- 1Department of Systems Engineering and Engineering Management, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
- 2School of Business Administration, Zhejiang University of Finance and Economics, Hangzhou, China
- 3School of Business, Monash University Malaysia, Selangor Darul Ehsan, Malaysia
- 4Occupational Health and Safety Council of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
The impact of personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention is a classic topic in the field of entrepreneurship research. Previous research mostly used simple linear models, leading to a gap in the study on the interrelationship among personal characteristics and their systematic influence on entrepreneurial intention. This study investigates the interrelationship among the four specific entrepreneurial characteristics (i.e., need for achievement, locus of control, risk-taking propensity, and creativity) and their systematic influence on the entrepreneurial intention of engineering students. The research data is from 210 engineering students via a survey. Logistic regression and path analysis were used for data analysis. The findings suggest that creativity and risk-taking directly influence entrepreneurial intention while the need for achievement and the locus of control influence it indirectly. Implications for entrepreneurship education are finally discussed.
Introduction
The impact of personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention is a classic topic in the field of entrepreneurship research. Tremendous studies applied simple correlation, t-test, ANOVA, or linear regression to investigate the subject. However, the results are so inconsistent that the research nearly comes to an end. Coming into the 1980s and 1990s, scholars started to suspect the relationship between the two variables (Gartner, 1988, 1990; Behave, 1994). There are even references saying that no further research on personal characteristics is needed (Gartner, 1988, 1990). This background triggers the following research questions:
Do the findings on the influence of personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention that insisted for several decades have no more meaning?
Do the inconsistent results of the effect of characteristics on intention thoroughly cast down the relationship between the two variables?
Do the behavioral models explain entrepreneurial intention perfectly?
Shall we continue to research the impact of personal characteristics in the period of entrepreneurial education?
There have been many studies on student entrepreneurial intention (Tkachev and Kolvereid, 1999; Lüthje and Franke, 2003; Wilson et al., 2007; Sun et al., 2017). However, İrengün and Arıkboğa (2015) suggest that education and training should center itself much more in changing personal attitudes than in knowledge. Personality is one of the factors that influence people’s attitudes. Frank et al. (2007) conducted a comprehensive study of different groups of people (e.g., secondary student students, university students, potential business founders, and successful business founders) and found different results for these groups using regression analysis. For example, for secondary school students, NACH and CA are significant entrepreneurial characteristics, while for university students, CA is not significant. For potential business founders and successful business founders, only RT significantly influences the entrepreneurial intention.
Therefore, it is timely research to re-consider the impact of personality on entrepreneurial intention for the following reasons.
Firstly, entrepreneurial individuals are the central part of entrepreneurship. Without the person, the action will never take place. However, not all people will become entrepreneurs even when circumstances are comparable. There must be some types of individual predisposition toward entrepreneurship, such as personal characteristics (Stewart, 1996). Therefore, personal characteristics are relevant for explaining the disposition of entrepreneurs to act entrepreneurially and why entrepreneurial behaviors differ under similar situations. Recent years see a revival of personality research in many areas such as performance, leadership, industrial, and organizational psychology (Rauch and Frese, 2000; Judge et al., 2002). Entrepreneurship is surely an active participant in this revival stream (Rauch and Frese, 2007a, b). Meta-analytic evidence (Collins et al., 2004; Stewart and Roth, 2004; Rauch and Frese, 2007b) suggests that personality does influence entrepreneurial intention and there has been a call to action for psychology for entrepreneurship research and practice (Hisrich et al., 2007), especially in entrepreneurial education (Hansemark, 1998; Frank et al., 2005; Hisrich et al., 2007) that aims at fostering entrepreneurial spirits and intentions.
Secondly, though the number of studies is huge, scholars have found incongruous results on the impact of personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention during past decades. For example, some researchers reported a significant influence of the need for achievement (Howell and Higgins, 1990; Langan-Fox and Roth, 1995), while some did not (Bonnett and Furnham, 1991; Cromie et al., 1992; Ho and Koh, 1992; Koh, 1996). Incongruous results were also found for other characteristics such as locus of control, risk-taking propensity, and creativity. It is perhaps a methodological issue. Majority of the studies focused on the simple correlation of the characteristics and intention or comparison of different groups (e.g., entrepreneurs and non-entrepreneurs) (Robinson et al., 1991a, b; Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992; Cromie et al., 1992; Ho and Koh, 1992; Carland et al., 1995; Green et al., 1996; Koh, 1996; Gurol and Atsan, 2006). Very few studies emphasized the interrelationships among the characteristics (Stewart, 1996). These types of research did not consider causal relationships and the issue of collinearity. Therefore some researchers claimed that the effect of personal characteristics on entrepreneurship had not been fully investigated (Johnson, 2003; Tett et al., 2003). Could there be significant interrelationship among the characteristics beyond their direct effect on intention? If the personal characteristics are measuring different perspectives of an individual, they are theoretically related to one another. This leads to another question: Is it possible that the relationships of the characteristics suppress their significant effect on intention? The influence of some characteristics on entrepreneurial intention is likely through the effect of others. This may help to illuminate the systematic influence of the characteristics and contribute to a better understanding of the impact. We clarify that the present study is not opposed to the behavioral approaches to entrepreneurship. Since neither personality nor behavioral approach fully explains the phenomenon, a comprehensive entrepreneurship model must consider a set of factors including background factors, personalities (broad and specific), attitudes, situational and contextual factors, and other factors that may affect the entrepreneurial intention, process, and performance. This study stresses the significance of personality, which is an inherent part of entrepreneurship study (Cromie et al., 1992; Stewart, 1996; Rauch and Frese, 2007a).
This study aims to investigate the empirical relationship between personal characteristics and entrepreneurial intention of engineering students. It focuses on two types of variables: entrepreneurial characteristics and intention. This study will contribute to (1) short-listing the most frequently cited personal characteristics that are pertinent to entrepreneurial intention, (2) exploring the interrelationship among these personal characteristics, (3) identifying the systematic impact of these personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention, and (4) offering advice for designing entrepreneurship education programs in line with personal characteristics.
Literature Review and Hypotheses
Entrepreneurial Intention (EINT) and the Theory of Planned Behavior
Psychologists have claimed that an assessment of current intentions is the most obvious way of predicting the behavior itself (Ajzen, 1991). In various situations, intentions are the most effective predictor of behaviors such as job search activities and career choice (Kolvereid, 1996). The underlying assumption is that behaviors can be planned and under volitional control. That means a person can decide to do or not to do at all. This is called the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980) that has been applied in many intention studies. The behaviors not planned or not under volitional control will not necessarily be directly determined by intentions and may thus result in a weak relation between intentions and behavior. Entrepreneurship is obviously planned. Entrepreneurs can decide to be involved in business creation or not. So it can be best predicted by the intention of the persons (Bird, 1988; Krueger and Carsrud, 1993). Many studies on entrepreneurship investigate entrepreneurial intention (Krueger et al., 2000; Carr and Sequeira, 2007), especially those on students (Davidsson, 1995; Autio et al., 1997; Fayolle et al., 2006a, b; Sun et al., 2017). In this study, entrepreneurial intention is defined as one’s judgments and attitude toward the likelihood of developing one’s venture and business (Grant, 1996). Before starting a new business, intentions to turn an idea into an actual business venture have been planned. The works by Bird (1988) and Krueger and Brazeal (1994) have shown that entrepreneurial behavior is the result of entrepreneurial intention. Studying intention gives us valuable insights into new venture initiation. For most students who are still on campus, the chance to start a new business is relatively low. It is difficult to collect data about entrepreneurial actions. So, the entrepreneurial intention is the most proper dependent variable if students are the subject of study.
Personal Characteristics and the Trait Theory of Entrepreneurship
According to the trait theory of entrepreneurship, people who have entrepreneurial characteristics tend to have higher intentions to be involved in entrepreneurial activities (Caird, 1991; Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992; Gurol and Atsan, 2006). The relationship will be elaborated in the following hypotheses.
