Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 23 September 2020
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Macphail Revisited View all 20 articles

Neural Processes Underlying Tool Use in Humans, Macaques, and Corvids

  • Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

It was thought that tool use in animals is an adaptive specialization. Recent studies, however, have shown that some non-tool-users, such as rooks and jays, can use and manufacture tools in laboratory settings. Despite the abundant evidence of tool use in corvids, little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying tool use in this family of birds. This review summarizes the current knowledge on the neural processes underlying tool use in humans, macaques and corvids. We suggest a possible neural network for tool use in macaques and hope this might inspire research to discover a similar brain network in corvids. We hope to establish a framework to elucidate the neural mechanisms that supported the convergent evolution of tool use in birds and mammals.

Introduction

The classic definition of tool use in animals is “the use of an external object as a functional extension of mouth or beak, hand or claw, in the attainment of an immediate goal” (van Lawick-Goodall, 1970, p.195), following the observations of tool use in a wild population of chimpanzees (Goodall, 1964). Since then, the definition of tool use has evolved to encompass the physical properties of tools, specifying that a tool must alter “the form, position or condition of another object, another organism or the user itself” (Beck, 1980, p.10), and to include ways of manipulating and manufacture of tools (St Amant and Horton, 2008) in order to distinguish between those species that have the capability to create or use an external object to solve a problem via dynamic mechanical interactions, i.e., “flexible tool users,” and those that are “stereotyped tool users,” i.e., species that perform object-related mechanical actions that are not intended to have a goal-directed interaction with another object (Hunt et al., 2013). This is an important distinction because flexible tool use is not phylogenetically widespread and seems to require a certain level of cognitive processing (Baber, 2003). In this paper, our focus will primarily be on flexible tool use.

The definition of tool use specifies that the tool has to be detached from the substrate and directly held in the animal’s hand or mouth. This definition has opened a debate in the field of tool-use since many animals, including birds, cannot hold tools in their hand or mouth but instead use their beak or foot and others may throw or drop objects to achieve their goals (St Amant and Horton, 2008). Given these differences in the way tools are used, a distinction was made between “true tool-users,” i.e., those that follow the traditional definition of tool-use albeit broadening the criteria to include beaks and limbs [e.g., New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) using twigs as hooks to retrieve food from small holes (Hunt, 1996)], and borderline or “proto-tool” users, i.e., those that use objects to obtain food that would otherwise be out of reach but do not hold these objects in their limbs or mouths/beaks [e.g., American crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos) dropping nuts in roads to get them crashed by the passing of automobiles over them (Grobecker and Pietsch, 1978)]. From a cognitive perspective, it makes sense to make this distinction, since holding the tool in a part of the user’s body might make the user include that object as part of their own body, while those animals that just throw or drop objects might not have the ability to include the object in their body-image. As Jacob Bronowski graciously expressed, “the hand is the cutting edge of the mind” (Bronowski, 1975, p.116), and thus we should not forget their importance for body awareness.

Recently, Fragaszy and Mangalam (2018) developed a theory of tool use which the authors termed “tooling.” This theory is framed in biomechanical and spatial concepts of action in order to determine when an object is used as a tool. It aims to reconceptualize the phenomenon of tool use. The authors developed the concept of “tooling,” which we adopt as a legitimate description of what we consider tool use: “Tooling is deliberately producing a mechanical effect upon a target object/surface by first grasping an object, thus transforming the body into the body-plus-object system, and then using the body-plus-object system to manage (at least one) spatial relation(s) between a grasped object and a target object/surface, creating a mechanical interface between the two” (Fragaszy and Mangalam, 2018, p.194).

Before Goodall’s observations, it was widely believed that tool use was a uniquely human characteristic (Oakley, 1972), since the use and manufacture of tools has historically been linked to the emergence of technical intelligence in humans given the complex problem solving and planning needed to create and use composite tools (i.e., tools made of two or more joined parts) (Ambrose, 2001). Since then, many observations of both proto- and true-tool use have been reported, not only in primates but also in other mammals (Mann et al., 2008; Root-Bernstein et al., 2019), birds (Hunt, 1996), reptiles (Dinets et al., 2015), fish (Brown, 2012), and insects (Pierce, 1986). It is worth mentioning that flexible tool use is mostly found in birds and primates, while insects and fish mostly show stereotypical tool use (Hunt et al., 2013). Most significantly, Hunt made the remarkable discovery that New Caledonian crows manufacture and craft a variety of tools which they use to obtain food that cannot be reached in any other way (Hunt, 1996). Subsequent research by Hunt and other members of Gray and Taylor’s research groups have revealed many fascinating findings about the complexity of physical cognition in these birds (Hunt and Gray, 2004; Taylor et al., 2009, 2010). These observations provide evidence that evolutionarily distant species are capable of similar complex motor skills that require a certain level of cognitive ability to perform them.

Hodos (1987) suggested the study of animal tool use as one of the specific intellectual abilities that can be used as a proxy to understanding the concept of animal intelligence proposed by Macphail (1987). Hodos (1987) argued that we would understand animal intelligence more rapidly if we focus our efforts in the study of specific intellectual abilities rather than in the search of general intelligence. However, our understanding of the neural processes underlying tool use in non-human animals remains scant, even though descriptive reports and ecological literature related to animal tool use has grown dramatically.

Having a proper understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying tool use is pivotal to comprehend the evolutionary processes that enabled evolutionarily distant animals to achieve similar cognitive capabilities because the comparison of the brain structures that are needed for this specific intellectual ability will shed light on the evolutionary paths that give rise to animal intelligence. This review compiles information regarding brain areas active during tool use in humans and macaques, and will suggest possible areas in the bird brain that could be a focus of study in the future.

Tool Use in Humans

The neural basis of tool use in humans was first studied in patients with brain lesions that impaired their ability to use tools, a consequence of a disorder known as apraxia (Johnson-Frey, 2004; Lewis, 2006; Higuchi et al., 2007; Frey, 2008). Patients with apraxia do not show difficulties in linguistic, sensory or lower level motor functions. However, they do exhibit an impaired ability to carry out acquired skills, including, although not specific to, the use of tools. There are two types of apraxia that affect tool use: ideomotor apraxia and ideational, or conceptual, apraxia. In ideomotor apraxia, although patients know what to do with a tool and can grasp and manipulate it, they seem to be unable to represent the associated motor actions needed to properly use the tools, failing to pantomime how the tools are used. These patients suffer from damage to the left posterior parietal and/or premotor cortex, or damage to the corpus callosum that results in isolation of the left hemisphere from the right (Frey, 2008). On the other hand, in ideational or conceptual apraxia, patients know how to handle the tools, but can not follow the order of a sequence of movements to achieve a goal that is the product of a multistep action. Ideational apraxia patients commonly have lesions at the intersection of the temporal-parietal-occipital cortices of the left hemisphere (Frey, 2008). The studies of apraxia show, not only that the motor skills and conceptual knowledge about complex actions such as tool use are dissociable, but also that they are represented in dissociable neural systems within the left cerebral hemisphere (Johnson-Frey, 2004; Lewis, 2006; Frey, 2008).

