I. SL), 3K01DK115638-03S1 (P.I. SL), and NIH K01DK124435 (P.I. TM).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Allen, K. L., Byrne, S. M., Hii, H., Van Eekelen, A., Mattes, E., and Foster, J. K. (2013). Neurocognitive functioning in adolescents with eating disorders: a population-based study. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 18, 355–375. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.698592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez-Bueno, C., Pesce, C., Cavero-Redondo, I., Sanchez-Lopez, M., Martínez-Hortelano, J. A., and Martinez-Vizcaino, V. (2017). The effect of physical activity interventions on children’s cognition and metacognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 729–738. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, R., Chapman, G. E., and Beagan, B. L. (2008). Autonomy and control: the co-construction of adolescent food choice. Appetite 50, 325–332. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batterink, L., Yokum, S., and Stice, E. (2010). Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 52, 1696–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belcher, B. R., Chou, C. P., Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., Hsu, Y. W., Byrd-Williams, C. E., McClain, A. D., et al. (2013). Leptin predicts a decline in moderate to vigorous physical activity in minority female children at risk for obesity. Pediatr. Obes. 8, 70–77. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00091.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belcher, B. R., Zink, J., Azad, A., Campbell, C. E., Chakravartti, S. P., and Herting, M. M. (2020). The roles of physical activity, exercise, and fitness in promoting resilience during adolescence: effects on mental well-being and brain development. Biol. Psychiatry: Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.005 (in press).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, E. E., and McNally, R. J. (2018). Exercise as a buffer against difficulties with emotion regulation: a pathway to emotional wellbeing. Behav. Res. Ther. 109, 29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, R. A. V., Costa, E. C., Sales, M. M., Fonteles, A. I., Moraes, J. F. V. N. D., and Barros, J. D. F. (2016). Acute effect of vigorous aerobic exercise on the inhibitory control in adolescents. Rev. Paul. Pediatr. 34, 154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.rppede.2016.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, M. E., LeMay-Russell, S., and Tanofsky-Kraff, M. (2019). Loss-of-control eating and obesity among children and adolescents. Curr. Obes. Rep. 8, 33–42. doi: 10.1007/s13679-019-0327-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calkins, S. D., and Marcovitch, S. (2010). “Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning” in Human brain development. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition. eds. S. D. Calkins and M. A. Bell (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 37–57.

Google Scholar

Carels, R. A., Douglass, O. M., Cacciapaglia, H. M., and O’Brien, W. H. (2004). An ecological momentary assessment of relapse crises in dieting. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 72, 341–348. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carraça, E. V., Silva, M. N., Coutinho, S. R., Vieira, P. N., Minderico, C. S., Sardinha, L. B., et al. (2013). The association between physical activity and eating self-regulation in overweight and obese women. Obes. Facts 6, 493–506. doi: 10.1159/000356449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K. I., Voss, M., Knecht, A., Pontifex, M. B., Castelli, D., et al. (2013). The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:72. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. K., Hung, C. L., Huang, C. J., Hatfield, B. D., and Hung, T. M. (2014). Effects of an aquatic exercise program on inhibitory control in children with ADHD: a preliminary study. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29, 217–223. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acu003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 30, 91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., and Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing. J. Neurosci. 17, 9285–9297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09285.1997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohle, S., Diel, K., and Hofmann, W. (2018). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite 124, 4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fakhouri, T. H., Hughes, J. P., Burt, V. L., Song, M., Fulton, J. E., and Ogden, C. L. (2014). Physical activity in US youth aged 12–15 years, 2012. NCHS Data Brief. Number 141. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Google Scholar