Relationship Between Personal Characteristics and Entrepreneurial Intention (EINT)
Personal characteristics are significant factors in an entrepreneurship model (Mueller and Thomas, 2000). They are found associated with entrepreneurial motivation and intentions (Grant, 1996). Characteristics that predispose an individual to entrepreneurial intentions are called entrepreneurial characteristics. From an extensive review of nearly 80 publications on psychological entrepreneurship, we identified four most frequently cited entrepreneurial characteristics, namely, need for achievement (NACH, which has been cited 42 times), risk-taking propensity (RT, 36 times), locus of control (LOC, 33 times), and creativity (CA, 30 times). Others were cited only a few times. The four characteristics are also considered as the specific personalities which are proximal to entrepreneurship (Johnson, 2003; Rauch and Frese, 2007a). We extracted 21 most recent empirical studies covering one or all of the four characteristics and summarized them in Table 1.
Different results exist regarding the impacts of the four characteristics on entrepreneurial intention (EINT). Among the 21 studies, only three studies (Caird, 1991; Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992; Gurol and Atsan, 2006) covered all four entrepreneurial characteristics and found the same results that all the four personal characteristics were significantly different between entrepreneurs (or entrepreneurially inclined persons) and non-entrepreneurs (or those who are non-entrepreneurially inclined) based on their t-test results. However, using the same data analysis method, Ho and Koh (1992) argued that NACH did not have a significant impact on entrepreneurial inclination. Considering only three of the characteristics (NACH, RT, and CA), Howell and Higgins (1990); Stewart (1996), and Stewart et al. (1999) used regression analysis or logit analysis/logistic regression analysis and found that all the three characteristics were significantly related to entrepreneurial intention. However, using logit analysis or ANOVA, Cromie et al. (1992) and Koh (1996) posited that among the four characteristics, only two of them, RT and CA, exerted significant influence on entrepreneurial intention. While Bonnett and Furnham (1991) considered the effect of NACH and LOC and reported that LOC was significantly related to entrepreneurial intention, Langan-Fox and Roth (1995) found the opposite results (i.e., LOC was not significant). On the other hand, Hansemark (1998) using t-test and Entrialgo et al. (2000) using regression contended that both NACH and LOC were significant.
Although all the above studies considered NACH, RT, LOC, and CA as the independent variables and EINT as the dependent, they had different results. Concerning these inconsistent results as well as the relatively simple analysis methods used, we see a need to study the four characteristics with more sophisticated statistical tools such as SEM path analysis, which can study the postulated causal relationship considering all variables involved (Kline, 1998). Therefore, this study will first adopt the same structure to investigate the direct relationship between the entrepreneurial characteristics and intention using SEM analysis and then go deeper to study the inter-relationships among the characteristics. In this sense, the first four hypotheses are related to the direct relationship of NACH, RT, LOC, and CA to EINT.
NACH is the impetus that drives a person to struggle for success and perfection (Sagie and Elizur, 1999). RT is the propensity for risk-taking as the perceived probability of receiving the rewards associated with success before the potential entrepreneur actually subjects himself/herself to the consequences associated with failure (Brockhaus, 1980). LOC refers to an individual’s perceptions about the main underlying causes of events in his/her life. Rotter (1966) reported that LOC could be seen as either internal or external. Internals have higher achievement motivation than externals because internals believe their behavior is guided by their personal decisions and efforts, whereas externals feel guided by fate, luck, or other external circumstances. CA relates to perceiving and acting in new and unique ways (Robinson et al., 1991b).
The four characteristics have been recognized as the core elements that influence an entrepreneur’s decision making and behaviors, and can be used to differentiate entrepreneurs from the general people. For example, it has been found that entrepreneurs who have high NACH are more desirable to be successful and are subsequently more probably to behave creatively and entrepreneurially (Langan-Fox and Roth, 1995; Koh, 1996). They have also significantly higher than non-founders on RT because they have risk preferences (Shaver and Scott, 1991; Stewart and Roth, 2001; Raab et al., 2005) and have an internal locus of control attributed to their high self-esteem or confidence to control their lives (Entrialgo et al., 2000; Utsch and Rauch, 2000). Further, entrepreneurs are significantly more creative than non-entrepreneurs as they keep searching for new opportunities and taking a creative attitude toward their businesses (Utsch and Rauch, 2000). Based on the above discussions, we developed a foundation for proposing the following hypotheses concerning the personal characteristics and entrepreneurial intention:
H1: Need for achievement positively relates to entrepreneurial intention.
H2: Risk-taking propensity positively relates to entrepreneurial intention.
H3: Locus of control positively relates to entrepreneurial intention.
H4: Creativity positively relates to entrepreneurial intention.
Relationships Among the Four Personal Characteristics
Beyond the direct relationships between entrepreneurial characteristics and intention, this study goes deeper to explore the hidden inter-relationship among them. Most of the studies on entrepreneurial characteristics merely concentrated on the simple relationship between the characteristics and intention, and seldom touched upon the inter-relationship among the characteristics. Although Stewart (1996) studied the interaction between RT and NACH and between RT and CA, the author did not indicate the direction of the relationships. As a result, the previous studies seem to leave a gap for exploring how the personal characteristics influence one another in the formation process of EINT. This is important because it can bring in-depth insights into how the personal characteristics contribute to EINT and hence helps to explain their weak relationship obtained in previous studies. These will derive implications for entrepreneurship education aimed at nurturing the entrepreneurial spirits and intentions of students. If entrepreneurial characteristics can be learned or changed through education (Timmons et al., 1985; Hood and Young, 1993; Hansemark, 1998), understanding the relations among the characteristics will help educators to design teaching activities that nurture entrepreneurial characteristics more effectively. Therefore, this study accounts for the inter-relationship among the entrepreneurial characteristics.
According to McClelland (1987), high need achievers demonstrate a higher performance in challenging tasks (RT), look for tasks involving personal responsibility and display high self-esteem and confidence (LOC), and are creative in the sense of looking for new and better ways to improve their performance (CA). Therefore, the four characteristics measure different perspectives of an entrepreneur and influence one another.
NACH is highly related to a strong task, goal orientation, and an obsession with a task to be done (McClelland, 1985). People with high NACH are desirable to assume personal responsibility for performing a task, incline to set difficult goals, and are more eager to receive feedback (Cherrington, 1991). Since higher confidence will result in desirable outcomes, a high level of NACH is assumed. Thus, those who have internal LOC are likely to have a high level of NACH (Spector, 1982; McClelland, 1985). This relation is explained by the theories of intrinsic motivation (Deci and Ryan, 1985; Amabile and Hennessey, 1992) that internal locus of causality will contribute to intrinsic motivation. NACH, with its properties of need for competence and self-determination, is considered as a conceptualization of intrinsic motivation (Deci and Ryan, 1985). In accordance with the theoretical consistency between LOC and NACH, some studies have indicated that internal LOC positively influences NACH (Yukl and Latham, 1978; Abdel-Halim, 1980).
Although conversely, achievement motivation may enhance internal control belief based on the high achievement motivation, entrepreneurs will insist on taking personal responsibility for their performance. Scholars generally appear convinced that LOC results in achievement motivation (Colin, 1998; Reeve, 2001). Believing in one’s active influence, internals have higher motivation to reach success. In contrast, externally controlled people may be more passive. If one believes that he/she is not able to control business outcomes, he/she has no reason to actively change one’s environment (Rauch and Frese, 2007a). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:
H5: Locus of control positively relates to the need for achievement.
Control beliefs are energized by a need for control over meaningful life and business situations. People with internal LOC believe in their abilities to control their actions. Thus they are more tolerant of the risks when starting a business (Hillson and Murray-Webster, 2007). Internal LOC presupposes personal responsibility that drives individuals to pursue a high achievement in challenging environments. Entrepreneurs are thought to perform best in situations where they have personal responsibility for venture outcomes (Petrakis, 2007). Moreover, entrepreneurs are also confident in their abilities and exhibit resilience in the face of setbacks and can start over again when disappointments happen. They will pursue value creativity and do things not generally done in the ordinary course, and typically considered as innovators as well (Schumpeter, 2000; Drucker, 2007). Thus, internal LOC beliefs drive individuals to take risks and perform in creative ways. The evidence supports this belief that individuals with greater internal beliefs are more creative, risk-taking, and more entrepreneurial (Miller et al., 1982; Caird, 1991; Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992; Chen et al., 1998). Therefore, we proposed that:
H6: Locus of control positively relates to risk-taking propensity.