Subsequently, fMRI and PET studies in healthy humans highlighted the areas of the cortex that are active during tool use or during mimicking and imaging tool use (reviewed in Lewis, 2006, see his Figure 5B). Lewis’ figures (2006) show that most activity during tool use is in the left hemisphere of the human cortex. This is the case for right-handed people, while the right hemisphere might have higher activation during tool use in left-handed people (reviewed in Lewis, 2006). Further studies comparing right- vs. left-handed people are needed in order to disentangle the lateralization of tool use in humans.

Peeters et al. (2009, 2013) identified the anterior supramarginal gyrus (aSMG), a specific region of the human brain left inferior parietal lobule (IPL), as being involved in both the execution and observation of tool actions. They did not find a similar activation in the IPL of rhesus monkeys that were trained to use tools. In a subsequent study, Gallivan et al. (2013) found specific brain areas involved only in tool-related actions, in contrast with brain regions involved only in hand-related actions, and suggested a brain network for human tool use (Figure 1). This brain network was later expanded to include the connection between the aSMG, which is active during the observation of the tool being moved to achieve a goal, and the putative human homolog of anterior intraparietal (phAIP) in macaques, a region active during observation of tool grasping (Figure 2; Orban and Caruana, 2014). Figure 2 showcases the cognitive processes involved in tool use, which are reasoning affordances (i.e., forming conclusions about the qualities of an object that defines its possible uses), mechanical problem solving (i.e., finding solutions to novel mechanical problems), and semantic knowledge (i.e., a type of long term memory consisting of concepts, facts, ideas, and beliefs). This suggested brain network also highlights the brain areas underpinning these cognitive processes, which would all provide input to the aSMG. For further reading, additional reviews on human tool use have been recently published (Osiurak and Badets, 2016; Reynaud et al., 2016).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cortical areas that coded only hand actions (red), only tool actions (blue), both hand and tool actions but using different neural representations (pink), and areas coding an action independently of it being performed with the hand or a tool (purple). Reproduced from Gallivan et al. (2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Representation of the human brain network suggested to be involved in tool use, including biological actions in blue, tool actions in red and blue, and cognitive processes in yellow boxes with green outlines. Dashed lines represent postulated connections. Reproduced from Orban and Caruana (2014).

Although the cerebellum is not mentioned in these brain networks, its function in tool-use has been under debate despite its role in sensory-motor control and learning of complex action sequences. For example, it is unclear why cerebellar lesions do not cause ideomotor apraxia if the cerebellum stores representations of tool-use skills (Johnson-Frey, 2004). Yet, its development during human evolution and its interactions with the neocortex have been related to greater computational efficiency for dealing with increasingly complex cultural and conceptual environments (Weaver, 2005). In great apes, cerebellar volume and lateral asymmetry of the cerebellum is related to species-specific differences in performance and hand preference for task that require precise motor skills, such as tool use and aimed throwing (Cantalupo and Hopkins, 2010). Additionally, the cerebellum plays an important role in the evolution of the capacity for planning, execution and understanding of complex behavioral sequences—including tool use and language (Barton, 2012; Barton and Venditti, 2014), and the neural mechanisms of tool use may be precursors for the neural basis of language and abstract thought (Hihara et al., 2003; Iriki and Taoka, 2012; Steele et al., 2012). These authors have suggested that the use of tools as equivalents of body parts would have triggered the development of more advanced problem solving skills: abstract thinking (i.e., the ability to think about things that are not physically present) was essential for the development of conceptual thinking (i.e., the ability of integrating a series of features that group together to form a class of ideas or objects) and hence, the development of language.

Despite the relevance of tool use to human evolution and the benefits that its study would bring to our understanding of brain evolution, the neural processes underlying tool use in other animals have not yet been closely studied, partly due to the methodological difficulty in reproducing the same sort of studies in animals. Given the hypothesis that tool use in humans would have led to the evolution of more complex cognitive abilities, comparisons of both the cognitive and neural mechanisms of tool use in humans and other vertebrates would increase our understanding of the evolution of physical cognition. An obvious starting point for such a comparison is to focus on non-human primates as there are many examples of tool-use in the wild, such as axing oysters (Gumert et al., 2009), nut-cracking, ant-dipping, and termite fishing, which have been previously reviewed (e.g., van Schaik et al., 1999; Whiten et al., 1999; Emery and Clayton, 2009), and in captivity, even in species that have not been observed using tools in their natural environment (van Lawick-Goodall, 1970; van Schaik et al., 1999). It is interesting to note that despite assumptions that non-human primates show flexible tool use, high cognitive abilities may not be necessary for the performance of tool use in many of the examples given (Penn and Povinelli, 2007). Instead, simpler forms of learning, such as affordance learning (i.e., learning about the use or purpose that an object can have, either directly or through social learning) may be responsible in some species for instrumental object manipulation (Whiten et al., 1999; Martin-Ordas et al., 2008; Gruber et al., 2011).

Keeping in consideration that the development of human high order cognition, including abstract thinking, might have been enhanced once humans used tools as equivalents of body parts (Iriki and Taoka, 2012), it is reasonable to assume that the comparison between humans and other vertebrates’ cognitive and neural mechanisms of tool use would increase our understanding of the evolution of physical cognition. Despite the abundant number of tool-use observations and cognitive studies, the study of the neural mechanisms governing tool-use in non-human primates is challenging. PET scanning studies, for example, require primates to remain completely immobile, except for the limb that uses the tool, during data collection to prevent confounding motion artifacts. Accurate measurements can only be achieved by confining the limbs not involved in the studied actions to small spaces and by limiting movement of the subject’s head using a custom-made chair. These constraints explain why there is so little non-human research on the matter, and also why most studies have very small sample sizes.

