Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 May 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Teacher Emotions Matter: Nature, Antecedents, and Effects View all 17 articles

Teachers’ Emotional Exhaustion: Associations With Their Typical Use of and Implicit Attitudes Toward Emotion Regulation Strategies

  • Department of Education, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands

Teaching is an emotionally challenging profession, sometimes resulting in high levels of teacher stress, burnout, and attrition. It has often been claimed that certain emotion regulation strategies can lower teachers’ feelings of burnout. The use of cognitive reappraisal (i.e., cognitively changing the emotional impact of a situation) has generally been associated with positive outcomes, whereas using expressive suppression (i.e., inhibiting emotional responses) usually has negative consequences. The present study investigated the association between teachers’ typical use of these two emotion regulation strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression) and their feelings of emotional exhaustion. Because there is evidence that regulating emotions could involve higher costs when regulation goes against individual preferences, we also explored the potentially moderating effect of teachers’ implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression on the association between typical use of emotion regulation strategies and teachers’ emotional exhaustion with an Implicit Association Test (IAT). We included the interpersonal teacher–student relationship (in terms of teacher agency and communion), teacher experience, and teacher gender as covariates in our analyses. Participants were 94 teachers in secondary education, vocational education, and teacher training for secondary education. Replicating findings from prior studies, hierarchical regression analyses showed that typical use of cognitive reappraisal, but not expressive suppression, was significantly related to lower levels of teachers’ emotional exhaustion. Teachers’ implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression moderated the relationship between the use of emotion regulation strategies and emotional exhaustion, but only in a subsample with more experienced teachers. Teachers who showed more interpersonal agency in class and had more years of teaching experience reported lower levels of emotional exhaustion. Interpersonal communion and gender were not directly associated with feelings of exhaustion. Implications for teacher training and suggestions for future research are discussed.

Introduction

Teaching is a challenging profession, resulting in high levels of burnout among teachers (EU-OSHA, 2013). Next to administrative workload (Farber, 1984; Van Droogenbroeck et al., 2014), a problematic teacher–student relationship is a well-known contributor to negative emotions, decreased work engagement, and increased emotional exhaustion (Spilt et al., 2011; Klassen et al., 2012; Aldrup et al., 2018). It has been suggested that the use of appropriate emotion regulation strategies might help to prevent feelings of burnout (Cross and Hong, 2012; Durr et al., 2014; Keller et al., 2014) and feelings of emotional exhaustion in particular (Tsouloupas et al., 2010). Most previous studies found that, in general, using cognitive reappraisal strategies (i.e., cognitively changing the emotional impact of a situation) yields more positive emotions, better interpersonal functioning, and higher levels of well-being, whereas using expressive suppression (i.e., inhibiting emotional responses) might result in more negative outcomes (Gross and John, 2003; Moore et al., 2008; Haga et al., 2009; Webb et al., 2012). However, regulating emotions by means of emotion regulation strategies is effortful and could involve higher costs when regulation goes against an individual’s implicit preference for emotion regulation versus emotion expression. For example, teachers often feel there are certain emotional display rules they need to adhere to (e.g., not expressing your anger toward student misbehavior). The extent to which an individual’s implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) is aligned with those display rules might affect the costs and benefits associated with the use of explicit emotion regulation strategies (Chang and Davis, 2009; Frenzel, 2014).

Little is known about the interplay of teachers’ typical use of and their implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation strategies. Studying this interplay could help to further specify the ways in which we can support teachers in dealing with unpleasant emotions, which might ultimately lead to lower levels of teacher stress and emotional exhaustion. Therefore, the present study investigated not only the association between teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their feelings of emotional exhaustion but also the potential moderating role of their implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression. Moreover, other factors that have been related to teacher’s emotional exhaustion were taken into account, namely, the quality of the teacher–student relationship (Chang, 2009; Spilt et al., 2011), years of teaching experience (Grandey, 2000; Harmsen et al., 2018), and teacher gender (Johnson and Spector, 2007; Olson et al., 2019).

Teacher burnout has received a considerable amount of attention, both from policy makers as well as in educational research. Burnout can be defined as “a prolonged response to chronic emotional and interpersonal stressors on the job, and is defined by the three dimensions of emotional exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy” (Maslach et al., 2001, p. 397). Emotional exhaustion is often considered to be the most central aspect of burnout and has been the focus of attention in many studies in the educational context (Chang, 2009; Tsouloupas et al., 2010; Goetz et al., 2015; Arens and Morin, 2016; Taxer et al., 2019). Moreover, it has been suggested that the use of effortful emotion regulation strategies might in particular affect employees’ feelings of emotional exhaustion because of the emotional dissonance and emotional labor they experience when applying regulation strategies (Johnson and Spector, 2007; Keller et al., 2014).

Already in the 1980s, studies indicated high levels of stress experienced by teachers, in some cases leading to burnout (Farber, 1984; Kyriacou, 1987). Since the introduction of the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI; Maslach et al., 1996), research on burnout has rapidly increased. Although such research efforts have improved our understanding of the phenomenon, recent papers indicate that teacher burnout is still frequent and has long-lasting consequences for both teachers and their students, such as high turnover and dropout rates among teachers and lower achievement and school satisfaction among students (Arens and Morin, 2016; Veldman et al., 2016; Lee, 2019). Daily emotions have often been described as the “building blocks” of burnout (Hollenstein, 2015), and the regulation of emotions and personal preferences regarding emotion regulation have been proposed to play an important role in personal well-being and social functioning (Sutton and Wheatley, 2003; Jiang et al., 2016).

Two of the most well-known emotion regulation strategies are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression (Gross and John, 2003; Moore et al., 2008). Cognitive reappraisal is an emotion regulation strategy aiming at explicitly and cognitively changing ones thoughts and behavior before an emotion has fully developed (also referred to as antecedent-focused or deep-acting strategies; Lazarus and Alfert, 1964; Gross and John, 2003; Lee et al., 2016). In the case of teaching, a teacher might, for example, choose to label the situation in which a student talks to another student during plenary instruction as a sign of interest rather than disengagement and thus experience positive instead of negative emotions. Cognitive reappraisal has been found to be the emotion regulation strategy with the best well-being outcomes for teachers and has therefore been considered effective (e.g., Gross and John, 2003; Barber et al., 2011; Becker et al., 2015; Jiang et al., 2016; Lavy and Eshet, 2018). However, some studies in other contexts have also indicated potential negative effects of cognitive reappraisal. For example, there is some evidence that cognitive reappraisal only has positive effects when used in uncontrollable situations (i.e., where you can only control yourself, not the environment), but negative effects in situations where participants reported that they could have influenced the stressful situation (Troy et al., 2013). Haines et al. (2016) also found evidence for what has been called the strategy-situation-fit hypothesis and the importance of regulatory flexibility (Bonanno and Burton, 2013) using an experience sampling design. They found that participants who were using cognitive reappraisal only in uncontrollable situations scored higher on well-being (Haines et al., 2016). This suggests that during teaching, which could be characterized as a (mostly) controllable situation for teachers, the effects of using cognitive reappraisal strategies might be limited or even negative.