H7: Locus of control positively relates to creativity.
People with a high level of needs tend to prefer challenging tasks (McClelland, 1985). In Atkinson’s (1957) formulation of risk-taking, NACH and fear of failure (i.e., anxiety about failing) have been considered as the operative and competing motives. The role of achievement motive in risk-taking was further explored by McClelland (1985) in relation to entrepreneurship. The individual’s needs and motives have been found to influence moderately risky and challenging tasks. McClelland believes that persons with high NACH have moderate RT. According to Atkinson (1957), a subjective probability of success of 50 percent is moderate risk and would generate the most achievement motivation. Stewart (1996) tested the relationship between RT and NACH and found a significant relationship between these two variables. However, the author did not specify the direction of the relationship. Hillson and Murray-Webster (2007) have posited that NACH enhances RT. They claimed that the level of achievement motivation affects an individual’s attitude toward the outcomes of risky situations, and therefore, the individual’s attitude toward risk. They also argued that high achievers are individuals who have a stronger motive to achieve relative to the motive to avoid failure, while low achievers have a stronger motive to avoid failure relative to their motive to achieve.
CA is another factor that is significantly influenced by NACH (Howell and Higgins, 1990). McClelland (1985) reviewed two decades of research and found that achievement-motivated individuals tend to move risk-taking like moving to another place to start a business and be more creative. “This is why entrepreneurial groups high in NACH are so fixated on finding a short cut to the goal that they may not be too particular about the means they use to reach it” (McClelland, 1985, p. 250). Utsch and Rauch’s (2000) study also stressed the positive relationship between the two variables. Thus, those with higher achievement motivation tend to be more creative. So the following two hypotheses are proposed.
H8: Need for achievement positively relates to risk-taking propensity.
H9: Need for achievement positively relates to creativity.
Finally, entrepreneurs explicitly see new and unusual solutions to problems, and they are expected to exhibit a greater degree of risk-taking (Cromie et al., 1992; Gurol and Atsan, 2006; Hillson and Murray-Webster, 2007). A significant relation between RT and CA was identified by Stewart (1996). Goldsmith (1994) and Howell and Higgins (1990) indicated that individuals who are willing to take risks tend to be more creative. Based on the discussion above, we postulate that:
H10: Risk-taking positively relates to creativity.
The Conceptual Model
The preceding discussion leads to the research model presented in Figure 1. In this model, we employ the concepts of both entrepreneurial characteristics and intention. As specified in the entrepreneurial literature, the four characteristics have a direct impact on entrepreneurial intention, respectively. Particularly, LOC predisposes individuals to risk-taking, achievement motivation, and creativity. NACH exerts a positive effect on both RT and CA. Lastly, RT is positively related to CA. This model has two basic features. Firstly, it comprises the characteristics of a typical trait model of entrepreneurship, demonstrating the specific impact of the personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention. Secondly, it expands the inter-relations among the characteristics, providing a complete picture of how the entrepreneurial characteristics influence intention. That is, the model offers an in-depth and systematic approach to explain the influence of the characteristics on intention.
Figure 1. The conceptual model and hypotheses. LOC, locus of control; NACH, Need for achievement; RT, Risk-taking; CA, Creativity; EINT, Entrepreneurial intention.
Materials and Methods
Survey Design
This study applied a cross-sectional survey design. This approach has been widely used in entrepreneurship studies (Krueger et al., 2000; Autio et al., 2001; Lüthje and Franke, 2003; Kristiansen and Indarti, 2004).
Questionnaires were sent to 300 students in engineering management courses (majored in manufacturing engineering and engineering management, and engineering management) in a university in Hong Kong. All questions were randomly printed without a logical sequence in versions that we distributed to the respondents. Also, there was no title for each section such that the students did not know the managerial content. Hence, the priming effects, item-context-induced mood states, and other biases related to the question context could be reduced (Podsakoff et al., 2003). Items measuring each of the constructs have been widely studied in entrepreneurship research (Rotter, 1966; Jackson, 1994) and have been considered valid and reliable with proven psychometric properties, which suggest that they are likely resistant to common method variance (Spector, 1987). Further, the items for each construct were different and no common items existed among the constructs. Hence it might help reduce confusion and ambiguity. Before the survey, a pilot study was also conducted among 10 engineering students to ensure clarity of all questions.
At the beginning of the survey, the respondents were told that the answers provided for selection did not mean the higher the better or the lower the better, and there was no right or wrong answers. All questions were anonymous. The survey was not a part of an exam or a form of evaluation and had nothing to do with the students’ performance. The students were told to answer the questions according to their true feelings about the questions, and the reliability of the completed questionnaires would be finally checked and that the improper ones would be screened out. These procedures help the respondents try to avoid providing similar answers to all questions, or providing answers on the basis to get social approval and acceptance, hence to reduce errors due to consistency motif and social desirability (Podsakoff et al., 2003).
Leniency bias (refers to the tendency for raters to rate their friends higher or better) was not relevant to our survey since the questions were not good or bad type, and they were about measuring the personalities of the students themselves. Implicit theory and illusion correlation (which refers to the rater’s assumption of co-occurrence and correlation of variables) was not concerned much in the study. The respondents were engineering students, not professional researchers in management. Further, they did not receive training in management correlation and management research. Moreover, the measure of personal characteristics (independent variables) and that of entrepreneurial intention (dependent variable) were different. The former was measure by 1–5 scale and the latter was categorical scale (Y/N). It seems difficult for those students to make consistency or implicit answers. Thus, the influence of implicit or explicit correlation was not problematic in this research.
Finally, this research does not aim at personality evaluation and is unlikely to seduce common method bias. Further, a set of procedural remedies have been applied to minimize the method biases. Therefore, the problem of common method bias may not seem to be a concern in this research. This was further confirmed by a Harmon one-factor test (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986), a widely used method to test the common method bias (Andersson and Bateman, 1997; Aulakh and Geneturk, 2000; Iverson and Maguire, 2000). If a single factor emerges from the factor analysis or one general factor that accounts for the majority of the covariance among the measure, common method bias is a problem. Results of the test suggested the presence of five factors, indicating that common method bias was not a pervasive problem in this study (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986; Andersson and Bateman, 1997; Aulakh and Geneturk, 2000) as well as the updated method (Tan et al., 2018; Loh et al., 2019).
Profile of the Respondents
One hundred and forty-three usable questionnaires were returned with a 48 percent of usable response rate. Non-response bias was investigated by comparing the first 25 percent of the response with the last 25 percent of the response. No significant differences were found for several demographic characteristics such as age, education level, work experience, and average academic performance, suggesting that non-response bias was probably not an issue in the sample (Armstrong and Overton, 1977). One hundred and twenty-four of the samples were male students and 19 were female students. The students were in their bachelor’s (46.85%) or master’s (53.15%) study when they joined the survey. The respondents have an average age of 27. The average work experience was 4.7 years. Eighty-two respondents had entrepreneurial intention to establish their businesses soon, while 61 respondents lacked the intention to start their businesses.
Measures
The questionnaires were developed based on five constructs: NACH, RT, LOC, CA, and EINT. Eight items measured each of the four characteristics. Respondents would indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement using a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The higher scores they chose, the higher level of NACH, RT, internal LOC, and CA they perceived.