Tool Use in Macaques

Macaques are of particular importance because they have special neurons that become active both when they see another individual performing an action and when they do the action themselves (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). These neurons, located in premotor cortex F5, are of two types: canonical neurons and mirror neurons. Canonical neurons respond to the presentation of a graspable object or are active when the macaques grasp that object, while mirror neurons respond when the macaque sees object-directed actions (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Iriki, 2006; although see Hickok, 2010 for a critical review on mirror neuron function). Similar neurons were subsequently found in both humans and birds (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Welberg, 2008), although it remains unclear whether they exists in birds outside of the context of song learning. The ability to use tools and the presence of mirror neurons in their brains make macaques an interesting model for the study of tool use in vertebrates since mirror neurons are active during object-directed actions. Although there are neurocognitive studies exploring tool-use in a number of other species of non-human primates (e.g., Hopkins et al., 2012, 2017; Phillips and Thompson, 2013; Mayer et al., 2019) we have focused the following section on macaques because our objective is to suggest a possible neural network for tool use in a non-human primate species. For this purpose, using a single genus instead of a combination of findings from multiple species prevents us from generating a misleading network, since different species might differ in many ways, including anatomically, mechanistically, behaviorally, and cognitively.

Macaque Active Brain Areas During Tool Use

Obayashi et al. (2001, 2002, 2004, 2007) performed a series of studies on two awaken-behaving male Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). They explored the brain areas that are active during tool use by using PET scans during a task in which the subjects were previously trained to use tools to collect an unreachable food pellet. They used a control task in which the subjects experienced almost the same sensorimotor circumstances as in the experimental task, but without any learning involved (i.e., manipulation of the control apparatus did not result in the macaques learning how to reach the reward, but once the macaques had manipulated the control apparatus the experimenters moved the reward within their reach). In their 2001 study, the subjects had to reach the pellet with one rack. In their 2002 study, they had to poke a pellet with a rack out of a transparent tube and reach it with a second rack. In their 2004 and 2007 studies, the subjects had to obtain an unreachable pellet by manipulating a joystick or a pair of dials, respectively, which controlled the position of a shovel that moved in a two-dimensional space. They found the following active brain areas during tool use in macaques (Obayashi et al., 2001, 2002, 2004, 2007).

Prefrontal Cortex (PFC)

Specifically, area 9/46 seems to be involved in executive functions, since it is active during a sequence of tool combination tasks but not during single tasks (Obayashi et al., 2002). It is also active during abstract actions like remote operations using dials in a set of sequences (Obayashi et al., 2007). Together with the cerebellum, this area is involved in the automatization of learned motor sequences (Obayashi et al., 2004).

Intraparietal Sulcus (IPS)

This region is the area of the brain that creates, stores, and updates the body-image, i.e., the primate’s awareness of where its limbs are in space, and what actions they are performing (Obayashi et al., 2001, 2002, 2004, 2007). It has an important role in tool use because it provides the individual with an updated spatial representation of the situation, which is vital for the successful completion of the goal.

Inferior Temporal Cortex

Including the posterior portion of inferior temporal cortices (area TEO). This region is involved in object recognition and memory. Its extensive connections with IPS suggests that it might help this other area in maintaining and manipulating the body-image (Obayashi et al., 2002).

Premotor Cortex

There are two areas of interest within this region: F5 and dorsal premotor cortex (PMD). F5, the area containing mirror neurons in macaques, is involved in the execution of goal-directed manual actions (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). The PMD is involved in planning coordinated activation of muscles and joints to accurately perform desired movements (Obayashi et al., 2001, 2004). Thus, the combined activation of these two areas might be involved in the accurate execution of goal-directed actions performed with tools.

Pre-supplementary Motor Area (pre-SMA)

This area, which receives input from the premotor cortex (especially from F5), was suggested to be involved in the maintenance and updating of the body-image, which would be helpful for the execution of tool-based/use actions and sequential movements (Obayashi et al., 2001, 2004).

Basal Ganglia

It was suggested that the basal ganglia, as well as the IPS bimodal neurons, is involved in the creation and maintenance of the spatiotemporal representation of the hand during tool use (Obayashi et al., 2001).

Cerebellum

The cerebellum was suggested to be involved in the learning processes required for tool use and “reconstruction of the acquired body-image,” and “may modulate higher cognitive functions of the executive process as a cerebro-cerebellar loop from an anatomical perspective” (Obayashi et al., 2007).

These brain areas are important for tool use but that is not their only role. They can also be involved in less functional or less goal-directed forms of object manipulation, such as object exploration or object play, often claimed to be associated in the development, evolution and daily expression of tool use (Smith, 1982; Kerney et al., 2017).

Based on the information collected in the aforementioned studies and the available data on macaque brain connections (Schaal, 1999; Hihara et al., 2006; Van Essen and Dierker, 2007; Borra et al., 2008; Smaers et al., 2013; Takada et al., 2013; Rizzolatti et al., 2014), we suggest the following brain network for tool use in macaques, represented in Figure 3. The visual input about the tool and the task or problem that needs to be solved using that tool is processed in the visual cortex, which sends this information to the inferior temporal cortex and the IPS. The inferior temporal cortex would process the information related to object recognition, and would then send this information to the IPS. All of this information would be processed in the IPS and a spatial representation of the situation would be created. This information would be sent to area F5 and PMD. These two areas would coordinate the muscles to accurately perform the goal-directed action, and would send this information to the Pre-SMA, which is involved in the execution of sequential movements. The Pre-SMA would update the basal ganglia about the motor action and the basal ganglia would update the Pre-SMA and the PMD about the hand movements during tool use. The information would be sent to a PFC-basal ganglia-cerebellum network, involved in novel motor sequences learning and automatization of learned motor sequences. Finally, the cerebellum would ensure a coordinated motor action. It will be interesting to know whether the same brain regions are involved in these aspects of tool use in other animals or whether they are specific to the macaque brain. Additionally, it will be interesting to study the correlation between the specific patterns of brain activity and the motor movements involved, including in the correlation the levels of cognitive and behavioral control, such as distinctions between flexible and stereotypic tool use, and true versus proto tool use. An exciting first step would be to evaluate these issues in corvids, since like macaques and some other primates, these birds have relatively large brains for their body size, are highly social, have relatively long life spans and are known to use tools for extractive foraging and other problem solving tasks (Emery and Clayton, 2004a) including species that only do so in captivity (Clayton and Emery, 2015). Indeed Clayton and her colleagues have argued for the convergence of cognition in primates and corvids (e.g., Emery and Clayton, 2004b; Seed et al., 2009; Taylor and Clayton, 2012; Legg et al., 2017; Baciadonna et al., 2020).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Suggested macaque brain network for tool use. Suggested pathways in red. The names of cortical areas are in black, and subcortical areas in gray. The location of the areas is approximated. IPS, Intraparietal Sulcus; F5, Area F5 of the Premotor Cortex; PFC, Prefrontal Cortex; PMD, Dorsal Premotor Cortex; Pre-SMA, Pre-Supplementary Motor Area; TEO, Posterior Portion of the Inferior Temporal Cortex.