Expressive suppression, on the other hand, aims at regulating the expression of an emotion that is already experienced (also referred to as response-focused or surface-acting strategies; Gross, 1998; Gross and John, 2003; Lee et al., 2016). Expressive suppression frequently occurs especially in situations where others are present and where the goal is to avoid conflict (English et al., 2017). For example, teachers may choose to suppress their anger and stay friendly in case of disrupting student behavior to avoid escalation or discussion with students. Expressive suppression has often been related to negative outcomes for teachers such as an increase in stress-related symptoms and emotional exhaustion (Butler et al., 2003; Moore et al., 2008; Haga et al., 2009; Barber et al., 2011; Jiang et al., 2016). However, there are studies in other contexts that showed positive effects of expressive suppression strategies, for example, in the context of combatting food cravings (Siep et al., 2012). Moreover, Richardson (2017) showed with a daily-diary study that using expressive suppression only had negative effects on affective well-being on high-stress days, but not on low-stress days where emotion regulation might be less costly. Also, in the teaching context, it has been suggested that expressive suppression of negative emotions may be functional because it is in line with common standards that guide appropriate expression of emotions in the classroom (i.e., display rules; Hagenauer and Volet, 2014; Taxer and Frenzel, 2015).

In sum, both cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression may have benefits as well as costs. Negative effects are often explained by the effort required from the teacher to exert emotion regulation strategies in general, which potentially impairs not only cognitive performance (Richards and Gross, 1999) and achievement (Low et al., 2017) but also peer relationships (Tsai et al., 2017) and social interactions (Butler et al., 2003). In other words, teachers may pay a price for using effortful emotion regulation strategies. Although regulating your emotions may help on the short term, the effort teachers put into it may have negative consequences in the long run. In the teaching situation, the cognitive resources absorbed by teachers’ use of effortful emotion regulation strategies might result in less available resources for classroom management and the actual teaching itself. Moreover, when teachers feel compelled to use emotion regulation strategies imposed by school leaders, parents, or society, this might have negative consequences for their well-being (Hagenauer and Volet, 2014; Taxer and Frenzel, 2015). Therefore, the use of emotion regulation strategies has also been referred to as emotional labor (Hochschild, 1983).

In line with Frenzel’s theoretical model on teacher emotions (Frenzel, 2014), it could be argued that teachers may view emotional labor as a positive part of their job. Implicit beliefs, habits, and cultural norms may help teachers to internalize an implicit positive attitude toward the use of emotion regulation strategies (Sutton and Harper, 2009; Sutton et al., 2009). Having such an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) has been proposed to be beneficial for the effort involved in the actual use of emotion regulation strategies and well-being outcomes (Koole and Jostmann, 2004; Mauss et al., 2006). An implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation might lower the cognitive and emotional costs of the use of effortful emotion regulation strategies (Mauss et al., 2007). Mauss et al. (2006) developed an Implicit Association Test (IAT) to examine implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) in addition to self-reported use of emotion regulation strategies. Participants who had an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) reported less anger and fewer negative thoughts after an anger provocation task that required emotion regulation. In a follow-up study, Hopp et al. (2011) found support for their hypothesis that having an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation might only increase psychological health when adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, are used, but not when using expressive suppression of emotions.

Thus, while using cognitive reappraisal strategies might help teachers to effectively deal with their emotions and diminish their feelings of emotional exhaustion, these strategies may also be experienced as effortful, depending on whether teachers do or do not have an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression). Similarly, when using expressive suppression, an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation may be beneficial because it may help teachers to suppress their emotions more easily, and the cognitive and emotional costs of suppression might therefore be lower.

The present study builds on the existing literature by investigating the association between teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies and emotional exhaustion and by exploring whether teachers’ implicit attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) had a moderating effect on this association using an IAT. Two main research questions and hypotheses guided our investigation:

1. To what extent is teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies associated with their level of emotional exhaustion?

In line with previous research, we expected that teachers’ use of cognitive reappraisal strategies and their emotional exhaustion level would be negatively associated (i.e., more use of cognitive reappraisal is associated with lower levels of emotional exhaustion), while using expressive suppression would be positively associated with emotional exhaustion levels (i.e., more expressive suppression is associated with higher levels of emotional exhaustion).

2. To what extent is teachers’ implicit attitude toward emotion regulation moderating the effect of their typical use of emotion regulation strategies on emotional exhaustion?

We expected that teachers who implicitly preferred emotion regulation above expressing emotions would experience lower costs of using effortful emotion regulation strategies, and thus the negative association between using cognitive reappraisal and feelings of emotional exhaustion would be accelerated (i.e., more negative), while the positive association between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion would be attenuated (i.e., less positive).

In the final step of our analyses, we included contextual and personal factors that have been associated with teacher burnout in previous studies, namely, the quality of the teacher–student relationship, teaching experience, and gender. We wanted to check whether our findings for the first two research questions would change when these variables were entered to the regression model. The teacher–student relationship has often been postulated as an important factor for teacher emotions and well-being (Hargreaves, 2000; Chang, 2009, 2013; Spilt et al., 2011; Van Droogenbroeck et al., 2014). Especially teachers who are both warm (high in communion) and demanding (high in agency) might be less vulnerable for emotional exhaustion (Wubbels et al., 2006; Bondy et al., 2013), but even teachers who have overall favorable and positive relationships with their students may sometimes report negative emotions and burnout symptoms (Donker et al., 2018). Research suggests that also teaching experience needs to be taken into account to understand teachers’ feelings of emotional exhaustion (Grandey, 2000; Troy et al., 2013). It has been found that beginning teachers may experience more intense emotions (Chang, 2009), which in turn may lead to stronger feelings of emotional exhaustion (Harmsen et al., 2018) and attrition of beginning teachers (Buchanan et al., 2013). Furthermore, gender differences have been found both in the use of emotion regulation strategies as well as in the consequences of engaging in emotion regulation. Women are more likely to report the use of emotion regulation strategies in general (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011), but men engage more often in emotion suppression in particular (Grandey, 2000; Spaapen et al., 2014). This could be problematic as using emotion expression has been associated with emotional exhaustion (Moore et al., 2008; Jiang et al., 2016). However, Johnson and Spector (2007) found that females experienced more negative consequences of expressive suppression than men. Although not all studies found gender differences in self-reported cognitive reappraisal (see, for example, Zlomke and Hahn, 2010), there is some evidence for neural differences between men and women engaging in emotion regulation that are potentially related to their implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation (McRae et al., 2008). Given these findings, we also included teacher gender in the present study. Our third research question thus was:

3. To what extent do the quality of the teacher–student relationship, teaching experience, and gender explain additional variance in teachers’ emotional exhaustion?