NACH was measured by a subset of items extracted from King (1985), while LOC was measured by Rotter’s (1966) scale used by Mueller and Thomas (2000). Items for RT and CA were adapted from the Jackson Personality Inventory (Jackson, 1994). The measurement was analyzed through factor analysis and reliability test. These data showed a subjects-to-variables ratio of 7.5:1, which is considered adequate for factor analysis (MacCallum et al., 1999; Hair et al., 2006). The data also showed an appropriate correlation matrix with substantial number of correlations greater than 0.30 (Flynn et al., 1994; Hair et al., 2006). Further, the Bartlett test of sphericity was significant and the Kaiser-Meyerr-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.807, which is far greater than the cut-off value of 0.6. In addition, measures of sampling adequacy (MSA) values were also well above the acceptable level of 0.5 (Coakes and Steed, 1999; Hair et al., 2006). Therefore, the data was appropriate to perform factor analysis. The remaining items of each of the entrepreneurial characteristics were exactly converged into their belonging factors, with loadings of 0.54 or above, which is practically significant (Hair et al., 2006). The total variance explained was 55 percent, which is higher than the acceptable level of 0.5 (Merenda, 1997). The values of Cronbach’s alpha were from 0.68 or above, indicating adequate internal reliability of the measurements (Nunnaly, 1978).
The measure of entrepreneurial intention was developed based on the item used in Krueger et al.’s (2000) study, in which a single-item variable was used. This study is interested in a general measure of intention to start up in the future rather than a specific measure (covering, such as specific kind of business to be created, or specific activities related to entrepreneurship to be performed). According to Nagy (2002) and Wanous et al. (1997), a single-item measure of a general construct is appropriate. Further single-item scales are less time-consuming and are not monotonous for the respondents to complete (Gardner et al., 1998), as many potential respondents are not willing to spend time to complete an extensive survey (Pomeroy et al., 2001).
Despite its inherent limitation, the single-item measure has been used in many different disciplines, including business (Nagy, 2002), education (Wanous and Hudy, 2001), and psychology (Killgore, 1999; Robins et al., 2001). These authors noted several advantages of single-item measures, such as increased face validity and flexibility, good reliability, and adequacy. In entrepreneurship, single-item scales measuring entrepreneurial intention have also been verified by researches. For example, Koh (1996); Krueger et al. (2000), and Peterman and Kennedy (2003) used a single item to measure of the entrepreneurial intention of respondents by asking if they have entrepreneurial intention. Similarly, in our study, the students were asked if they had intention to establish their own business in the future.
Demographic information was also collected to develop a profile of the sample and verify that respondents who had entrepreneurial intention and those who had not been homogeneous with respect to demographic factors. This helped ensure that such factors did not confound the results. For this purpose, questions on age, gender, education level, work experience, and academic performance were included in the questionnaire. The questionnaire is listed in Appendix.
Data Analysis Methods
A set of statistical methods (using SPSS and Amos) was employed. The first step of the analysis involved a chi-square test, which was used to verify that the respondents who had entrepreneurial intention and those who had not in the sample were homogeneous with respect to their demographic characteristics. Secondly, t-tests were conducted to test if significant differences regarding the four characteristics existed between the two subgroups. Then binary logistic regression and SEM path analysis were used to evaluate the conceptual model and to test the 10 null hypotheses specified in the study. Binary logistic regression (logistic model or logit model) was employed because it is useful to describe the relationship between one or more factors and an outcome (which only takes two possible values: yes or no) (Agresti, 2002). Thus, it is suitable to analyze the direct effect of the four characteristics on entrepreneurial intention, which is a dichotomy in nature (i.e., H1–H4). The sample size of 143 of this study satisfies the requirement of logistic regression analysis, where the ratio of 20 observations for each predictor variable is suggested (Hair et al., 1998). In the logistic regression model, the intention was the dependent variable, and the four characteristics were independent.
The relationships among the characteristics (i.e., H5–H10) were tested by path analysis, a subset of Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). In the path model, LOC was exogenous variable, while the other three characteristics were endogenous variables. The SEM path analysis used in this study has two advantages: (1) It simultaneously tests all relationships within the model; and (2) It tests the goodness of fit for different nested models (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988). The relationship among the characteristics can be tested simultaneously with path analysis [using Amos 16.0 with maximum likelihood estimation (MLE)]. The sample size of this study just fulfilled the requirement that a sample size of 100–200 is recommended to have confidence in the goodness of fit test (Hoyle, 1995). In terms of model fit, the following indices were used: Chi-square (x∧2) statistics, Chi-square statistics divided by the degree of freedom (x∧2/df); Goodness-of-fit index (GFI), Adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI), Comparative fit index (CFI), Normed fit index (NFI), Tucker-Lewis coefficient (TLI), and Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). As suggested in the literature (Kline, 1998), the following criteria of the indices were used to assess the model-fitting: x∧2 statistic is not significant (p > 0.05); x∧2/df ratio is recommended to be less than 3; the values of GFI, AGFI, CFI, NFI, and TLI are recommended to be greater than 0.90; and RMSEA is recommended to be up to 0.05, and acceptable up to 0.08.
Analysis and Results
Demographic Factors
The objective of this study is to investigate the systematic influence of personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention of engineering students. Chi-square tests of independence were performed to investigate the impact of the demographic factors on the decision of the students on performing entrepreneurial acts to ensure factors such as demographic variables did not confound the results. Thus, the differences with respect to the age, gender, education level, work experience, and average academic performance between those who had entrepreneurial intention and those who had not were tested. The results are reported in Table 2. None of the demographic factors was significantly different between the two groups at the 0.05 significance level. Accordingly, these demographic factors had no significant impact on entrepreneurial intention. That is, the two groups of respondents could be considered homogeneous concerning their backgrounds. Given the results, it was possible to test if entrepreneurial intention was significantly associated with the four characteristics without the confounding effects of demographic variables.
The mean scores shown in Table 3 were consistent with expectations reflected in the literature review and indicated that those who with entrepreneurial intention had greater need for achievement and higher propensity to take risk, and were more internally controlled and creative. To investigate the differences statistically at the univariate level, t-tests were conducted (Table 3). At a 0.05 significance level, the results showed that the entrepreneurially intended had significantly higher NACH (p = 0.005) and propensity to take risk (p = 0.000), and were more internally controlled (p = 0.001) and creative (p = 0.000). These imply that the characteristics were related to entrepreneurial intention and provide evidence for further investigation of their systematic impact.
Tests of Hypotheses
Direct Effect of the Personal Characteristics on Entrepreneurial Intention (H1–H4)
Performing a logistic regression analysis to test H1–H4. Table 4 summarizes the results. The Hosmer-Lemeshow (H-L) Chi-square test for the logistic regression model yielded a p-value of 0.279, thus suggesting a good fit. The holdout accuracy rates of the logistic regression model are presented in the upper part of the table. When the four characteristics enter into the model equation simultaneously, the overall accuracy rate of the final model was good at 81.1 percent, which is much higher than blindly estimating the most frequent category (entrepreneurial intention) for all cases at 57.3 percent. This accuracy rate is acceptably high compared with the figure reported by Robinson et al. (1991b), 77 percent, using NACH, LOC, CA, and self-confidence to predict entrepreneurial intention. Therefore, the four characteristics respectively had a significant effect on EINT of the engineering students. The importance of specific parameters of the model is reflected in the lower part of Table 4. Using p = 0.05 as a cutoff criterion for not including variables in the equation, it appears that RT (H2) and CA (H4) are important predictors of entrepreneurial intention, while NACH (H1) and LOC (H3) did not appear to be important. Thus, H2 and H4 were supported while H1 and H3 were rejected.
The Relations Among the Four Personal Characteristics (H5–H10)
H5 to H10 postulating the relationships among the four characteristics were tested with path analysis. Since the hypothesized path model was saturated, perfect model fit was obtained (i.e., GFI = NFI = CFI = 1.0), but the path coefficient from RT to CA was not significant (H10). This path was eliminated and the modified model was tested again. The results are shown in Table 5. As can be seen in the new model, all relationships hypothesized were significant (path LOC → NACH and NACH → CA were significant at p = 0.001; LOC → CA and LOC → RT at p = 0.05; NACH → RT was marginally significant at p = 0.1). A chi-square of 1.235 (df = 1; p = 0.267), x∧2/df ratio (1.235), other goodness-of-fit statistics greater than 0.9 (GFI = 0.996; AGFI = 0.957; CFI = 0.998; NFI = 0.989; TLI = 0.987) and REMSEA (0.041) indicated good model fit. In general, the model explained 34 percent of NACH, 14 percent of RT, and 22 percent of CA. Therefore, H5, 6, 7, 8, and 9 were supported. LOC had a positive effect on NACH, RT, and CA, respectively; NACH was positively related to RT and CA. However, RT was failed to relate to CA. Thus, H10 was not supported.