Tool Use in Corvids

The study of the neural processes of tool use in birds is of great interest, given that some species of birds have shown cognitive abilities and tool-use capability similar to those seen in primates (van Lawick-Goodall, 1970; Lefebvre et al., 2002; Emery and Clayton, 2004a, b, 2009; Clayton and Emery, 2015;, although see a comparison between chimpanzees and New Caledonian crows’ tool use in McGrew, 2013). By studying the neural processes of tool use in birds we could identify the brain areas involved in meticulous motor skills and complex cognitive abilities such as problem solving and future planning, which would help us understanding the complexity of the avian brain and its analogies with the mammalian brain (Grodzinski and Clayton, 2010). This is crucial to increasing our understanding of the convergent evolution of physical cognition among vertebrates.

Among birds, corvids are the family with the highest known number of true tool-user species (Lefebvre et al., 2002). For instance, New Caledonian crows manufacture hook tools out of plants to collect food in the wild (Hunt, 1996), with different populations within New Caledonia showing different strategies to manufacture these tools, which is hypothesized to have evolved through a process of cumulative change (Hunt and Gray, 2003). New Caledonian crows also select tools of the right length to achieve their goal (Chappell and Kacelnik, 2002), shape unfamiliar materials to create usable tools for specific tasks (Weir et al., 2002), manufacture tools of the best diameter to achieve a reward (Chappell and Kacelnik, 2004), can infer weights from how objects move under a current of wind (Jelbert et al., 2019), and are capable of spontaneous meta-tool use (using a tool to obtain a tool in order to achieve a goal) (Taylor et al., 2007). Recently, it was found that Hawaiian crows (Corvus hawaiiensis), which were considered extinct in the early 2000s and currently live in captivity, are capable of tool use and tool manufacture (Rutz et al., 2016).

Other species of corvids that are not known to be tool-users in the wild are capable of readily using and making tools in captivity. For example, rooks (Corvus frugilegus) were able to spontaneously create hook tools from pieces of straight wire and use them to collect food that would otherwise have been out of beak reach (Bird and Emery, 2009a). Furthermore they could use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating reward (Bird and Emery, 2009b), a task also performed by other non-tool-using corvids such as Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) (Cheke et al., 2011). Northern blue jays (Cyanocitta cristata) also made and used tools to obtain food out of reach (Jones and Kamil, 1973). Furthermore, ravens (Corvus corax) demonstrated problem-solving skills by string-pulling to collect food (Heinrich and Bugnyar, 2005), and are capable of planning for events using tools with delays of up to 17 h, including bartering tokens for food rewards (Kabadayi and Osvath, 2017). Many of these behaviors are under cognitive control, as tasks such as wire bending, meta-tool use, inferring the weight of objects, using objects to raise volumes of water, and bartering for tools, require some level of mental processing in order to conceive the problem, to understand the characteristics of the available objects, to plan a solution, and to successfully perform the task to solve the problem. Unfortunately, the neural mechanisms underlying these mental processes are still unknown.

It is for all these reasons that corvids make an excellent model for the study of the neural mechanisms of tool use (Clayton and Emery, 2015; Güntürkün, 2020). Furthermore, they belong to the order Passeriformes, in which the neural mechanisms of song learning in songbirds has been extensively studied, and so there is a wider amount of available information on neuroanatomy and neural networks for corvids compared to other families of tool-user birds (Jarvis et al., 2005). For example, some species of the order Psittaciformes (which includes parrots, lorikeets, and cockatoos) are capable of using and manufacturing tools (Auersperg et al., 2012, 2014; Lambert et al., 2015), and, like corvids, they are also social, big-brained, and vocal learners. However, psittaciformes are evolutionarily less closely related to songbirds, and so the study of their neural circuits has received less attention.

Lefebvre et al. (2002) found that the brain size of true tool-users was significantly larger than that of borderline tool-user species. However, the relatively larger brain of crows and ravens is not correlated to tool use, innovative feeding strategies, and dispersal success (Jønsson et al., 2012). Lefebvre et al. (2002) also found that, after examining the size of four different areas of the telencephalon, the neostriatum [later renamed as nidopallium (Jarvis et al., 2005)] was the only area significantly larger in true tool-using birds compared to borderline tool-users. However, this size difference does not mean that the nidopallium is the only area involved in tool use; rather, it means that the cognitive ability to use tools might depend on the relative size of the nidopallium.

Subsequent studies showed that the neuronal density of the brain of several bird species significantly exceeds the neural densities of many mammals, including primates, of similar brain mass (Olkowicz et al., 2016). This finding could explain why birds, which have relatively small brains compared to mammals, are capable of performing high cognitive tasks. When comparing studies of apes, corvids and pigeons, Güntürkün et al. (2017, p.39) suggest that a “neuronal surplus may translate into faster and more flexible learning, making the acquisition of certain abstract abilities a much easier task”. However, a fundamental difference between avian and mammalian brains is that birds lack pyramidal neurons. The inability to create long extensions (i.e., association fibers) that arise from pyramidal neurons means that to exhibit similar behavior to mammals, birds would need more neurons to make the same computations. Additionally, the small size of avian neurons also allows them to have higher neuronal density. An additional hypothesis that we suggest is that the nuclei-organized bird forebrain, unlike the cerebral cortex of primates which is organized in layers, might be computationally more powerful to encode tool-use skills than the isolated neurons in monkeys. To explain the differences in tool use between humans and monkeys, Orban and Caruana (2014) suggested that humans are capable of using tools because we have grouped neurons that respond to tool action observation, unlike some species of monkeys which were unable to learn to use tools. This same hypothesis can therefore be applied to birds, as we have done here. However, further studies are needed to figure out whether tool-use birds own specific neurons that respond to tool action observation.