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were 94 teachers aged from 20 to 64 years (M = 35.26, SD = 12.53), and 52.1% of the teachers had less than 5 years of teaching experience. The gender distribution was about equal (55.3% female), and 84% of the teachers was right-handed. The sample consisted of teachers from secondary education (N = 49), vocational education (N = 36), and a teacher training program for secondary education (N = 9). As expected, the groups differed in age [F(2,91) = 4.80, p = 0.010] and teaching experience [χ2(2) = 24.58, p < 0.001; Table 1] with vocational education teachers being older than secondary education teachers, and there was a higher percentage of teachers with less than 5 years of teaching experience in the group of secondary education teachers and student teachers. Because teaching experience and age were highly correlated (r = 0.75), we included only teaching experience in our analyses. There were no significant differences between the groups in terms of gender [χ2(2) = 3.10, p = 0.212]. Per teacher, one class of students provided ratings of the teacher–student relationship, which were averaged per teacher. There were on average 19 students per class who gave ratings (SD = 6). Most students were aged between 14 and 20.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Teacher characteristics for the full sample and the three separate groups.

Design and Procedure

The design of the study got ethical approval before data were collected (FETC16-110). Participating teachers and students were informed a priori about the research by means of an information letter and were asked to sign an informed consent form. Teachers first completed a computer task to measure their implicit attitude toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression [i.e., the Emotion Regulation-IAT (ER-IAT)] and subsequently completed a digital questionnaire measuring their typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their emotional exhaustion to prevent possible effects of the questionnaires on the ER-IAT. Completing the ER-IAT and questionnaires took a maximum of 30 min per teacher. Students filled in the questionnaire on their perception of the teacher–student relationship during one of their lessons (approximately 5 min).

Measures

Emotional Exhaustion

Emotional exhaustion was measured using a Dutch translation of the MBI (Maslach et al., 1996) with a specific focus on the teaching context [i.e., the Utrecht Burnout Scale for Teachers (UBOS-L); Schaufeli and Van Dierendonck, 2000]. The UBOS-L was used to measure all three aspects of teachers’ burnout: emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and personal accomplishment. For this study, we only used data from the emotional exhaustion scale as this is often considered to be the most central aspect of burnout (Chang, 2009; Goetz et al., 2015; Arens and Morin, 2016). Emotional exhaustion was measured with eight items, measuring the extent to which a teacher feels “empty” or exhausted due to work-related efforts (e.g., “I feel like I am at the end of my rope”). Items were answered on a seven-point Likert scale bounded by 0 “never” and 6 “daily.” Internal consistency of this scale in the present study was good (α = 0.88).

Emotion Regulation Strategies

Teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies was measured using the Dutch version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross and John, 2003; Koole and Jostmann, 2004). The ERQ covers the two most well-known emotion regulation strategies cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Cognitive reappraisal was measured with six items, such as “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in.” Expressive suppression was measured with four items, for example, “I keep my emotions to myself.” All items were measured on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “strongly disagree” to 7 “strongly agree.” In the current study, internal consistency was good for the cognitive reappraisal (α = 0.84) and sufficient for the expressive suppression scale (α = 0.73; cf. Koole and Jostmann, 2004).

Implicit Attitude Toward Emotion Regulation

Implicit Association Tests were introduced by Greenwald et al. (1998) and have been widely used since then in the field of social psychology and neighboring fields. IATs were developed to measure the strength of automatic associations between categories and aim to test people’s relative preference for one construct over the other. The underlying assumption is that it will take participants longer to categorize words when the combined constructs are not in line with their implicit attitudes. Mauss et al. (2006) developed the ER-IAT to measure individuals’ implicit positive versus negative evaluation of emotion regulation versus emotion expression. The ER-IAT was translated into Dutch for the current study (see Supplementary Material A for a description of the translation process).

The ER-IAT is a computer-based reaction time task in which participants need to assign target-concept words such as “controlled” to either the “emotion regulation” or “emotion expression” category and attribute words such as “pleasant” to the “positive” or “negative” category. An overview of the words that we used in the present study is included in Supplementary Material A. The words appear one by one in the middle of the screen, and participants assign the word to the category on the top left by pressing a key on the left side of the keyboard (i.e., “d”) or to the category on the right by pressing a key on the right side (i.e., “k”; see Table 2 for screenshots). Participants were asked to respond as quickly as possible, but without making errors. When making an error, participants need to repeat the assignment and attribute the word to the correct category (i.e., built-in error penalty).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of the different task blocks in the Emotion Regulation–Implicit Association Test (ER-IAT).

The ER-IAT consists of five blocks, of which Blocks 1, 2, and 4 are practice trials (20 trials per block; see Table 2 for an overview of the blocks). In Block 1, the task starts with an introduction of the target-concept discrimination by assigning words to one of the following categories: (a) emotion regulation or (b) emotion expression. In Block 2, the attribute dimension is introduced with two categories: (a) positive or (b) negative. In Block 3, the previous two blocks were superimposed. Participants were asked to assign words to one of two combined categories: (a) emotion regulation or positive on the left and (b) emotion expression or negative on the right side of the screen. This block consists of 20 practice trials and 40 test trails. In Block 4, the participant learned to reverse the response assignment for the target discrimination of Block 1 (i.e., the target-concept category on the left and right switch places; Table 2). In Block 5, participants were again asked to categorize items into two combined categories. However, the response assignment for the target discrimination is still reversed. Thus, participants need to assign words to (a) emotion expression or positive and (b) emotion regulation or negative. Similar to Block 3, this block consisted of 20 practice trials and 40 test trails. The order of the blocks was not counterbalanced, as we were interested in the relative size of the ER-IAT effect across participants (in line with Mauss et al., 2006).

We followed the algorithm of Greenwald et al. (2003) to score the ER-IAT reaction time data (D) using both practice and test trials of Blocks 3 and 5. To account for individual variability of the latencies, average latencies of the trials were divided by an individual’s standard deviation. The final ER-IAT score was calculated by subtracting averages of Block 3 from averages of Block 5. The stronger (or weaker) the association of particular pairs of categories (e.g., “emotion regulation” and “positive”) is, the lower (or higher) the response times are compared to other category pairs (e.g., “emotion expression” and “positive”). A score above zero indicated an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation, and a score below zero implied an implicit positive attitude toward emotion expression. Good internal consistency was found using split-half methodology over Blocks 3 and 5 (α = 0.80).