Fleig, L., Lippke, S., Pomp, S., and Schwarzer, R. (2011). Intervention effects of exercise self-regulation on physical exercise and eating fruits and vegetables: a longitudinal study in orthopedic and cardiac rehabilitation. Prev. Med. 53, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franco-Alvarenga, P. E., Brietzke, C., José Coelho-Júnior, H., Canestri, R., Nagata, E. Y., Asano, R. Y., et al. (2019). Physical education class can improve acute inhibitory control in elementary school students. Motriz: Revista de Educação Física 25:e101906. doi: 10.1590/s1980-6574201900020007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B. (2017). Are loss of control while eating and overeating valid constructs? A critical review of the literature. Obes. Rev. 18, 412–449. doi: 10.1111/obr.12491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., Dickstein, D. P., MacNamara, A. E., Phan, K. L., O’Brien, S., Le Grange, D., et al. (2018a). A pilot study of neural correlates of loss of control eating in children with overweight/obesity: probing intermittent access to food as a means of eliciting disinhibited eating. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 43, 846–855. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsy009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., Hipwell, A. E., Stepp, S. D., McTigue, K. M., and Keenan, K. (2015). Weight gain, executive functioning, and eating behaviors among girls. Pediatrics 136, e856–e863. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-0622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., Lavender, J. M., Hipwell, A. E., Stepp, S. D., and Keenan, K. (2017). Emotion regulation and loss of control eating in community-based adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 183–191. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0152-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., Smith, K. E., Crosby, R. D., Boyd, H. K., Dougherty, E., Engel, S. G., et al. (2018b). Ecological momentary assessment of maladaptive eating in children and adolescents with overweight or obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 549–557. doi: 10.1002/eat.22864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grenard, J. L., Stacy, A. W., Shiffman, S., Baraldi, A. N., MacKinnon, D. P., Lockhart, G., et al. (2013). Sweetened drink and snacking cues in adolescents. A study using ecological momentary assessment. Appetite 67, 61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harriger, J. A., and Thompson, J. K. (2012). Psychological consequences of obesity: weight bias and body image in overweight and obese youth. Int. Rev. Psychiatr. 24, 247–253. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2012.678817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, J., Cai, Z., and Fan, X. (2017). Prevalence of binge and loss of control eating among children and adolescents with overweight and obesity: an exploratory meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 50, 91–103. doi: 10.1002/eat.22661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilbert, A., Hartmann, A. S., Czaja, J., and Schoebi, D. (2013). Natural course of preadolescent loss of control eating. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 684–693. doi: 10.1037/a0033330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilbert, A., Rief, W., Tuschen-Caffier, B., de Zwaan, M., and Czaja, J. (2009). Loss of control eating and psychological maintenance in children: an ecological momentary assessment study. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Kamijo, K., and Scudder, M. (2011). A review of chronic and acute physical activity participation on neuroelectric measures of brain health and cognition during childhood. Prev. Med. 52, S21–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, R. J., Alonso-Alonso, M., Bond, D. S., Pascual-Leone, A., and Blackburn, G. L. (2011). The neurocognitive connection between physical activity and eating behaviour. Obes. Rev. 12, 800–812. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00893.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, N. R., Jaramillo, M., Ramirez, S., Altman, D. R., Rubin, S. G., Yang, S. B., et al. (2020). Executive functioning and disinhibited eating in children and adolescents. Pediatr. Obes. 15:e12614. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, B. J., Cliff, D. P., Chong, K. H., and Parrish, A. M. (2019). Longitudinal changes in domains of physical activity during childhood and adolescence: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 22, 695–701. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, J., Matheson, B. E., Kaye, W. H., and Boutelle, K. N. (2014). Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 38, 494–506. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, N. E., Raine, L. B., Drollette, E. S., Castelli, D. M., Khan, N. A., Kramer, A. F., et al. (2020). The differential relationship of an afterschool physical activity intervention on brain function and cognition in children with obesity and their normal weight peers. Pediatr. Obes. e12708. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Herrmann, C., Brand, S., and Pühse, U. (2018). Chronic effects of exercise implemented during school-break time on neurophysiological indices of inhibitory control in adolescents. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 10, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2017.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, S., O’Connor, S. G., Belcher, B. R., and Page, K. A. (2018). Effects of physical activity and sedentary behavior on brain response to high-calorie food cues in young adults. Obesity 26, 540–546. doi: 10.1002/oby.22107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maayan, L., Hoogendoorn, C., Sweat, V., and Convit, A. (2011). Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity 19, 1382–1387. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maraki, M., Tsofliou, F., Pitsiladis, Y. P., Malkova, D., Mutrie, N., and Higgins, S. (2005). Acute effects of a single exercise class on appetite, energy intake and mood. Is there a time of day effect? Appetite 45, 272–278. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., and Cheng, K. (2006). Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 717–733. doi: 10.1348/135910706X96481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, L. G., Conway, T. L., Arredondo, E. M., Elder, J. P., Kerr, J., McKenzie, T. L., et al. (2017). Where and when adolescents are physically active: neighborhood environment and psychosocial correlates and their interactions. Prev. Med. 105, 337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice-Dunn, H., and Prentice-Dunn, S. (2012). Physical activity, sedentary behavior, and childhood obesity: a review of cross-sectional studies. Psychol. Health Med. 17, 255–273. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2011.608806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ranzenhofer, L. M., Engel, S. G., Crosby, R. D., Anderson, M., Vannucci, A., Cohen, L. A., et al. (2014). Using ecological momentary assessment to examine interpersonal and affective predictors of loss of control eating in adolescent girls. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 748–757. doi: 10.1002/eat.22333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sallis, J. F., Prochaska, J. J., and Taylor, W. C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Med/Sci. Sports Exerc. 32, 963–975. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200005000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. E., Haedt-Matt, A., Mason, T. B., Wang, S., Yang, C., Unick, J. L., et al. (2020a). Associations between naturalistically-assessed physical activity patterns, affect, and eating in youth with overweight and obesity. J. Behav. Med. doi: 10.1007/s10865-020-00152-3 [Epub ahead of print]