In mammals, the prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role in problem solving (Mushiake et al., 2009). Given the many examples of problem-solving skills of corvids (e.g., Hunt, 1996; Chappell and Kacelnik, 2002, 2004; Weir et al., 2002; Clayton, 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Bird and Emery, 2009a, b; Tebbich et al., 2012), it is reasonable to assume that there must be a bird brain area capable of processing information to solve problems in a similar fashion as the mammalian prefrontal cortex does (Güntürkün, 2005; Güntürkün and Bugnyar, 2016). Identifying this possible area is crucial to understand how the cognitive abilities of corvids and apes have evolved via convergent evolution. A specific part of the nidopallium, called the caudolateral nidopallium (NCL) has been suggested as an analogous to the mammalian prefrontal cortex due to its high density of dopaminergic axons (Divac and Mogensen, 1985; Waldmann and Güntürkün, 1993; Güntürkün, 2005) and their function in reward processing (Koenen et al., 2013), prospective processing (reviewed in Colombo et al., 2017), reversal learning, response inhibition and working memory, obtained from studies in pigeons (reviewed in Striedter, 2013). The involvement of NCL in tool use is uncertain, as neurological studies in tool-using birds are rare. However, given the abundant connections of the NCL with other brain areas involved in tool use, it is thought that the NCL is “a critical integrative area for telencephalic sensorimotor pathways” (Striedter, 2013, p.63). Figure 1 in Striedter (2013) highlights the major avian brain areas and circuit diagrams that emphasize the role of NCL in tool use. Neurocognitive studies in crows have shown the involvement of the NCL in cognitive tasks that are important for tool use, such as visual working memory (Veit et al., 2014), associative learning (Veit et al., 2015), abstraction of general principles (Veit and Nieder, 2013), or relative numerosity discrimination (Ditz and Nieder, 2015, 2016). The NCL also shows properties such as flexible neuronal tuning depending on behaviorally relevant tasks (Veit et al., 2017) which is crucial to encode task relevant information.

New Caledonian crows, which are known for their exceptional ability to build and use tools, have an enlarged mesopallium, pallidostriatal complex, septum and tegmentum, compared to three other passeriformes (carrion crows, jays and sparrows) (Mehlhorn et al., 2010). Mehlhorn et al. (2010) suggest that the mesopallium might be required for enhancement of basic tool skills, while the nidopallium, which was also enlarged in this species although not significantly, might have a role in cognitive and motor skills required for basic tool use. They also suggest that the pallidostriatal complex might be important in these birds to learn to manufacture and use tools in novel and familiar situations, and the tegmentum would be involved in the fine motor skills needed for tool manufacture and use, while the septum would integrate several stimuli in order to modulate complex behaviors, which might not be directly involved with tool use. In summary, the study by Mehlhorn et al. (2010) corroborates the findings of previous studies, such as Timmermans et al. (2000), in that the size of the mesopallium is correlated to the feeding innovation rate in birds. However, it is difficult to determine what roles the septum and tegmentum could play in tool use without further investigations, particularly for the tegmentum, which is a large, multifunctional region.

Another region that seems to be important for tool use is the cerebellum. The cerebellum seems to be active during tool use in macaques and humans, as described previously. In birds, it was found that, although the total size of the cerebellum was not significantly different between tool-user and non-tool-user species, the number of folds in the cerebellar cortex was significantly larger in the former (Iwaniuk et al., 2009). It is possible that the increase in the number of folds might have been a way to supply the increased motor, sensory, and cognitive processing demands of the cerebellum of tool-user birds. Furthermore, a recent study in parrots describes a telencephalic-midbrain-cerebellar circuit that resembles the one in primates (Gutiérrez-Ibáñez et al., 2018), which is associated with the evolution of complex cognitive abilities, as described in previous sections. Particularly, the medial spiriform nucleus (SpM), which connects the pallial regions of the telencephalon with the cerebellum in birds, is greatly enlarged in parrots compared with other birds, suggesting that a stronger link between the pallium and cerebellum is associated with cognition (Gutiérrez-Ibáñez et al., 2018). Further studies in birds are needed to clarify the specific role of the cerebellum and the SpM during tool use.

The current literature differs in whether tool-using birds learn to use tools from conspecifics or not. A study on woodpecker finches (Cactospiza pallida) found that these birds probably learn to use tools by trial and error during their development (Tebbich et al., 2001). However, a study of Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana) found social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in the males of this species (Auersperg et al., 2014), and a study of New Caledonian crows found evidence for probable transmission of tool design between crows (Hunt and Gray, 2003). Thus, although true imitation of tool use has not been observed in birds and, hence, it is not possible to claim that tool-user birds must have mirror neurons, it is important to keep in mind that some of the tool-user species have developed the cognitive abilities to learn specific motor actions by observing others. This feature may imply the development of specific neural characteristics in tool-user birds, which might not be present in species that do not use tools and do not learn from others. In order to answer whether birds have developed neural mechanisms that are similar to humans and non-human primates when learning to use tools, it would be of great interest to study the activity in neurons of the above mentioned brain areas during observations of tool use. However, the size of these brain areas is so large that more research needs to be done before specific nuclei within these areas can be selected for study. The study of the neural processes of tool use in birds is therefore of pivotal interest in order to answer this and other questions, such as what mechanisms are behind the neural development that allows the generation of the same kind of complex behavioral patterns in unrelated species, so we can establish a framework to elucidate the neural mechanisms that supported the convergent evolution of tool use in birds and mammals.

Conclusion

The study of the neural processes underlying tool use in humans and non-human primates has received increased attention over the last decade. Early studies in patients suffering from apraxia showed that complex actions that require motor skills and conceptual knowledge, such as tool use, are represented in dissociable neural systems within the left cerebral hemisphere. Two interconnected regions are particularly important, the anterior supramarginal gyrus and the putative human homolog of the anterior intraparietal cortex. However, it is clear that these two regions do not work alone, and a complex neural network is required to use tools. Similarly, in macaques there are also two areas of particular interest: area F5 and the dorsal premotor cortex, both located within the premotor cortex. However, although these two regions seem to be of utmost importance during tool use in macaques, it is evident that, as in humans, tool use requires the activation of a complex network of brain activation, as we have suggested here. We hope that a similar brain network can be elucidated for tool-using corvids in the future, given the abundant evidence of tool use and manufacture in this family of birds. We know that New Caledonian crows and Hawaiian crows can use and manufacture tools, and New Caledonian crows can infer their physical properties (Hunt, 1996; Chappell and Kacelnik, 2002, 2004; Weir et al., 2002; Taylor et al., 2007; Rutz et al., 2016; Jelbert et al., 2019). Furthermore, corvid species that are not tool-users in the wild, such as rooks, Eurasian jays, and northern blue jays, can use and manufacture tools in captivity and use these skills to solve problems (Jones and Kamil, 1973; Seed et al., 2006; Tebbich et al., 2007; Bird and Emery, 2009a, b; Cheke et al., 2011). Additionally, ravens can use tools to obtain unreachable food and use tokens to barter (Heinrich and Bugnyar, 2005; Kabadayi and Osvath, 2017). Despite this plethora of examples, we have yet to discover the neural mechanisms underpinning these behaviors and their cognitive control in corvids. However, we do know about the neural mechanisms of song production and song learning in songbirds, which are birds of the same order as corvids. The knowledge on songbird neuroanatomy may serve as a basis to explore the neural mechanisms of tool use in corvids and to elucidate a neural network underpinning tool-using behavior. We have reviewed the current evidence of several bird brain regions that could be involved in tool use in corvids and that should be the focus of study in future research, such as the NCL, mesopallium, pallidostriatal complex, SpM, cerebellum, and areas of the tegmentum. Understanding the neural processes of tool use in animals other than primates would not only increase our understanding of the evolution of physical cognition in vertebrates, including a better understanding of animal intelligence, but also benefit our society by providing new models with which scientists can work to understand the origins of complex motor skills, and ultimately improve the lives of those affected by motor disabilities.