Interpersonal Teacher–Student Relationship

The Questionnaire on Teacher Interaction (QTI; Wubbels et al., 1985) was used to chart student perceptions of the teacher–student relationship in terms of teachers’ Agency and Communion in class. Agency refers to taking the lead, social influence, or dominance. Communion refers to friendliness, affection, or warmth. Twenty-four items were used to measure both dimensions and as is customary in circumplex measures, each item was weighted separately for each interpersonal dimension. For example, “this teacher is strict” was weighted strongly positive for Agency (i.e., 0.92) and less strongly and negative for teacher Communion (i.e., -0.38). The item “this teacher is patient” on the other hand was weighted moderately negative (i.e., -0.38) for Agency and strongly positive for Communion (i.e., 0.92; see for a comprehensive explanation Den Brok et al., 2004). Items were answered on a five-point Likert scale bounded by 1 “(almost) never” and 5 “(almost) always.” Students’ scores on the items were averaged at the class level. In the current study, we found good internal consistency for both agency and communion at the class level, with α = 0.89 and α = 0.93, respectively (cf. Den Brok et al., 2004).

Data Analysis

Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were used to examine the effect of teachers’ typical use of and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation strategies on teachers’ level of emotional exhaustion (step 1) and to investigate the moderating role of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation (step 2). In a third step, we tested the effect of including our covariates. The statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS version 24. Based on visual inspection of scatterplots, linearity could be assumed. Variables were normally distributed (skewness and kurtosis values were between -1.96 and 1.96 for all variables). To avoid problems with multicollinearity, the interaction terms of typical use of and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation were created by multiplying the centered scores (Helm and Mark, 2012). The assumption of homoscedasticity was met, and residuals were independent and normally distributed. Furthermore, one univariate outlier was identified for the interaction term between expression suppression and the ER-IAT (z-score = 3.79), but further inspection showed that there was no reason to delete this value. There were no multivariate outliers (Cook’s distance < 1 for all variables).

We investigated the research questions in separate models for cognitive reappraisal (model 1) and expressive suppression (model 2) to increase the power. The steps are described below for model 1, but were similar for model 2 (see Table 3 for an overview of the regression models). For model 1a, cognitive reappraisal and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation were entered in the first step as predictors of emotional exhaustion. In the second step, to explore the added value of including the moderating role of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation, the interaction effect between cognitive reappraisal and implicit attitudes was added to the model. Graphical representations were made to interpret the direction of the moderation (Helm and Mark, 2012) using PROCESS version 3.3 in SPSS version 24. In the third step, teaching experience and gender were added to the model as potential covariates. These were included only in the final step because of the limited sample size and in order to assess the robustness of the models tested in steps 1 and 2. A separate model (model 1b) was estimated where we added teacher agency and communion in the third step, since data on the teacher–student relationship were only available for a subsample (N = 72). Because all missing data for the teacher–student relationship were located in the group of teachers with less than 5 years of experience (secondary education teachers N = 21 and student teachers N = 1) and thus not missing completely at random (MCAR), we compared a model using the default option listwise deletion (LD, N = 72) and using multiple imputation (MI) with five imputations (N = 94). All predictor and outcome variables and several auxiliary variables (i.e., handedness, age, and education type) were included as predictors for the imputations (Collins et al., 2001).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of conducted hierarchical multiple regression models.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 4. On average, teachers scored low on emotional exhaustion (M = 1.64, SD = 1.05), but scores ranged from 0 (never) to 5.13 (several times a week to daily). They reported significantly more use of cognitive reappraisal (M = 4.66, SD = 1.01) than expression suppression [M = 3.30, SD = 1.09; t(93) = 9.487, p < 0.001]. Teachers’ implicit attitude toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression ranged from negative to positive with a mean of −0.09 (SD = 0.45), indicating on average a slight preference for emotion expression compared to emotion regulation. Teachers’ communion levels were moderate and teachers’ agency levels were relatively lower than in prior research (Claessens et al., 2016), which can be explained by the relatively large number of teachers with less than 5 years of experience in our sample (cf. Brekelmans et al., 2005). Indeed, we found a significant association between teaching experience and agency (r = 0.33; Table 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for the full sample and the three separate groups.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Pearson correlations for the full sample (N = 94).

Correlational Analyses

The correlational analyses (Table 5) showed that teachers who reported higher levels of emotional exhaustion were more likely to report less use of cognitive reappraisal (r = −0.24), were perceived as lower on teacher agency by students (r = −0.31), and had less teaching experience (r = −0.23). Although there was no correlation between teachers’ implicit attitude toward emotion regulation and their feelings of emotional exhaustion, we found that teachers who had an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) tended to report somewhat more use of expressive suppression strategies (r = 0.21). Female teachers reported more use of cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategies (r = 0.27). Finally, we found that students tended to report higher levels of communion in classrooms of teachers who reported to use more expressive suppression (r = 0.26).

Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses

Cognitive Reappraisal

To examine the relationship between cognitive reappraisal and emotional exhaustion, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted. Table 6 presents a summary of the results, including unstandardized regression coefficients (b), standardized regression coefficients (β), and the standard errors (SE) of the unstandardized regression coefficients. In step 1, we found that cognitive reappraisal was a significant predictor of emotional exhaustion (β = −0.24, p = 0.019) meaning that teachers who reported more use of cognitive reappraisal scored lower on emotional exhaustion and vice versa. Cognitive reappraisal and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation accounted for a non-significant 5.9% of the variance in emotional exhaustion. Adding the interaction term between cognitive reappraisal and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation in step 2 explained an additional non-significant 1.3% of the variance in emotional exhaustion. Cognitive reappraisal remained a significant predictor (β = −0.25, p = 0.015), but implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation were not a statistically significant moderator (see Figure B1 in Supplementary Material B for a visualization). In the third step, we included teaching experience and gender, and together these factors explained a significant 12.4% of the variance in emotional exhaustion. Next to cognitive reappraisal, teaching experience was a significant predictor of emotional exhaustion. Teachers with less than 5 years of experience scored on average 0.44 higher on emotional exhaustion than teachers with more than 5 years of experience.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of the hierarchical multiple regression for Model 1a.

To explore the effect of the teacher–student relationship on teachers’ emotional exhaustion, we included agency and communion in the third step for a subsample of teachers (N = 72; see bold coefficients in Table 7). This model explained a significant 19.2% in the variance of emotional exhaustion. Both cognitive reappraisal (β = −0.28, p = 0.017) and agency (β = −0.32, p = 0.007) were significant predictors of emotional exhaustion. To test the effect of the teacher–student relationship for the whole sample (N = 94), we conducted MI with five imputations. Table 7 presents the pooled results of the regression analysis in italic. Percentages of explained variance and standardized regression coefficients cannot be computed for pooled data. Similar to the models above, cognitive reappraisal (b = −0.22, p = 0.028) and teacher agency (b = −2.85, p = 0.006) predicted teachers’ emotional exhaustion significantly.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Results of the hierarchical multiple regressions for model 1b using LD and MI.

