Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 28 April 2020
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Context-Dependent Plasticity in Social Species: Feedback Loops Between Individual and Social Environment View all 9 articles

Regulation of Ant Foraging: A Review of the Role of Information Use and Personality

\r\nSwetashree Kolay*Swetashree Kolay1*Raphaël Boulay&#x;Raphaël Boulay2†Patrizia d&#x;Ettorre,Patrizia d’Ettorre1,3
  • 1Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology (LEEC) UR4443, Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Villetaneuse, France
  • 2Institute of Insect Biology (IRBI), UMR CNRS 7261, University of Tours, Tours, France
  • 3Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France

Animals live in heterogeneous environments where food resources are transient and have to be exploited rapidly. Ants show a wide range of foraging strategies and this activity is tightly regulated irrespective of the mode of recruitment used. Individual foragers base their decision to forage on information received from nestmates (social information). Transmission of information can be in the form of direct physical interactions such as antennation or indirect exchange of information such as laying of pheromone trails. Foragers also rely on information from their internal states or experience (personal information). The interaction between these two sources of information gives rise to plasticity in foraging behavior. Recent studies have examined the role of personality (consistent inter-individual variation in behavioral traits) during ant foraging. Since colonies differ from each other in the distribution of personalities of their members, colonies may consistently differ in behavioral traits, giving rise to colony level personality. However, the interaction between information use and personality, especially at the individual level, remains unexplored. Here, we briefly summarize the literature on the effect of social and personal information on the regulation of ant foraging and the effect of personality on this behavior. We point out that a more focused examination of the interplay between personality and information use will help us understand how behavioral plasticity in the context of foraging is shaped at the colony and individual levels.

Introduction

Animals live in complex and heterogeneous environments with fluctuating resource availability. Effective decision making in different contexts is critical to their survival and fitness. In group-living species, including humans, collective decisions emerge from the actions of individual group members. The behavior of each individual is modulated by the behavior of others and affects the group as a whole (Conradt and List, 2009). Social insects such as ants live in colonies which consist of tens to millions of individuals and function as self-organized systems without central leadership (Jandt and Gordon, 2016). This is possible due to exchange of information among nest mates which allows individuals to coordinate their activities, thus maximizing colony efficiency (Duarte et al., 2011). Here, we first describe the different modes of foraging recruitment in ants. We then discuss how the use of different sources of information and consistent variation in behavioral traits among colonies and individuals contribute to bring about context-dependent plasticity in ant foraging behavior.

Recruitment Mechanisms in Ants

Of the more than 16,000 recorded species of ants (Bolton, 2020), foraging recruitment has been studied in detail in only a handful of species and they use different strategies to recruit nestmates to the food source (reviewed in Holldobler and Wilson, 1990).

(1) Tandem running: In this mode of recruitment, a recruiter who knows the location of the food source leads one nestmate at a time from the nest to the food source (reviewed in Franklin, 2014). Cohesion between the tandem pair is maintained by contiguous physical contact between the two ants and/or by short-range chemical signals emitted by the recruiter. The number of recruited foragers is largely proportional to the number of successful scouts as the recruits have to be actively guided by the recruiters on each trip.

(2) Mass recruitment using chemical trails: Here, ants that find a food source lay a pheromone trail while returning to the nest and this triggers the recruitment of nestmates (Wilson, 1962). The recruits, in turn, reinforce the trail while returning to the nest which leads to further recruitment (reviewed in Czaczkes et al., 2015). The number of ants joining the trail is a function of its strength. To prevent excessive mobilization of foragers, reinforcement of the trail is downregulated or an inhibitory signal may be produced.

(3) Group recruitment: In this case, one ant summons a few nestmates at a time from the nest and the recruited ants follow the leader ant closely to reach the food source. Although a trail may be laid by the leader, it is not enough to stimulate recruitment alone. Here, as in tandem running, the number of recruits will be determined by the number of successful scouts (Holldobler and Wilson, 1990).

(4) Group retrieving based on distant homing: This is another mode of recruitment that has been proposed where individual scouts appear to transmit information about a distant food source to groups of recruits through direct physical contact such as antennation. No other cues such as chemical trails or direct guiding is required for recruitment (Reznikova, 2008).

Independent of the recruitment strategy, the recruit receives information about the food source such as scent and type of food from the recruiter. This information helps the recruit in making its foraging decisions such as whether to initiate foraging, which food source to select and which path to follow.

Use of Social and Personal Information in Regulation of Foraging

In social insect colonies, particularly in large ones, group members only have access to local information based on their position in the nest and the nestmates present in the vicinity (Mersch, 2016). Thus, foragers may not have direct access to information about food requirement of the colony. In addition, foragers have to choose from a range of alternatives such as type and location of food and path to the food source. Effective communication among workers is critical in regulating foraging activity. Individual foragers within a colony base their decisions to engage in foraging on social information received from their nestmates and their personal information. Social information may be obtained via direct interactions with nestmates and/or by indirect exchange of information. Foragers also rely on personal information based on their internal states, their interactions with the environment or their past experiences (reviewed in Dall et al., 2005). The majority of studies on information use during foraging have focused on trail laying species and there is little information available on species using other recruitment strategies.