In sum, the test results (significant relationships) are summarized in Figure 2. It can be seen that the four characteristics influenced entrepreneurial intention in a holistic way. LOC positively affected the other three characteristics and NACH had a positive impact on RT and CA, which exerted a direct effect on the intention.
Figure 2. Influence of the personal characteristics on entrepreneurial intention. LOC, locus of control; NACH, Need for achievement; RT, Risk-taking; CA, Creativity; EINT, Entrepreneurial intention.
Discussion and Implications
Research Method Implications
This study investigated the systematic influence of the four specific entrepreneurial characteristics (NACH, RT, LOC, and CA) on EINT through examining their inter-relationships. Most of the existing studies on the relationship between personal characteristics and intention focused on examining the responses of entrepreneurs and non-entrepreneurs (Robinson et al., 1991b; Carland et al., 1995; Palich and Bagby, 1995; Chen et al., 1998), those who were entrepreneurial oriented/inclined and those who were not (Koh, 1996; Gurol and Atsan, 2006), business-owners/managers and other occupation groups (Caird, 1991), and students, entrepreneurs, and managers (Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992). These results relied on the analysis of t-test, Pearson correlation, ANOVA, or simple linear model (Bonnett and Furnham, 1991; Cromie and O’Donoghue, 1992; Cromie et al., 1992; Ho and Koh, 1992; Green et al., 1996; Gurol and Atsan, 2006). It seems challenging to reveal the comprehensive influence of the personality characteristics on intention based on the simple correlations of the characteristics and the comparison results of different groups. Therefore, as highly emphasized in the review study of Dean et al. (2007), more sophisticated research designs are needed for hypothesis testing in the field of entrepreneurship. This study investigated the systematic effect of entrepreneurial characteristics on EINT using SEM path analysis, a sophisticated statistical method which is appropriate to understand the critical aspects of entrepreneurship (Brush et al., 2003; Dean et al., 2007).
Theoretical Implications
Results of the current study (in Table 4) indicated that collectively, the four characteristics could distinguish those who had entrepreneurial intention from those who had not at an overall holdout accuracy rate of 81.1 percent, which could be considered adequate. The results showed that the engineering students who had a higher chance to take risk and were more creative tended to create their businesses. The findings supported the literature on trait model that people who possess the entrepreneurial characteristics tend to have higher intention to perform entrepreneurial acts and are consistent with the findings of Caird (1991); Cromie and O’Donoghue (1992), and Gurol and Atsan (2006). The findings were also in line with the logit analysis results of Koh (1996), where risk-taking and creativity were the predictors of entrepreneurial inclination.
Although NACH (H1) and internal LOC (H3) were not found to influence the students’ entrepreneurial intention directly, they had a significant indirect effect. For example, LOC affected ENIT through its effect on the other three characteristics (H5–H7), and NACH affected ENIT through RT and CA (H8–H9) (Table 5). That is, the students who had internal control beliefs would have higher achievement motivation, higher propensity to take risk, and be more creative. Further, those who had higher entrepreneurial motivation would have stronger desire to take risk and adopt creative ideas. The current results concurred with the previous findings that internal locus of control leads to high motivation (Colin, 1998; Reeve, 2001), thus predicts preference for risky and challenging tasks (Hillson and Murray-Webster, 2007) and turns individuals to be less conforming and do things not generally done in the ordinary course of business routine (Schumpeter, 2000; Drucker, 2007).
RT and CA (H10) has no significant relationship. This finding could be thought of as the consequence of the study environment focusing on learning of theories, which lacked actual practice. For engineering students, they had few opportunities to take risky business actions in their studies, particularly related to creating a new venture. Thus the students might not comprehend risk-taking in relation to entrepreneurship. In this case, CA and RT could be considered as two independent concepts for the students as their creative mind was not attributed to their RT. In short, in the context of engineering students, CA was not affected by RT.
Finally, the most important theoretical implication is about the adoption of the theoretical model in intention research. This research indicated that no matter if based on the trait theory or behavior theory, the model should include the systematic impact of personal characteristics in their models. In recent years, intention-based models, such as the theory of planned behavior (TPB) (Ajzen, 1991), Shapero’s entrepreneurial event model (Shapero and Sokol, 1982), and Bird’s intention model (Bird, 1988), have been used in the field of entrepreneurship to capture the link between an individual and his or her actions. Particularly, TPB has received more attention from scholars to explain the planned behavior of starting a business, which is best predicted by intentions (Krueger and Carsrud, 1993). These models take into account the impact of factors such as attitudes (Ajzen, 1991), social pressure (Shapero and Sokol, 1982; Ajzen, 1991), perceived behavioral control (Shapero and Sokol, 1982; Ajzen, 1991), and goals of starting a business (Bird, 1988), little concern about personal characteristics is considered in these models. According to Ajzen (2005), personality traits are one of the background factors that influences attitudes of the TPB model.
Practical Implications for Entrepreneurship Education
The findings of the current study bear practical implications for entrepreneurship education that aims at enhancing entrepreneurial intention of engineering students. Researchers have suggested that entrepreneurs are not born; they are made (Gorman et al., 1997; Fiet, 2001a,b). The trait model can be applied to entrepreneurship education (Koh, 1996; Hansemark, 1998) and the psychological attributes have been recognized as important for entrepreneurial activity and economic growth (Singh, 1989; Shaver and Scott, 1991). Although personality characteristics are generally stable and not subject to cursory change, some researchers have provided evidence that some characteristics (specific personal characteristics to entrepreneurship such as NACH, RT, LOC, and CA) can be learned or enhanced through entrepreneurial training and education (McClelland and Winter, 1969; Miron and McClelland, 1979; Timmons et al., 1985; Hood and Young, 1993; Hansemark, 1998). Rotter (1966) recognized that internal LOC beliefs could be learned through training and education experiences. McClelland and Winter (1969) and Miron and McClelland (1979) showed that NACH could be developed, although the training programs were for under-achieving students, minorities, or in under-developed areas. On the other hand, Heckhausen and Krug (1982) found that NACH is learnable. Studying the effect of a formal entrepreneurship program (aimed at developing abilities, knowledge, skills, attitudes, and personal attributes which are important for entrepreneurial activities) on personal characteristics, Hansemark (1998) found that participation in an entrepreneurship education program would lead to a higher level of NACH and increase internal orientation of LOC. Although his study did not explicitly explain which elements of the program caused the results, it clearly indicated that personal characteristics, which are related to entrepreneurial action, could be developed and that this could be done through entrepreneurship education. Further, in Hood and Young’s (1993) research, the importance of “teaching” the personal characteristics such as RT and CA in entrepreneurship education to train successful entrepreneurs was also emphasized.
Working on these premises, the contents of entrepreneurship education should enhance or facilitate the development of entrepreneurial characteristics. Educators that seek ways in which to foster entrepreneurial intention of students need to recognize the holistic influences of these factors. This research provides a sequence in personal development via entrepreneurship education. The findings suggest that internal LOC is the elementary part of the influencing chain. It will facilitate the development of NACH, RT, and CA. Educators can develop LOC focusing on confidence building, commitment, and personal responsibility for the goals they set. The purpose is to help students to exhibit self-confidence in showing their abilities to perform tasks and insist on taking personal responsibilities for their performance. The next step is to develop students’ desire for achievement by practicing challenging tasks or games (from easy to difficult levels) or set different levels of goals of achievement during the entrepreneurship program/course (e.g., short-term and long-term realistic goals). According to Hansemark (1998), when developing internal LOC and NACH, the students must believe in their ability to bring about change and to control their own lives. Those who with more internal locus of control feel more confident and capable of performing entrepreneurial acts, thus facilitating their RT and CA, which will finally lead to the formation of entrepreneurial intention. Based on Hood and Young’s (1993) research, RT can be effectively trained by business experience, class discussion, discussion with business people/leaders, mentoring and seminars, while CA can be trained through class experiential exercises (including creativity games and exercises), discussion groups, case studies, mentoring, simulation, and game theory, which are effective pedagogical vehicles for inspiring students’ innovative thinking, changing their mentalities or learning new ones.