Author Contributions

MC-Á wrote the manuscript, with critical reviews and important discussions and additions by NC. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Prof. Sarah Woolley for useful comments on the first version of the manuscript, Ben Farrar for helpful comments and discussion of the manuscript, and Camilla Cenni, Prof. Jean-Baptiste Leca, Prof. Michael Colombo, and an undisclosed reviewer for their thorough and useful revisions of the manuscript.

References

Ambrose, S. H. (2001). Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science 291, 1748–1753. doi: 10.1126/science.1059487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auersperg, A. M. I., Szabo, B., Von Bayern, A. M. P., and Kacelnik, A. (2012). Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo. Curr. Biol. 22, R903–R904. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auersperg, A. M. I., von Bayern, A. M. I., Weber, S., Szabadvari, A., Bugnyar, T., and Kacelnik, A. (2014). Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20140972. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and Tool Use: Forms of Engagement in Human and Animal Use of Tools. London: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Baciadonna, L., Cornero, F., Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2020). Convergent evolution of complex cognition: Insights from the field of avian cognition into the study of self-awareness. Learn. Behav. doi: 10.3758/s13420-020-00434-5 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barton, R. A. (2012). Embodied cognitive evolution and the cerebellum. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2097–2107. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, R. A., and Venditti, C. (2014). Report rapid evolution of the cerebellum in humans and other great apes. Curr. Biol. 24, 2440–2444. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal Tool Behavior. New York, NY: Garland STPM Press.

Google Scholar

Bird, C. D., and Emery, N. J. (2009a). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 10370–10375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901008106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, C. D., and Emery, N. J. (2009b). Rooks use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating worm. Curr. Biol. 19, 1410–1414. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borra, E., Belmalih, A., Calzavara, R., Gerbella, M., Murata, A., Rozzi, S., et al. (2008). Cortical connections of the macaque anterior intraparietal (AIP) area. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1094–1111. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronowski, J. (1975). The Ascent of Man. Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Co.

Google Scholar

Brown, C. (2012). Tool use in fishes. Fish Fish. 13, 105–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-2979.2011.00451.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantalupo, C., and Hopkins, W. (2010). The cerebellum and its contribution to complex tasks in higher primates: a comparative perspective. Cortex 46, 821–830. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chappell, J., and Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Anim. Cogn. 5, 71–78. doi: 10.1007/s10071-002-0130-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chappell, J., and Kacelnik, A. (2004). Selection of tool diameter by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Anim. Cogn. 7, 121–127. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0202-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheke, L. G., Bird, C. D., and Clayton, N. S. (2011). Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius). Anim. Cogn. 14, 441–455. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0379-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, N. (2007). Animal cognition: crows spontaneously solve a metatool task. Curr. Biol. 17, 894–895. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, N. S., and Emery, N. J. (2015). Avian models for human cognitive neuroscience: a proposal. Neuron 86, 1330–1342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, M., Klarer, A., Johnston, M., and Rose, J. (2017). Prospective processing: behavioural and neural evidence. Jpn. J. Anim. Psychol. 67, 47–61. doi: 10.2502/janip.67.2.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinets, V., Brueggen, J. C., and Brueggen, J. D. (2015). Crocodilians use tools for hunting. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 27, 74–78. doi: 10.1080/03949370.2013.858276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditz, H. M., and Nieder, A. (2015). Neurons selective to the number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 7827–7832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504245112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditz, H. M., and Nieder, A. (2016). Sensory and working memory representations of small and large numerosities in the crow endbrain. J. Neurosci. 36, 12044–12052. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1521-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Divac, I., and Mogensen, J. (1985). The prefrontal “cortex” in the pigeon catecholamine histofluorescence. Neuroscience 15, 677–682. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(85)90069-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2004a). “Comparing the complex cognition of birds and primates,” in Comparative Vertebrate Cognition: Are Primates Superior to Non-Primates? eds L. J. Rogers and G. Kaplan (New York, NY: Kluwer Academic), 3–55. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-8913-0_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2004b). The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306, 1903–1908. doi: 10.1126/science.1098410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2009). Tool use and physical cognition in birds and mammals. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fragaszy, D. M., and Mangalam, M. (2018). Tooling. Adv. Study Behav. 50, 177–241. doi: 10.1016/bs.asb.2018.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frey, S. H. (2008). Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 1951–1957. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, J. P., McLean, D. A., Valyear, K. F., and Culham, J. C. (2013). Decoding the neural mechanisms of human tool use. eLife 2:e00425. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodall, J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature 201, 1264–1266. doi: 10.1038/2011264a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grobecker, D. B., and Pietsch, T. W. (1978). Crows use automobiles as nutcrackers. Auk 95, 760–761.

Google Scholar

Grodzinski, U., and Clayton, N. S. (2010). Problems faced by food-caching corvids and the evolution of cognitive solutions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 365, 977–987. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Reynolds, V., Wrangham, R., and Zuberbu, K. (2011). Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 1:128. doi: 10.1038/srep00128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gumert, M. D., Kluck, M., and Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman sea region of Thailand. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 594–608. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntürkün, O. (2005). The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 14, 686–693. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntürkün, O. (2020). The surprising power of the avian mind. Sci. Am. 322, 48–55.

Google Scholar

Güntürkün, O., and Bugnyar, T. (2016). Cognition without Cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 291–303. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntürkün, O., Ströckens, F., Scarf, D., and Colombo, M. (2017). Apes, feathered apes, and pigeons: differences and similarities. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 16, 35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Ibáñez, C., Iwaniuk, A. N., and Wylie, D. R. (2018). Parrots have evolved a primate-like telenceplalic-midbrain-cerebellar circuit. Sci. Rep. 8:9960.