Expressive Suppression

Table 8 presents the results for teachers’ use of expressive suppression (model 2a). Expressive suppression and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation accounted for a negligible 0.5% of variance in emotional exhaustion; neither expressive suppression nor implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation were significant predictors. The model including also the interaction term between expressive suppression and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation explained a non-significant 1.4% of the variance in emotional exhaustion. Implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation were not a statistically significant moderator of the relationship between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion (see Figure B2 in Supplementary Material B for a visualization). Including teaching experience and gender in the third step led to an extra 5.5% explained variance of emotional exhaustion. We found that teachers with less than 5 years of experience scored on average 0.49 higher on emotional exhaustion than teachers with more than 5 years of experience.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Results of the hierarchical multiple regression for Model 2a.

Table 9 presents a summary of the results for model 2b including teacher agency and communion. Using LD (N = 72), we found that a significant 15.8% of the variance of emotional exhaustion could be explained by our predictors. In contrast to the previous models, teachers’ implicit attitude toward emotion regulation was a significant moderator of the relationship between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion in step 2 (β = −0.27, p = 0.030). Figure 1 illustrates that teachers who showed a stronger implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation and reported making more use of expressive suppression strategies reported lower emotional exhaustion. On the other hand, teachers who showed a stronger implicit positive attitude toward emotion expression and reported making more use of expressive suppression strategies reported higher levels of emotional exhaustion. A potential explanation for this finding might be that this subsample contained less teachers with limited years of experience (<5 years) than the full sample. Since we found that teaching experience was a significant predictor of emotional exhaustion in model 1/2a, it is possible that the predictive value of the interaction between using expressive suppression and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) only holds for teachers with more years of experience. The moderation effect failed to reach significance in the final step (β = −0.23, p = 0.055), where we found agency to be the only significant predictor of emotional exhaustion (β = −0.29, p = 0.014). In our model using MI (N = 94), implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation did not significantly moderate the relationship between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion (b = −0.20, p = 0.326). Again, only agency predicted emotional exhaustion significantly (b = −3.01, p = 0.005).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Results of the hierarchical multiple regression for Model 2b using LD and MI.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Visualization of the moderating effect of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation on the relationship between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion within the subsample (step 2).

Discussion

The present study investigated the association of teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression) with teachers’ feelings of emotional exhaustion and explored the moderating effect of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression on this association with an IAT. We included covariates that have been associated with teachers’ burnout level in previous studies (i.e., the teacher–student relationship, teaching experience, and gender). This study marks one of the first attempts to examine the potential moderating role of teachers’ implicit preferences on the relation between their typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their emotional exhaustion.

Emotion Regulation Strategies and Emotional Exhaustion

In line with our hypothesis, we found that teachers who reported more use of cognitive reappraisal tended to report less emotional exhaustion. The strength of this association was comparable to previous studies in the teaching context (Tsouloupas et al., 2010; Andela and Truchot, 2015). This suggests that cognitive reappraisal is an adaptive emotion regulation strategy also in the teaching context (e.g., Gross and John, 2003; Barber et al., 2011; Becker et al., 2015). It might be worthwhile for teacher training and professional development programs for teachers to develop a training program focused on practicing cognitive reappraisal. Recent studies in other fields have shown positive effects of such trainings on well-being and stress reduction (Denny and Ochsner, 2014; Ranney et al., 2017).

In contrast to findings from other studies (mainly outside the educational context), we did not find a significant positive relationship between expressive suppression and emotional exhaustion. A possible explanation might be that expressive suppression can sometimes be functional for teachers in the classroom context because it conforms to display rules (cf. Frenzel, 2014). Sutton et al. (2009), for example, differentiated between regulating positive and negative emotions and found that teachers disagreed on the effectiveness of expressing negative emotions. Two-thirds of the teachers in their study reported less teaching effectiveness after expressing negative emotions (Sutton et al., 2009) and thus seemed to have a preference for suppressing their negative emotions during teaching.

Moderating Effect of Implicit Attitudes Toward Emotion Regulation

Contrary to our hypothesis, implicit positive attitudes toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) did not directly predict emotional exhaustion levels nor did it moderate the relationship between teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their feelings of emotional exhaustion in the full sample. This contradicts previous findings by Hopp et al. (2011) outside the educational context, who found a positive moderating effect of an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation when combined with the use of cognitive reappraisal strategies. The absence of this moderating effect in our sample of teachers is possibly due to relatively strong display rules in the educational setting, which might diminish the effect of teachers’ individual preferences or implicit attitudes (Gosserand and Diefendorff, 2005; Mauss et al., 2008).

Interestingly, we did find a significant moderation effect of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation in a subsample with more experienced teachers. More experienced teachers who demonstrated an implicit positive attitude toward regulating their emotions and who reported making more use of expressive suppression strategies tended to report lower levels of emotional exhaustion. Likewise, teachers who showed an implicit positive attitude toward emotion expression and reported making more use of expressive suppression strategies reported somewhat higher levels of emotional exhaustion. The effect of teachers’ typical use of and implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation versus emotion expression on their emotional exhaustion might thus be dissimilar for teachers with different levels of experience. We found that not only the levels of emotional exhaustion were highest for teachers with less than 5 years of experience, but they also used less cognitive reappraisal compared to more experienced teachers. Sutton and Harper (2009) suggest that beginning teachers might experience high stress levels, which may result in more difficulty in regulating their emotions and therefore, they may directly express their emotions more often. Experienced teachers have been found to be more likely to regulate in the immediate situation by either reappraising or suppressing the emotion (Sutton and Harper, 2009), and for them it might be more important to have an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) as a buffer against the potential negative effects of expressive suppression. Experienced teachers might encounter negative consequences of expressive suppression only when they have a preference for emotion expression. Along similar lines, for teachers who have internalized the display rules of emotion regulation, using expressive suppression may not be harmful. It should be noted that the moderation effect was not significant anymore when teacher agency was included in the model, thus the higher agency level of more experienced teachers might compensate for the potential negative effects of using expressive suppression while having an implicit positive attitude toward emotion expression.