Conclusion and Future Research
This study explores the systematic influence of personal characteristics (NACH, RT, LOC, and CA) on EINT by examining the inter-relationships among the personal characteristics. This is perhaps the first research to reveal how the characteristics, directly and indirectly, influence entrepreneurial intention. Sophisticated statistical methods (logistic regression and SEM path analysis) were used for data analysis. The results showed that RT and CA were the dominant direct predictors of EINT of the engineering students, while LOC and NACH had indirect influences. For example, LOC facilitated the other three characteristics, and NACH facilitated both RT and CA. The findings support the proposition that the four characteristics influence EINT holistically, instead of individually. This confirms the value of personal characteristics in the traditional trait model and provides new insights into understanding their impact on EINT. The contributions of this research include (1) filling in the literature gap on the interrelationship among personal characteristics; (2) filling in the literature gap on the holistic influence of personal characteristics on EINT; (3) removing the doubt on whether personal characteristics are worthwhile to be studied; (4) applying SEM path analysis to examine the interdependent effect of personal characteristic on EINT; and (5) exploring a few new areas for future research on personal characteristics as well as their combination with education and behavior models.
There are a few limitations in this study which may lead to future research. Firstly, this study focuses on engineering students in Hong Kong. Future research can be expanded to students of different disciplines, in different years of study, or from other countries. It can also be conducted in secondary students as researcher conducted (Paço et al., 2011) or extend the research from intention to activities (Raposo and Paço, 2011).
Secondly, this study is based on a cross-section survey. A longitudinal study in the future on entrepreneurship programs/courses could help us measure the effects of education on students’ personal characteristics and the effect of intentions on actual entrepreneurial behaviors. Thirdly, the sample size of this study fulfills the requirement of factor analysis, logistic regression analysis, and achieves the minimum requirement of SEM path analysis. Larger sample size is suggested to get more stable results (Hair et al., 1998). Fourthly, the model was tested separately by two different methods, logistic regression and SEM path analysis, that the general analysis of the entire model was hidden due to the dichotomous nature of the dependent variable. This research can be repeated by modifying the dependent variable into a numerical scale (such as a 5-point Likert scale). Hence, the whole model can be analyzed altogether by SEM path analysis and more statistically convincing results can be obtained.
Finally, we suggest that future models consider the systematic influence of the personal characteristics on EINT or on the intention models. Background factors and contextual factors may also be included to have a complete model. Since personal characteristics can be developed or enhanced through education, such integration is particularly important to develop an entrepreneurship education model that explains how education fosters the entrepreneurial intention of students through developing their entrepreneurial characteristics as well as acquiring relevant knowledge and skills. With its strong flux of renewed interest, entrepreneurial characteristics are set to be an even more important area for academic and professional research in the future rather than being a label of convenience.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.
Ethics Statement
Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author Contributions
HS: writing. WN: conceptualization and writing. P-LT: writing – review and editing. CL: data collection and analysis.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Abdel-Halim, A. (1980). Effect of person-job compatibility on managerial reactions to role ambiguity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 26, 193–211. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(80)90054-9
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Amabile, T., and Hennessey, B. (1992). “The motivation for creativity in children,” in Achievement and Motivation: A Social-Developmental Perspective, eds A. Boggiano and T. Pittman (New York: Cambridge University Press), 54–74.
Anderson, J., and Gerbing, D. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411
Andersson, L., and Bateman, T. (1997). Cynicism in the workplace: some causes and effects. J. Organ. Behav. 18, 449–469. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1379(199709)18:5<449::aid-job808>3.0.co;2-o
Armstrong, J., and Overton, T. (1977). Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys. J. Mark. Res. 14, 396–402. doi: 10.1177/002224377701400320
Atkinson, J. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol. Rev. 64, 359–372. doi: 10.1037/h0043445
Aulakh, P., and Geneturk, E. (2000). International principal-agent relationships-control, governance and performance. Ind. Mark. Manage. 29, 521–538.
Autio, E., Keeley, R., Klofsten, M., and Ulfstedt, T. (1997). Entrepreneurial intent among students: testing and intent model in Asia, Scandinavia, and USA. Paper Presented at the 17th Babson College, Kauffman Foundation Enterpreneurial Research Coference. Babson Park, MA: Babson College, 145–160.
Autio, E., Keeley, R., Klofsten, M., Parker, G., and Hay, M. (2001). Entrepreneurial intent among students in Scandinavia and in the USA. Enterp. Innov. Manage. Stud. 2, 145–160. doi: 10.1080/14632440110094632
Behave, M. (1994). A process model of entrepreneurial venture creation. J. Bus. Ventur. 9, 223–242. doi: 10.1016/0883-9026(94)90031-0
Bird, B. (1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: the case for intention. Acad. Manage. Rev. 13, 442–453. doi: 10.5465/amr.1988.4306970
Bonnett, C., and Furnham, A. (1991). Who wants to be an entrepreneur? a study of adolescents interested in a young enterprise scheme. J. Econ. Psychol. 12, 465–478. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(91)90027-q
Brockhaus, R. (1980). Risk taking propensity of entrepreneurs. Acad. Manage. J. 23, 509–520. doi: 10.5465/255515
Brush, C., Duhaime, I., Gartner, W., Stewart, A., Katz, J., and Hitt, M. E. (2003). Doctoral education in the field of entrepreneurship. J. Manag. 29, 309–331. doi: 10.1002/biot.201800002
Caird, S. (1991). Testing enterprising tendency in occupational groups. Br. J. Manage. 2, 177–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.1991.tb00025.x
Carland, J., Carland, J., and Pearce, J. (1995). Risk taking propensity among entrepreneurs, small business owners, and managers. J. Bus. Entrep. 7, 15–23.
Carr, J., and Sequeira, J. (2007). Prior family business exposure as intergenerational influence and entrepreneurial intent: a theory of planned behavior approach. J. Bus. Res. 60, 1090–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.12.016
Chen, P., Greene, P., and Crick, A. (1998). Does entrepreneurial self efficacy distinguish entrepreneurs from managers? J. Bus. Ventur. 13, 295–316. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9026(97)00029-3
Cherrington, D. (1991). “Need theories of motivation,” in Motivation and Work Behavior, eds R. Steers and L. Porter (New York: McGraw-Hill).
Collins, C., Hanges, P., and Locke, E. (2004). The relationship of achievement motivation to entrepreneurial behavior: a meta-analysis. Hum. Perform. 17, 95–117. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup1701_5
Cromie, S., and O’Donoghue, J. (1992). Assessing entrepreneurial inclinations. Int. Small Bus. J. 10, 66–72.
Cromie, S., Callaghan, I., and Jansen, M. (1992). The entrepreneurial tendencies of mangers: a research note. Br. J. Manage. 3, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.1992.tb00031.x
Davidsson, P. (1995). Culture, structure and regional levels of entrepreneurship. Entrep. Reg. Dev. 7, 41–62. doi: 10.1080/08985629500000003
Dean, M., Shook, C., and Payne, G. (2007). The past, present, and future of entrepreneurship research: data analytic trends and training. Entrep. Theory Pract. 31, 601–618. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2007.00190.x
Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York: Springer.
Drucker, P. (2007). Innovation and Entrepreneurship: Practice and Principles. London: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Entrialgo, M., Fernandez, E., and Vazquez, C. (2000). Characters of managers as determinants of entrepreneurial orientation: some Spanish evidence. Enterp. Creat. Manag. Stud. 1, 187–205. doi: 10.1080/14632440050119596
Fayolle, A., Gailly, B., and Lassas-Clerc, N. (2006a). Assessing the impact of entrepreneurship education programs: a new methodology. J. Eur. Ind. Train. 30, 701–720. doi: 10.1108/03090590610715022
Fayolle, A., Gailly, B., and Lassas-Clerc, N. (2006b). Effect and counter-effect of entrepreneurship education and social context on students’ intentions. Estudios De Econ. Aplicada 24, 509–523.