Google Scholar

Heinrich, B., and Bugnyar, T. (2005). Testing problem solving in ravens: string-pulling to reach food. Ethology 111, 962–976. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01133.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2010). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1229–1243. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higuchi, S., Imamizu, H., and Kawato, M. (2007). Cerebellar activity evoked by common tool-use execution and imagery tasks: an fMRI study. Cortex 43, 350–358. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70460-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hihara, S., Notoya, T., Tanaka, M., Ichinose, S., Ojima, H., Obayashi, S., et al. (2006). Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia 44, 2636–2646. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hihara, S., Yamada, H., Iriki, A., and Okanoya, K. (2003). Spontaneous vocal differentiation of coo-calls for tools and food in Japanese monkeys. Neurosci. Res. 45, 383–389. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(03)00011-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodos, W. (1987). Animal general intelligence: an idea ahead of its time. Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 668–668. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00055138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopkins, W. D., Meguerditchian, A., Coulon, O., Misiura, M., Pope, S., Mareno, M. C., et al. (2017). Motor skill for tool-use is associated with asymmetries in Broca’s area and the motor hand area of the precentral gyrus in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behav. Brain Res. 318, 71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., and Schaeffer, J. A. (2012). The neural and cognitive correlates of aimed throwing in chimpanzees: a magnetic resonance image and behavioural study on a unique form of social tool use. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 37–47. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379, 249–251. doi: 10.1038/379249a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. R., and Gray, R. D. (2003). Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 867–874. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. R., and Gray, R. D. (2004). The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows. Biol. Lett. 271, 88–90. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. R., Gray, R. D., and Taylor, A. H. (2013). “Why is tool use rare in animals?” in Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology, eds C. M. Sanz, J. Call, and C. Boesch (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 89–118. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511894800.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriki, A. (2006). The neural origins and implications of imitation, mirror neurons and tool use. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 660–667. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriki, A., and Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 10–23. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N., Lefebvre, L., and Wylie, D. R. (2009). The comparative approach and brain–behaviour relationships: a tool for understanding tool use. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 150–159. doi: 10.1037/a0015678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarvis, E. D., Güntürkün, O., Bruce, L., Csillag, A., Karten, H., Kuenzel, W., et al. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 151–159. doi: 10.1038/nrn1606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelbert, S. A., Miller, R., Schiestl, M., Boeckle, M., Cheke, L. G., Gray, R. D., et al. (2019). New Caledonian crows infer the weight of objects from observing their movements in a breeze. Proc. Biol. Sci. 286:20182332. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.2332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2004). The neural bases of complex tool use in humans. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 71–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, T. B., and Kamil, A. C. (1973). Tool-making and tool-using in the Northern blue jay. Science 180, 1076–1078. doi: 10.1126/science.180.4090.1076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jønsson, K. A., Fabre, P., and Irestedt, M. (2012). Brains, tools, innovation and biogeography in crows and ravens. BMC Evol. Biol. 12:72. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabadayi, C., and Osvath, M. (2017). Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science 357, 202–204. doi: 10.1126/science.aam8138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerney, M., Smaers, J. B., Schoenemann, P. T., and Dunn, J. C. (2017). The coevolution of play and the cortico-cerebellar system in primates. Primates 58, 485–491. doi: 10.1007/s10329-017-0615-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenen, C., Millar, J., and Colombo, M. (2013). How bad do you want it? Reward modulation in the Avian Nidopallium Caudolaterale. Behav. Neurosci. 127, 544–554. doi: 10.1037/a0033551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, M. L., Seed, A. M., and Slocombe, K. E. (2015). A novel form of spontaneous tool use displayed by several captive greater vasa parrots (Coracopsis vasa). Biol. Lett. 11:20150861. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefebvre, L., Nicolakakis, N., and Boire, D. (2002). Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour 139, 939–973. doi: 10.1163/156853902320387918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legg, E. W., Ostojic, L., and Clayton, N. S. (2017). “Routes to the convergent evolution of cognition,” in Routledge Handbook of Evolution and Philosophy, ed. R. Joyce (Abingdon: Routledge), 237–251. doi: 10.4324/9781315764863-17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, J. W. (2006). Cortical networks related to human use of tools. Neuroscientist 12, 211–231. doi: 10.1177/1073858406288327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macphail, E. M. (1987). The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 645–656. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00054984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, J., Sargeant, B. L., Watson-Capps, J. J., Gibson, Q. A., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., et al. (2008). Why do dolphins carry sponges? PLoS One 3:e3868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., and Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Anim. Cogn. 11, 423–430. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0132-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, A., Lewenfus, G., Bittencourt-navarrete, R. E., Clasca, F., and Guedes, J. (2019). Thalamic inputs to posterior parietal cortical areas involved in skilled forelimb movement and tool use in the capuchin monkey. Cereb. Cortex 29, 5098–5115. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGrew, W. C. (2013). Is primate tool use special? Chimpanzee and new caledonian crow compared. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368:20120422. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehlhorn, J., Hunt, G. R., Gray, R. D., Rehkämper, G., and Güntürkün, O. (2010). Tool-making New Caledonian crows have large associative brain areas. Brain Behav. Evol. 75, 63–70. doi: 10.1159/000295151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mushiake, H., Sakamoto, K., Saito, N., Inui, T., Aihara, K., and Tanji, J. (2009). Involvement of the prefrontal cortex in problem solving. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 85, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(09)85001-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakley, K. P. (1972). Man the Tool-Maker, 6th Edn. London: British Museum (Natural History).