Contextual and Personal Factors

We found that teachers with higher interpersonal agency levels and more teaching experience reported less emotional exhaustion. This is in line with the finding that the strategy–situation fit is an important protective factor against developing burnout symptoms (Troy et al., 2013; Haines et al., 2016). Furthermore, it supports earlier findings that less experienced teachers report more tension and negative emotions (Harmsen et al., 2018). This is important, as emotional exhaustion is a major reason for beginning teachers to quit the profession (Buchanan et al., 2013). Hence, the first 5 years of teaching may serve as a sensitive period for promoting regulation strategies that help to lower feelings of stress and emotional exhaustion. Teachers’ level of communion and teachers’ gender were not associated with their level of emotional exhaustion. However, we found that teachers who were perceived as being relatively high on communion reported more use of expressive suppression strategies, which may indicate that expressive suppression could help to build more positive relationships with students. Further, female teachers reported more use of cognitive reappraisal strategies than men, which is in line with previous findings (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011). Research is needed to further examine the potential indirect effects of these covariates on teachers’ emotional exhaustion via differential use of emotion regulation strategies.

Limitations and Future Directions

The present study was one of the first to test the role of teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation in teachers’ level of emotional exhaustion. The findings of the current study should be interpreted with care, and replication studies are needed to validate the results. In future research, it should be tested whether the differential role of an implicit preference for emotion regulation versus emotion expression for beginning versus more experienced teachers holds in a larger sample. Other aspects of teacher burnout—such as depersonalization or personal accomplishment—should be investigated to see if their association with emotion regulation differs from the findings with regard to emotional exhaustion. Also, future studies could integrate more diverse implicit measures of emotion regulation, such as physiological measures (e.g., Donker et al., 2018) or student ratings of teachers’ emotion regulation (e.g., Jiang et al., 2016) to get a more integrative view of implicit processes in teachers’ emotion regulation.

Although the effects were on average small, they were similar to other studies on teacher burnout (e.g., Evers et al., 2004) and are in line with the idea that many factors interact in predicting teachers’ level of burnout (Chang, 2009; Frenzel, 2014). It would be interesting for future research on teacher emotions and burnout to make more use of measures of implicit preferences for emotion regulation versus emotion expression. We found a small positive correlation between teachers’ use of expressive suppression and their implicit attitude toward emotion regulation. This suggests that the ER-IAT might tap into the motivation to use expressive suppression beyond having only a positive attitude toward emotion regulation.

A limitation is that we used student ratings of the teacher–student relationship. Although their reliability and validity have been shown in previous studies (Wubbels and Brekelmans, 2005; Den Brok et al., 2006), what teachers themselves think about the interpersonal relationship with students might be more strongly related to their own well-being (Aldrup et al., 2018). Finally, it should be noted that our findings are correlational, and thus, we cannot draw any causal conclusions. It is possible that more emotionally exhausted teachers might use cognitive reappraisal to a lesser degree because they do not have the cognitive capacity left to engage in emotion regulation (Richards and Gross, 1999).

Conclusion

Notwithstanding these limitations and suggestions for further research, the present study adds to our knowledge about emotion regulation in teachers. First of all, there were large differences between teachers in how their emotion regulation strategies and preferences were associated with feelings of emotional exhaustion. This illustrates that the potential benefits and costs of emotion regulation may differ among teachers and that there may be potentially important moderating variables. The results replicated previous findings about the benefits of using cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategies and the important role of experience and interpersonal agency in reducing teachers’ feelings of emotional exhaustion in general. A moderating effect of implicit attitudes toward emotion regulation on the association between teachers’ typical use of emotion regulation strategies and their emotional exhaustion level was only found in a subsample with more experienced teachers. This suggests that having an implicit positive attitude toward emotion regulation (as opposed to emotion expression) might help to attenuate emotional exhaustion, especially when one is using expressive suppression strategies, which are common in educational settings due to the display rules for teachers. Studies such as these can inform teacher educators on how to better prepare teachers for their emotional job or support them during professional development courses, for instance, by discussing effective emotion regulation strategies while taking into account the role of more implicit attitudes, both in beginning and more experienced teachers.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Review Board of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

MD, TG, and TM designed the study. MD recruited participants and collected the data. MD and ME analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Netherlands Initiative for Education Research (NRO/PROO grant 405-14-300-039), which resides under the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Anja Tolpekina for her engagement in the development of the ER-IAT and Suzanne van Hoogstraten, Kim Severien, Maaike van der Veen, Joeri Baarda, Hillery Hommes, and Talitah Postma for their contribution to data collection. Special thanks to all the teachers and students participating in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00867/full#supplementary-material

References

Aldrup, K., Klusmann, U., Lüdtke, O., Göllner, R., and Trautwein, U. (2018). Student misbehavior and teacher well-being: testing the mediating role of the teacher-student relationship. Learn. Instruct. 58, 126–136. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andela, M., and Truchot, D. (2015). Emotional labour and burnout: some methodological considerations and refinements. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 47, 321–332. doi: 10.1037/cbs0000024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arens, A. K., and Morin, A. J. S. (2016). Relations between teachers’ emotional exhaustion and students’ educational outcomes. J Educ. Psychol. 108, 800–813. doi: 10.1037/edu0000105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, L. K., Grawitch, M. J., Carson, R. L., and Tsouloupas, C. N. (2011). Costs and benefits of supportive versus disciplinary emotion regulation strategies in teachers. Stress Health 27, 173–187. doi: 10.1002/smi.1357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, E. S., Keller, M. M., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., and Taxer, J. L. (2015). Antecedents of teachers’ emotions in the classroom: an intraindividual approach. Front. Psychol. 6:635. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonanno, G. A., and Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: an individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 591–612. doi: 10.1177/1745691613504116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bondy, E., Ross, D. D., Hambacher, E., and Acosta, M. (2013). Becoming warm demanders: perspectives and practices of first year teachers. Urban Educ. 48, 420–450. doi: 10.1177/0042085912456846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brekelmans, M., Wubbels, T., and Van Tartwijk, J. (2005). Teacher-student relationships across the teaching career. Int. J. Educ. Res. 43, 55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2006.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, J., Prescott, A., Schuck, S., Aubusson, P., and Burke, P. (2013). Teacher retention and attrition: views of early career teachers. Aus. J. Teacher Educ. 38, 112–129. doi: 10.14221/ajte.2013v38n3.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., and Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion 3, 48–67. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M. L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of teacher burnout: examining the emotional work of teachers. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 21, 193–218. doi: 10.1007/s10648-009-9106-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M. L. (2013). Toward a theoretical model to understand teacher emotions and teacher burnout in the context of student misbehavior: appraisal, regulation and coping. Motiv. Emot. 37, 799–817. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9335-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M. L., and Davis, H. A. (2009). “Understanding the role of teacher appraisals in shaping the dynamics of their relationships with students: deconstructing teachers’ judgments of disruptive behavior/students,” in Advances in Teacher Emotion Research: The Impact on Teachers’ Lives, eds P. A. Schutz and M. Zembylas, (Boston, MA: Springer), 95–127. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-0564-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claessens, L., Van Tartwijk, J., Pennings, H. J. M., Van der Want, A., Verloop, N., Den Brok, P., et al. (2016). Beginning and experienced secondary school teachers’ self- and student schema in positive and problematic teacher–student relationships. Teach. Teacher Educ. 55, 88–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., and Kam, C.-M. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychol. Methods 6, 330–351. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.6.4.330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, D. I., and Hong, J. Y. (2012). An ecological examination of teachers’ emotions in the school context. Teach. Teach. Educ. 28, 957–967. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2012.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Brok, P., Brekelmans, M., and Wubbels, T. (2004). Interpersonal teacher behaviour and student outcomes. School Effect. Sch. Improv. 15, 407–442. doi: 10.1080/09243450512331383262