Fiet, J. O. (2001a). The theoretical side of teaching entrepreneurship. J. Bus. Ventur. 16, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/S0883-9026(99)00041-5
Fiet, J. O. (2001b). The pedagogical side of entrepreneurship theory. J. Bus. Ventur. 16, 101–117. doi: 10.1016/S0883-9026(99)00042-7
Flynn, B., Schroeder, R., and Sakakibara, S. (1994). A framework for quality management research and an associated measurement instrument. J. Oper. Manag. 11, 339–366. doi: 10.1016/s0272-6963(97)90004-8
Frank, H., Korunka, C., Lueger, M., and Mugler, J. (2005). Entrepreneurial orientation and education in Austrian secondary schools: status quo and recommendations. J. Small Bus. Enterp. Dev. 12, 259–273. doi: 10.1108/14626000510594647
Frank, H., Lueger, M., and Korunka, C. (2007). The significance of personality in business start-up intentions, start-up realization and business success. Entrep. Reg. Dev. 19, 227–251. doi: 10.1080/08985620701218387
Gardner, D., Cummings, L., Dunham, R., and Pierce, J. (1998). Single-item versus multiple item measurement scales: an empirical comparison. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 58, 898–915. doi: 10.1177/0013164498058006003
Gartner, W. (1988). Who is an entrepreneur? Is the wrong question. Am. J. Small Bus. 20, 11–32. doi: 10.1177/104225878801200401
Gartner, W. (1990). What are we talking about when we talk about entrepreneurship? J. Bus. Ventur. 5, 15–28.
Goldsmith, R. (1994). “Creativity style and personality theory,” in Adaptors and Innovators: Styles of Creativity and Problem Solving, ed. M. Kirton (London: Routledge), 34–50.
Gorman, G., Hanlon, D., and King, W. (1997). Some research perspectives on entrepreneurship education, enterprise education and education for small business management: a ten-year literature review. Int. Small Bus. J. 15, 59–77.
Grant, J. (1996). The proactive personality scale as a predictor of entrepreneurial intentions. Int. Small Bus. Manag. 34, 42–49.
Green, R., David, R., and Dent, M. (1996). The Russian entrepreneur: a study of personalit characters. Int. J. Entrep. Behav. Res. 2, 49–58.
Gurol, Y., and Atsan, N. (2006). Entrepreneurial characters amongst university students: some insights for entrepreneurship education and training in Turkey. Educ. Train. 48, 25–38. doi: 10.1108/00400910610645716
Hair, J., Black, B., Babin, B., Anderson, R., and Tatham, R. (2006). Multivariate Data Analysis, 6 Edn. Upper Saddle River, N.J: Prentice-Hall.
Hair, J., Tatham, R., Anderson, R., and William, B. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis, 5 Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hansemark, O. (1998). The effects of an entrepreneurship program on need for achievement. Int. J. Entrep. Behav. 14, 28–50. doi: 10.1108/13552559810203957
Heckhausen, H., and Krug, S. (1982). “Motive modification,” in Motivation and Society, ed. S. Stewart (San Franscisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 274–318.
Hillson, D., and Murray-Webster, R. (2007). Understanding and Managing Risk Attitude, 2nd Edn. Aldershot: Burlington.
Hisrich, R., Langan-Fox, J., and Grant, S. (2007). Entrepreneurship research and practice: a call to action for psychology. Am. Psychol. 62, 575–589. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.62.6.575
Ho, T., and Koh, H. (1992). Differences in psychological characters between entrepreneurially inclined and non-entrepreneurially inclined accounting graduates in Singapore. Entrep. Creat. Change Inte. J. 1, 243–354.
Hood, J., and Young, J. (1993). Entrepreneurship’s requisite areas of development: a survey of top executives in successful entrepreneurial firms. J. Bus. Ventur. 8, 115–135. doi: 10.1016/0883-9026(93)90015-w
Howell, J., and Higgins, C. (1990). Champions of technological innovation. Adm. Sci. Q. 35, 317–341.
İrengün, O., and Arıkboğa, Ş. (2015). The effect of personality traits on social entrepreneurship intentions: a field research. Procedia – Soc. Behav. Sci. 195, 1186–1195. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.06.172
Iverson, R., and Maguire, C. (2000). The relationship between job and life satisfaction: evidence from a remote mining community. Hum. Relat. 53, 807–839. doi: 10.1177/0018726700536003
Jackson, D. (1994). Jackson Personality Index, Sigma Assessments. Port Huron, MI: SIGMA Assessment Systems.
Johnson, J. (2003). “Toward a better understanding of the relationship between personality and individual job performance,” in Personality and Work Reconsidering the Role of Personality in Organizations, ed. M. R. Barrick (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 83–120.
Judge, T., Bono, J., Ilies, R., and Gerhardt, M. (2002). Personality and leadership: a qualitative and quantitative review. J. Appl. Psychol. 87, 765–780. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765
Killgore, W. (1999). The visual analogue mood scale: can a single-item scale accurately classify depressive mood state? Psychol. Rep. 85, 1238–1243. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1999.85.3f.1238
King, A. (1985). Self-analysis and assessment of entrepreneurial potential. Simul. Games 16, 399–416. doi: 10.1177/104687818501600402
Kline, R. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: The Guildford press.
Koh, H. (1996). Testing hypotheses of entrepreneurial characters: a study of Hong Kong MBA students. J. Manage. Psychol. 11, 12–25. doi: 10.1108/02683949610113566
Kolvereid, L. (1996). Prediction of employment status choice intentions. Entrep. Theory Pract. Fall 21, 47–57.
Kristiansen, S., and Indarti, N. (2004). Entrepreneurial intention among Indonesian and Norwegian students. J. Enterp. Culture 12, 55–78. doi: 10.1142/s021849580400004x
Krueger, N., and Brazeal, D. (1994). Entrepreneurial potential and potential entrepreneurs. Entrep. Theory Pract. 18, 91–104. doi: 10.1177/104225879401800307
Krueger, N., and Carsrud, A. (1993). Entrepreneurial intentions: applying the theory of planned behavior. Entrep. Reg. Dev. 5, 315–330. doi: 10.1080/08985629300000020
Krueger, N., Reilly, M., and Carsrud, A. (2000). Competing models of entrepreneurial intentions. J. Bus. Ventur. 15, 411–432. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217971
Langan-Fox, J., and Roth, S. (1995). Achievement motivation and female entrepreneurs. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 68, 209–218. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1995.tb00582.x
Loh, X. M., Lee, V. H., Tan, G. W. H., Hew, J. J., and Ooi, K. B. (2019). Towards a cashless society: the imminent role of wearable technology. J. Comput. Inf. Syst. doi: 10.1080/08874417.2019.1688733
Lüthje, C., and Franke, N. (2003). The ‘making’ of an entrepreneur: testing a model of entrepreneurial intent among engineering students at MIT. R D Manag. 33, 135–147. doi: 10.1111/1467-9310.00288
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., and Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 4, 84–99. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.4.1.84
McClelland, D. (1987). Characteristics of successful entrepreneurs. J. Creat. Behav. 21, 219–233. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1987.tb00479.x
McClelland, D., and Winter, D. (1969). Motivating Economic Achievement. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Merenda, P. F. (1997). A Guide to the proper use of factor analysis in the conduct and reporting of research: pitfalls to avoid. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 30, 156–164. doi: 10.1080/07481756.1997.12068936
Miller, D., Kets de Vries, M., and Toulouse, J. (1982). Top executive locus of control and its relationship to strategy, environment and structure. Acad. Manage. J. 25, 237–253. doi: 10.5465/255988
Miron, D., and McClelland, D. (1979). The impact of achievement motivation training on small business performance. Calif. Manage. Rev. 21, 13–28. doi: 10.2307/41164830
Mueller, S., and Thomas, A. (2000). Culture and entrepreneurial potential: a nine country study of locus of control and innovativeness. J. Bus. Ventur. 16, 51–75.