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. E., O’Connor, S., Mason, T. B., Wang, S., Dzubur, E., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2020b). Differential associations between naturalistically-assessed physical activity and emotional eating among weight-discordant siblings. Pediatr. Obes. [Epub head of print]

Google Scholar

Spruijt-Metz, D., Belcher, B. R., Hsu, Y. W., McClain, A. D., Chou, C. P., Nguyen-Rodriguez, S., et al. (2013). Temporal relationship between insulin sensitivity and the pubertal decline in physical activity in peripubertal Hispanic and African American females. Diabetes Care 36, 3739–3745. doi: 10.2337/dc13-0083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stojek, M., Shank, L. M., Vannucci, A., Bongiorno, D. M., Nelson, E. E., Waters, A. J., et al. (2018). A systematic review of attentional biases in disorders involving binge eating. Appetite 123, 367–389. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudikoff, E. L., Bertolin, M., Lordo, D. N., and Kaufman, D. A. S. (2015). Relationships between executive function and emotional regulation in healthy children. J. Neurol. Psychol. 2.

Google Scholar

Syväoja, H. J., Tammelin, T. H., Ahonen, T., Kankaanpää, A., and Kantomaa, M. T. (2014). The associations of objectively measured physical activity and sedentary time with cognitive functions in school-aged children. PLoS One 9:e103559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Schvey, N. A., and Grilo, C. M. (2020). A developmental framework of binge-eating disorder based on pediatric loss of control eating. Am. Psychol. 75, 189–203. doi: 10.1037/amp0000592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Shomaker, L. B., Olsen, C., Roza, C. A., Wolkoff, L. E., Columbo, K. M., et al. (2011). A prospective study of pediatric loss of control eating and psychological outcomes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 108–118. doi: 10.1037/a0021406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. H., and Oliver, A. J. (2009). Acute effects of brisk walking on urges to eat chocolate, affect, and responses to a stressor and chocolate cue. An experimental study. Appetite 52, 155–160. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, R. E., Peters, D. P. III, Takahashi, P. J., and Birkhead-Flight, A. M. (1993). Mood and behavior (smoking and sugar snacking) following moderate exercise: a partial test of self-regulation theory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 14, 97–104. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90178-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thivel, D., and Chaput, J. P. (2014). Are post-exercise appetite sensations and energy intake coupled in children and adolescents? Sports Med. 44, 735–741. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0160-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Niet, A. G., Smith, J., Scherder, E. J., Oosterlaan, J., Hartman, E., and Visscher, C. (2015). Associations between daily physical activity and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. J. Sci. Med. Sport 18, 673–677. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Malderen, E., Goossens, L., Verbeken, S., and Kemps, E. (2018). Unravelling the association between inhibitory control and loss of control over eating among adolescents. Appetite 125, 401–409. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verburgh, L., Königs, M., Scherder, E. J., and Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 48, 973–979. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2011). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater efficiency of the network underlying cognitive control in preadolescent children. Neuroscience 199, 166–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: loss of control eating, physical activity, executive functioning, inhibitory control, pediatrics

Citation: Mason TB, Smith KE, Belcher BR, Dunton GF and Luo S (2020) New Insights Into Causal Pathways Between the Pediatric Age-Related Physical Activity Decline and Loss of Control Eating: A Narrative Review and Proposed Conceptual Model. Front. Psychol. 11:578690. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.578690

Received: 30 June 2020; Accepted: 14 September 2020;
Published: 14 October 2020.

Edited by:

Thomas Kubiak, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Germany

Reviewed by:

Laura Maria König, University of Bayreuth, Germany
Aleksandra Kaurin, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Copyright © 2020 Mason, Smith, Belcher, Dunton and Luo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shan Luo, shanluo@usc.edu

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.