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Brok, P., Brekelmans, M., and Wubbels, T. (2006). Multilevel issues in research using students’ perceptions of learning environments: the case of the Questionnaire on Teacher Interaction. Learn. Environ. Res. 9, 199–213. doi: 10.1007/s10984-006-9013-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denny, B. T., and Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Behavioral effects of longitudinal training in cognitive reappraisal. Emotion 14, 425–433. doi: 10.1037/a0035276.Behavioral

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donker, M. H., Van Gog, T., and Mainhard, M. T. (2018). A quantitative exploration of two teachers with contrasting emotions: intra-individual process analyses of physiology and interpersonal behavior. Front. Learn. Res. 6:162–184. doi: 10.14786/flr.v6i3.372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durr, T., Chang, M. L., and Carson, R. L. (2014). “Curbing teacher burnout: the transactional factors of teacher efficacy and emotion management,” Teacher Motivation: Theory and Practice eds H. Watt, S. Karabenick, and P. Richardson, (Abingdon: Routledge) doi: 10.4324/9780203119273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., and Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in daily life: the role of social context and goals. Motiv. Emot. 41, 230–242. doi: 10.1007/s11031-016-9597-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

EU-OSHA (2013). European Opinion Poll on Occupational Safety and Health 2013. Spain: EU-OSHA.

Google Scholar

Evers, W. J. G., Tomic, W., and Brouwers, A. (2004). Burnout among teachers: students’ and teachers’ perceptions compared. Sch. Psychol. Int. 25, 131–148. doi: 10.1177/0143034304043670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farber, B. A. (1984). Stress and burnout in suburban teachers. J. Educ. Res. 77, 325–331.

Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C. (2014). “Teacher emotions,” in International Handbook of Emotions in Education, eds E. A. Linnenbrink-Garcia and R. Pekrun, (New York, NY: Routledge), 494–519.

Google Scholar

Goetz, T., Becker, E. S., Bieg, M., Keller, M. M., Frenzel, A. C., and Hall, N. C. (2015). The glass half empty: how emotional exhaustion affects the state-trait discrepancy in self-reports of teaching emotions. PLoS One 10:e137441. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosserand, R. H., and Diefendorff, J. M. (2005). Emotional display rules and emotional labor: the moderating role of commitment. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 1256–1264. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 95–110. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 481–481. doi: 10.1037/h0087889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 224–237. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haga, S. M., Kraft, P., and Corby, E.-K. (2009). Emotion regulation: antecedents and well-being outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in cross-cultural samples. J. Happ. Stud. 10, 271–291. doi: 10.1007/s10902-007-9080-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagenauer, G., and Volet, S. E. (2014). “I don’t hide my feelings, even though I try to”: insight into teacher educator emotion display. Aus. Educ. Res. 41, 261–281. doi: 10.1007/s13384-013-0129-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haines, S. J., Gleeson, J., Kuppens, P., Hollenstein, T., Ciarrochi, J., Labuschagne, I., et al. (2016). The wisdom to know the difference: strategy-situation fit in emotion regulation in daily life is associated with well-being. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1651–1659. doi: 10.1177/0956797616669086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hargreaves, A. (2000). Mixed emotions: teachers’ perceptions of their interactions with students. Teach. Teach. Educ. 16, 811–826. doi: 10.1016/S0742-051X(00)00028-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmsen, R., Helms-Lorenz, M., Maulana, R., and Van Veen, K. (2018). The relationship between beginning teachers’ stress causes, stress responses, teaching behaviour and attrition. Teach. Teach. 24, 626–643. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2018.1465404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helm, R., and Mark, A. (2012). Analysis and evaluation of moderator effects in regression models: state of art, alternatives and empirical example. Rev. Manag. Sci. 6, 307–332. doi: 10.1007/s11846-010-0057-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Hollenstein, T. (2015). This time, it’s real: affective flexibility, time scales, feedback loops, and the regulation of emotion. Emot. Rev. 7, 308–315. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, H., Troy, A. S., and Mauss, I. B. (2011). The unconscious pursuit of emotion regulation: implications for psychological health. Cogn. Emot. 25, 532–545. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.532606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Vauras, M., Volet, S., and Wang, Y. (2016). Teachers’ emotions and emotion regulation strategies: self- and students’ perceptions. Teach. Teach. Educ. 54, 22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.11.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, H. A. M., and Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: do emotional intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process? J. Occup. Health Psychol. 12, 319–333. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, M. M., Chang, M.-L., Becker, E. S., Goetz, T., and Frenzel, A. C. (2014). Teachers’ emotional experiences and exhaustion as predictors of emotional labor in the classroom: an experience sampling study. Front. Psychol. 5:1442. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., and New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: a new measure for Dutch word frequency based on film subtitles. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 643–650. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R. M., Perry, N. E., and Frenzel, A. C. (2012). Teachers’ relatedness with students: an underemphasized component of teachers’ basic psychological needs. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 150–165. doi: 10.1037/a0026253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koole, S. L., and Jostmann, N. B. (2004). Getting a grip on your feelings: effects of action orientation and external demands on intuitive affect regulation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 974–990. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyriacou, C. (1987). Teacher stress and burnout: an international review. Educ. Res. 29, 146–152. doi: 10.1080/0013188870290207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavy, S., and Eshet, R. (2018). Spiral effects of teachers’ emotions and emotion regulation strategies: evidence from a daily diary study. Teach. Teach. Educ. 73, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2018.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive appraisal. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 69, 195–205. doi: 10.1037/h0044635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M., Pekrun, R., Taxer, J. L., Schutz, P. A., Vogl, E., and Xie, X. (2016). Teachers’ emotions and emotion management: integrating emotion regulation theory with emotional labor research. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 19, 843–863. doi: 10.1007/s11218-016-9359-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. H. (2019). Emotional labor, teacher burnout, and turnover intention in high-school physical education teaching. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 25, 236–253. doi: 10.1177/1356336X17719559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, R. S. T., Overall, N. C., Hammond, M. D., and Girme, Y. U. (2017). Emotional suppression during personal goal pursuit impedes goal strivings and achievement. Emotion 17, 208–223. doi: 10.1037/emo0000218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., and Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory, 3rd Edn. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., and Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 397–422. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., and Gross, J. J. (2007). Automatic emotion regulation. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 1, 146–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., and Gross, J. J. (2008). “Culture and automatic emotion regulation,” in Regulating Emotions: Culture, Social Necessity, and Biological Inheritance, eds S. J. S. Ismer, S. Kronast, C. Van Scheve, and M. Vanderkerckhove, (London: Blackwell Publishing), 39–60.