Nagy, M. (2002). Using a single-item approach to measure facet job satisfaction. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 75, 77–86. doi: 10.1348/096317902167658
Paço, A., Ferreira, J., Raposo, M., Rodrigues, Gouveia, R., and Dinis, A. (2011). Entrepreneurial intention among secondary students: findings from portugal. Int. J. Entrep. Small Bus. 13, 92–106.
Palich, L. E., and Bagby, D. (1995). Using cognitive theory to explain entrepreneurial risk-taking: challenging conventional wisdom. J. Bus. Ventur. 10, 425–438. doi: 10.1016/0883-9026(95)00082-j
Peterman, N., and Kennedy, J. (2003). Enterprise education: influencing students’ perceptions of entrepreneurship. Entrep. Theory Pract. Winter 28, 129–144. doi: 10.1046/j.1540-6520.2003.00035.x
Petrakis, P. (2007). The effects of risk and time on entrepreneurship. Int. Entrep. Manag. 3, 277–291. doi: 10.1007/s11365-006-0026-8
Podsakoff, P., and Organ, D. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: problems and prospects. J. Manag. 12, 531–544. doi: 10.1177/014920638601200408
Podsakoff, P., MacKenzie, S., Lee, J., and Podsakoff, N. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
Pomeroy, I., Clark, C., and Philip, I. (2001). The effectiveness of very short scales for depression screening in elderly medical patients. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 16, 321–326. doi: 10.1002/gps.344
Raab, G., Stedham, Y., and Neuner, M. (2005). Entrepreneurial potential: an exploratory study of business students in the U.S. and Germany. J. Bus. Manag. 11, 71–88.
Raposo, M., and Paço, A. (2011). Entrepreneurship education: relationship between education and entrepreneurial activity. Psicothema 23, 453–457.
Rauch, A., and Frese, M. (2000). “Psychological approaches to entrepreneurial success: a general model and an overview of findings,” in International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, eds C. Cooper and I. Robertson (New York: John Wiley and Sons), 101–141.
Rauch, A., and Frese, M. (2007a). “Born to be a entrepreneur? Revisiting the personality approach to entrepreneurship,” in Psychology Entrepreneurship, eds J. Baum, M. Frese, and R. Baron (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 41–65.
Rauch, A., and Frese, M. (2007b). Let’s put the person back into entrepreneurship research: SA meta-analysis on the relationship between business owners’ personality and business creation and success. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 16, 353–385. doi: 10.1080/13594320701595438
Robins, R., Hendin, H., and Trzesniewski, K. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem: construct validation of a single-item measure and the rosenberg self-esteem scale. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 151–161. doi: 10.1177/0146167201272002
Robinson, P., Huefner, J., and Hunt, H. (1991a). Entrepreneurial research on student subjects does not generalize to real world entrepreneurs. J. Small Bus. Manag. 12, 42–50.
Robinson, P., Stimpson, D., Huefner, J., and Hunt, H. (1991b). An attitude approach to the prediction of entrepreneurship. Entrep. Theory Pract. 15, 13–31.
Rotter, J. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monog. Gen. Appl. 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976
Sagie, A., and Elizur, D. (1999). Achievement motive and entrepreneurial orientation: a structural analysis. J. Organ. Behav. 20, 375–387. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1379(199905)20:3<375::aid-job884>3.0.co;2-y
Schumpeter, J. A. (2000). “Entrepreneurship as innovation,” in Entrepreneurship: The Social Science View, ed. R. Swedberg (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 51–75.
Shapero, A., and Sokol, L. (1982). ““Social dimensions of entrepreneurship”,” in The Encyclopedia of Entrepreneurship, ed. C. E. Kent (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall), 72–89.
Shaver, K., and Scott, L. (1991). Person, process, choice: the psychology of new venture creation. Entrep. Theory Pract. 16, 23–45.
Singh, S. (1989). Projective and psychometric correlates of managerial success. Br. J. Psychol. 34, 28–36.
Spector, P. (1982). Behavior in organizations as a function of employee’s locus of control. Psychol. Bull. 91, 482–497. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.3.482
Spector, P. (1987). Method variance as an artifact in self-reported affect and perceptions at work: Myth or significant problem? J. Appl. Psychol. 72, 438–443. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.72.3.438
Stewart, J. W. (1996). Psychological Correlates of Entrepreneurship. London: Garland publishing Inc.
Stewart, W. J., Watson, W., Carland, J., and Carland, J. (1999). A proclivity for entrepreneurship: a comparison of entrepreneurs, small business owners, and corporate managers. J. Bus. Ventur. 14, 189–214.
Stewart, W., and Roth, P. (2001). Risk propensity differences between entrepreneurs and managers: a meta-analytic review. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 145–153. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.145
Stewart, W., and Roth, P. (2004). Data-quality affects meta-analytic conclusions: a response to Miner and Raju concerning entrepreneurial risk propensity. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 14–21. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.1.14
Sun, H., Lo, C. T., Liang, B., and Wong, Y. L. B. (2017). The impact of entrepreneurial education on entrepreneurial intention of engineering students in Hong Kong. Manag. Dec. 55, 1371–1393. doi: 10.1108/md-06-2016-0392
Tan, G. W. H., Lee, V. H., Hew, J. J., Ooi, K. B., and Wong, L. W. (2018). The interactive mobile social media advertising: an imminent approach to advertise tourism products and services? Telemat. Inform. 35, 2270–2288. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2018.09.005
Tett, R., Steele, J., and Beaurgard, R. (2003). Broad and narrow measures on both sides of the personality-job performance relationship. J. Organ. Behav. 24, 335–356. doi: 10.1002/job.191
Timmons, J., Smollen, L., and Dingee, A. (1985). New Venture Creation, 2nd Edn. Homewood, IL: Richard D. Irwin.
Tkachev, A., and Kolvereid, L. (1999). Self-employment intentions among russian students. Entrep. Reg. Dev. 11, 269–280. doi: 10.1080/089856299283209
Utsch, A., and Rauch, A. (2000). Innovativeness and initiative as mediators between achievement orientation and venture performance. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 9, 45–62. doi: 10.1080/135943200398058
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A., and Hudy, M. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: how good are single-items measures? J. Appl. Psychol. 82, 247–252. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.247
Wanous, J., and Hudy, M. J. (2001). Single-item reliability: a replication and extension. Organ. Res. Methods. 4, 361–375. doi: 10.1177/109442810144003
Wilson, F., Kickul, J., and Marlino, D. (2007). Gender, entrepreneurial self-efficacy, and entrepreneurial career intentions: implications for entrepreneurship education. Entrep. Theory Pract. 31, 387–406. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2007.00179.x
Appendix | The Questionnaire
Section 1: Your personal information (tick or circle only one choice).
a. Age: <25:___, 25–30:___, 31–35:___, >35:___.
b. Gender: Male: ___, Female: ___.
c. Education: High diploma: ___, Bachelor: ___, Master: ___, PhD: ___.
d. Years of working experiences: <5:___, 5–10:___, >10:___.
e. Your estimated average performance for all education: A:___, B:___, C:___.
f. Do you have the intention of establishing your own business in the future: Yes:___, No:___.
Section 2: Personal characteristics.
Strongly disagree = 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 = Strongly agree
Keywords: personal characteristics, entrepreneurial intention, entrepreneurship education, need for achievement, risk-taking propensity, locus of control, creativity
Citation: Sun H, Ni W, Teh P-L and Lo C (2020) The Systematic Impact of Personal Characteristics on Entrepreneurial Intentions of Engineering Students. Front. Psychol. 11:1072. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01072
Received: 07 March 2020; Accepted: 27 April 2020;
Published: 03 June 2020.
Edited by:
Chih-Hung Yuan, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, ChinaReviewed by:
Garry Tan, Taylor’s University, MalaysiaYu Rongjian, Zhejiang Gongshang University, China
Arminda Paço, University of Beira Interior, Portugal
Copyright © 2020 Sun, Ni, Teh and Lo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Wenbin Ni, neesebin@163.com; nwb@zufe.edu.cn