Google Scholar

Obayashi, S., Matsumoto, R., Suhara, T., Nagai, Y., and Iriki, A. (2007). Functional organization of monkey brain for abstract operation. Cortex 43, 389–396. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70464-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obayashi, S., Suhara, T., Kawabe, K., Okauchi, T., Maeda, J., Akine, Y., et al. (2001). Functional brain mapping of monkey tool use. Neuroimage 14, 853–861. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obayashi, S., Suhara, T., Nagai, Y., Maeda, J., Hihara, S., and Iriki, A. (2002). Macaque prefrontal activity associated with extensive tool use. Neuroreport 13, 2349–2354. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000044228.79663.ca

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obayashi, S., Suhara, T., Nagai, Y., Okauchi, T., Maeda, J., and Iriki, A. (2004). Monkey brain areas underlying remote-controlled operation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 1397–1407. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03200.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olkowicz, S., Kocourek, M., Luèan, R. K., Porteš, M., Fitch, W. T., Herculano-Houzel, S., et al. (2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 7255–7260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517131113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orban, G. A., and Caruana, F. (2014). The neural basis of human tool use. Front. Psychol. 5:310. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osiurak, F., and Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychol. Rev. 123, 534–568. doi: 10.1037/rev0000027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro, M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G., et al. (2009). The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J. Neurosci. 29, 11523–11539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2040-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, R. R., Rizzolatti, G., and Orban, G. A. (2013). Functional properties of the left parietal tool use region. Neuroimage 78, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penn, D. C., and Povinelli, D. J. (2007). Causal cognition in human and nonhuman animals: a comparative, critical review. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 97–118. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, K. A., and Thompson, C. R. (2013). Hand preference for tool-use in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) is associated with asymmetry of the primary motor cortex. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 435–440. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, J. D. J. (1986). A review of tool use in insects. Florida Entomol. 69, 95–104. doi: 10.2307/3494748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., and Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: a critical review of neuroimaging data. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 64, 421–437. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., and Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. Physiol. Rev. 94, 655–706. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00009.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Root-Bernstein, M., Narayan, T., Cornier, L., and Bourgeois, A. (2019). Context-specific tool use by Sus cebifrons. Mamm. Biol. 98, 102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2019.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutz, C., Klump, B. C., Komarczyk, L., Leighton, R., Kramer, J., Wischnewski, S., et al. (2016). Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature 537, 403–407. doi: 10.1038/nature19103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaal, S. (1999). Is imitation learning the route to humanoid robots? Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 233–242. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01327-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seed, A., Emery, N., and Clayton, N. (2009). Intelligence in corvids and apes: a case of convergent evolution? Ethology 115, 401–420. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01644.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seed, A. M., Tebbich, S., Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2006). Investigating physical cognition in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Curr. Biol. 16, 697–701. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smaers, J. B., Steele, J., Case, C. R., and Amunts, K. (2013). Laterality and the evolution of the prefronto-cerebellar system in anthropoids. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1288, 59–69. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 139–155. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0001092X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St Amant, R., and Horton, T. E. (2008). Revisiting the definition of animal tool use. Anim. Behav. 75, 1199–1208. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, J., Ferrari, P. F., and Fogassi, L. (2012). From action to language: comparative perspectives on primate tool use, gesture and the evolution of human language. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 4–9. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Striedter, G. F. (2013). Bird brains and tool use: beyond instrumental conditioning. Brain Behav. Evol. 82, 55–67. doi: 10.1159/000352003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takada, M., Hoshi, E., Saga, Y., Inoue, K., Miyachi, S., Hatanaka, N., et al. (2013). “Organization of two cortico-basal ganglia loop circuits that arise from distinct sectors of the monkey dorsal premotor cortex,” in Basal Ganglia - An Integrative View, ed. F. A. Barrios (Rijeka: InTech), 103–116. doi: 10.5772/54822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. H., and Clayton, N. S. (2012). Evidence from convergent evolution and causal reasoning suggests that conclusions on human uniqueness may be premature. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 241–242. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x11001956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. H., Elliffe, D., Hunt, G. R., and Gray, R. D. (2010). Complex cognition and behavioural innovation in New Caledonian crows. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 2637–2643. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C., and Gray, R. D. (2007). Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Curr. Biol. 17, 1504–1507. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Medina, F. S., and Gray, R. D. (2009). Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 247–254. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tebbich, S., Seed, A. M., Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2007). Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Anim. Cogn. 10, 225–231. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0061-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tebbich, S., Stankewitz, S., and Teschke, I. (2012). The relationship between foraging, learning abilities and neophobia in two species of Darwin’s finches. Ethology 118, 135–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02001.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tebbich, S., Taborsky, M., Fessl, B., and Blomqvist, D. (2001). Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning? Proc. Biol. Sci. 268, 2189–2193. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmermans, S., Lefebvre, L., Boire, D., and Basu, P. (2000). Relative size of the hyperstriatum ventrale is the best predictor of feeding innovation rate in birds. Brain Behav. Evol. 56, 196–203. doi: 10.1159/000047204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Essen, D. C., and Dierker, D. L. (2007). Surface-based and probabilistic atlases of primate cerebral cortex. Neuron 56, 209–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1970). Tool-using in primates and other vertebrates. Adv. Study Behav. 3, 195–249. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)60157-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., and Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 719–741. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1999.0304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, L., Hartmann, K., and Nieder, A. (2014). Neuronal correlates of visual working memory in the corvid endbrain. J. Neurosci. 34, 7778–7786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0612-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, L., Hartmann, K., and Nieder, A. (2017). Spatially tuned neurons in Corvid Nidopallium Caudolaterale signal target position during visual search. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1103–1112. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, L., and Nieder, A. (2013). Abstract rule neurons in the endbrain support intelligent behaviour in corvid songbirds. Nat. Commun. 4:2878. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, L., Pidpruzhnykova, G., and Nieder, A. (2015). Associative learning rapidly establishes neuronal representations of upcoming behavioral choices in crows. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 15208–15213. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509760112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldmann, C., and Güntürkün, O. (1993). The dopaminergic innervation of the pigeon caudolateral forebrain: immunocytochemical evidence for a “prefrontal cortex” in birds? Brain Res. 600, 225–234. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91377-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, A. H. (2005). Reciprocal evolution of the cerebellum and neocortex in fossil humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 3576–3580. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500692102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weir, A. A. S., Chappell, J., and Kacelnik, A. (2002). Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science 297:981. doi: 10.1126/science.1073433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welberg, L. (2008). Mirror neurons: singing in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9:163. doi: 10.1038/nrn2340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiten, A., Goodall, J., Mcgrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685.

Google Scholar

Keywords: tool use, neural mechanisms, neural network, causal reasoning, macaques, corvids

Citation: Cabrera-Álvarez MJ and Clayton NS (2020) Neural Processes Underlying Tool Use in Humans, Macaques, and Corvids. Front. Psychol. 11:560669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560669

Received: 09 May 2020; Accepted: 31 August 2020;
Published: 23 September 2020.

Edited by:

Michael Colombo, University of Otago, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Andrew N. Iwaniuk, University of Lethbridge, Canada
Jean-Baptiste Leca, University of Lethbridge, Canada
Camilla Cenni, Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Canada, in collaboration with reviewer J-BL

Copyright © 2020 Cabrera-Álvarez and Clayton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María J. Cabrera-Álvarez, CabreraResearch@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.