Google Scholar

Mauss, I. B., Evers, C., Wilhelm, F. H., and Gross, J. J. (2006). How to bite your tongue without blowing your top: implicit evaluation of emotion regulation predicts affective responding to anger provocation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 589–602. doi: 10.1177/0146167205283841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J. D., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 11, 143–162. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., and Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms? Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 993–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Pers. Individ. Differ. 51, 704–708. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, R. E., McKenzie, J., Mills, K. A., Patulny, R., Bellocchi, A., and Caristo, F. (2019). Gendered emotion management and teacher outcomes in secondary school teaching: a review. Teach. Teach. Educ. 80, 128–144. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2019.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ranney, R. M., Bruehlman-Senecal, E., and Ayduk, O. (2017). Comparing the effects of three online cognitive reappraisal trainings on well-being. J. Happ. Stud. 18, 1319–1338. doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9779-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. M., and Gross, J. J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of emotion suppression. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 1033–1044. doi: 10.1177/01461672992511010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, C. M. E. (2017). Emotion regulation in the context of daily stress: impact on daily affect. Pers. Individ. Differ. 112, 150–156. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Van Dierendonck, D. (2000). Utrechtse Burnout Scale (UBOS). Testhandleiding. [Utrecht Burnout Scale. Test Manual]. Amsterdam: Harcourt Test Services.

Google Scholar

Siep, N., Roefs, A., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., Bonte, M., and Jansen, A. (2012). Fighting food temptations: the modulating effects of short-term cognitive reappraisal, suppression and up-regulation on mesocorticolimbic activity related to appetitive motivation. Neuroimage 60, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaapen, D. L., Waters, F., Brummer, L., Stopa, L., and Bucks, R. S. (2014). The emotion regulation questionnaire: validation of the ERQ-9 in two community samples. Psychol. Assess. 26, 46–54. doi: 10.1037/a0034474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spilt, J. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., and Thijs, J. T. (2011). Teacher wellbeing: the importance of teacher-student relationships. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 457–477. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9170-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, R. E., and Harper, E. (2009). “Teachers’ emotion regulation,” in International Handbook of Research on Teachers and Teaching, eds L. J. Saha and A. G. Dworkin, (Boston, MA: Springer), 389–401.

Google Scholar

Sutton, R. E., Mudrey-Camino, R., and Knight, C. C. (2009). Teachers’ emotion regulation and classroom management. Theor. Pract. 48, 130–137. doi: 10.1080/00405840902776418

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, R. E., and Wheatley, K. F. (2003). Teachers’ emotion and teaching: a review of the literature and directions for future research. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 15, 327–358. doi: 10.1023/A:1026131715856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taxer, J. L., Becker-Kurz, B., and Frenzel, A. C. (2019). Do quality teacher–student relationships protect teachers from emotional exhaustion? The mediating role of enjoyment and anger. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 22, 209–226. doi: 10.1007/s11218-018-9468-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taxer, J. L., and Frenzel, A. C. (2015). Facets of teachers’ emotional lives: a quantitative investigation of teachers’ genuine, faked, and hidden emotions. Teach. Teach. Educ. 49, 78–88. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troy, A. S., Shallcross, A. J., and Mauss, I. B. (2013). A person-by-situation approach to emotion regulation: cognitive reappraisal can either help or hurt, depending on the context. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2505–2514. doi: 10.1177/0956797613496434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, W., Nguyen, D. J., Weiss, B., Ngo, V., and Lau, A. S. (2017). Cultural differences in the reciprocal relations between emotion suppression coping, depressive symptoms and interpersonal functioning among adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 657–669. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0192-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsouloupas, C. N., Carson, R. L., Matthews, R., Grawitch, M. J., and Barber, L. K. (2010). Exploring the association between teachers’ perceived student misbehaviour and emotional exhaustion: the importance of teacher efficacy beliefs and emotion regulation. Educ. Psychol. 30, 173–189. doi: 10.1080/01443410903494460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Bergh, L., Denessen, E., Hornstra, L., Voeten, M., and Holland, R. W. (2010). The implicit prejudiced attitudes of teachers: relations to teacher expectations and the ethnic achievement gap. Am. Educ. Res. J. 47, 497–527. doi: 10.3102/0002831209353594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Droogenbroeck, F., Spruyt, B., and Vanroelen, C. (2014). Burnout among senior teachers?: investigating the role of workload and interpersonal relationships at work. Teach. Teach. Educ. 43, 99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veldman, I., Admiraal, W., Van Tartwijk, J., Mainhard, T., and Wubbels, T. (2016). Veteran teachers’ job satisfaction as a function of personal demands and resources in the relationships with their students. Teach. Teach. 22, 913–926. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2016.1200546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, T. L., Miles, E., and Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychol. Bull. 138, 775–808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wubbels, T., and Brekelmans, M. (2005). Two decades of research on teacher-student relationships in class. Int. J. Educ. Res. 43, 6–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2006.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wubbels, T., Brekelmans, M., Den Brok, P., and Van Tartwijk, J. (2006). “An interpersonal perspective on classroom management in secondary classrooms in the Netherlands,” in Handbook of Classroom Management: Research, Practice, and Contemporary Issues, eds C. M. Evertson and C. S. Weinstein, (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 1161–1191.

Google Scholar

Wubbels, T., Créton, H. A., and Hooymayers, H. P. (1985). Discipline problems of beginning teachers, interactional teacher behaviour mapped out. Abstr. Resour. Educ. 20:153.

Google Scholar

Zlomke, K. R., and Hahn, K. S. (2010). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies: gender differences and associations to worry. Pers. Individ. Differ. 48, 408–413. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: teachers, emotional exhaustion, emotion regulation, expressive suppression, cognitive reappraisal, Implicit Association Test

Citation: Donker MH, Erisman MC, van Gog T and Mainhard T (2020) Teachers’ Emotional Exhaustion: Associations With Their Typical Use of and Implicit Attitudes Toward Emotion Regulation Strategies. Front. Psychol. 11:867. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00867

Received: 31 January 2020; Accepted: 07 April 2020;
Published: 29 May 2020.

Edited by:

Hongbiao Yin, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China

Reviewed by:

Ilona Papousek, University of Graz, Austria
Corinna M. Perchtold-Stefan, University of Graz, Austria

Copyright © 2020 Donker, Erisman, van Gog and Mainhard. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Monika H. Donker, bS5oLmRvbmtlckB1dS5ubA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.