Social Information

In trail laying species, the chemical trail, which usually contains multiple pheromones, transmits information about the food source to potential recruits. The number of ants laying trail pheromones as well as the intensity of pheromone deposition is related to the quality of food in several species such as the black garden ant Lasius niger (Mailleux et al., 2003; Detrain and Prieur, 2014), the pavement ant Tetramorium caespitum (Collignon and Detrain, 2010) and the Pharaoh’s ant Monomorium pharaonis (Jackson and Châline, 2007). However, it has recently been suggested that pheromone trails may actually provide rather inaccurate information about food quality (Czaczkes et al., 2019). In addition to recruiting workers from the nest, the trail also stimulates scouts who are already outside to join the trail, as has been seen in the neotropical species Pheidole oxyops (Czaczkes and Ratnieks, 2012). Use of a combination of two pheromones – a long-lasting pheromone and a shorter lasting one – which allows colonies to track foraging resources more effectively while maintaining foraging cohesion has been documented in M. pharaonis (Jackson et al., 2006), the army ant Leptogenys distinguenda (Witte and Maschwitz, 2002) and the big headed ant Pheidole megacephala (Dussutour et al., 2009). In order to downregulate recruitment to a food source, L. niger foragers reduce pheromone deposition on trails that have already been heavily marked by trail pheromones (Czaczkes et al., 2013a) while a no-entry pheromone appears to repel foragers from unrewarding paths in M. pharaonis (Robinson et al., 2005).

Much information can be exchanged through direct physical contact between nestmates. High collision rates between foragers on a trail cause them to reduce pheromone deposition (Czaczkes et al., 2013b) or drive some ants to choose an alternate path in L. niger (Dussutour et al., 2004). Encounters between returning and outgoing foragers convey information about the partner’s identity, the type of food being exploited and the richness of the food source. Leaf-cutter ants, Atta cephalotes, which collect leaves for the symbiotic fungus gardens inside their nests, use encounters on the trail to exchange information about the type of leaves being collected (Farji-Brener et al., 2010). Contact with food residues on a recruiter’s body informs the recruits about the food type that is being exploited and this increases the success of finding the food patch in L. niger (Le Breton and Fourcassié, 2004). In tandem running species, continuous antennal contact between the recruiter and the recruit is essential for progression of the tandem run (Richardson et al., 2007). During each tandem run, the recruits get the opportunity to learn the path to the food source and they, in turn, recruit other nestmates (Franklin and Franks, 2012). Scouts of Formica polyctena appear to convey quantitative information about the location of food sources to recruits through antennal contact (Reznikova and Ryabko, 2011).

Cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) comprising of a blend of different hydrocarbons are present in a wax layer on the insect body (Blomquist and Bagneres, 2010). The CHC profile of individuals is related to their task repertoire and can inform the task decisions of nestmates. For example, it has been shown in the red harvester ant Pogonomyrmex barbatus that foragers have a higher ratio of saturated, linear hydrocarbons to linear alkenes and branched alkanes on the cuticle as compared to workers performing tasks inside the nest (Wagner et al., 2001). This forager-specific CHC profile not only helps in preventing water loss, which is critical as these ants forage in hot and dry conditions, but has also a communicative function by affecting task decisions of others (Greene and Gordon, 2003). Brief antennal contacts with a returning forager at the nest entrance allows inactive foragers to assess its CHC profile and whether it is carrying food. The combination of both odors is required to stimulate foraging in this species (Greene et al., 2013).

Personal Information

Personal information may be related to an individual’s physiology with leaner individuals making extra foraging trips in response to an increased demand for foraging, as has been observed in Temnothorax albipennis (Robinson et al., 2009a). A forager’s decision to initiate recruitment may be based on an internal response threshold such as ingestion of a desired volume of liquid food at a food source as shown in L. niger (Mailleux et al., 2000) and this threshold increases under conditions of starvation (Mailleux et al., 2006). Enhanced response to recruitment signals after a period of starvation has been observed in species such as L. niger (Mailleux et al., 2011), Linepithema humile and Euprenolepis procera (von Thienen et al., 2016). Personal information may also be based on prior experience. In Ooceraea biroi, foraging tendency among individuals of the same age is strongly correlated to successful foraging experiences in the past (Ravary et al., 2007). In two Formica species, individual foragers tend to return to sites where they have had positive experiences in the past (Tanner, 2009). Tandem running recruiters use visual landmarks to improve upon previously learnt routes (Pratt et al., 2001) and likelihood of becoming a recruiter increases with experience (Franklin et al., 2012).

Interplay Between Social and Personal Information

Individual ants extensively use both social and personal information to make foraging decisions but reliance on a particular source of information depends on its content relative to other sources. L. niger uses a combination of route memory and trail pheromones to maximize foraging efficiency (Czaczkes et al., 2011). In species that use visual cues to form route memory, low light conditions may lead to reliance on social signals rather than reliance on personal memories as has been reported in L. niger (Jones et al., 2019) and Formica pratensis (Beugnon and Fourcassié, 1988). In T. albipennis, contact with returning foragers at the nest entrance causes bouts of activity. In the absence of this social information, physiology of individual foragers predicts which ants will leave the nest as mentioned earlier (Robinson et al., 2009b). When there is a conflict between social and private information, individuals depend on personal information to make foraging decisions in many species such as Acromyrmex subterraneus (Almeida et al., 2018), Formica lugubris (Fourcassié and Beugnon, 1988), L. niger (Aron et al., 1993; Grüter et al., 2011), and Paraponera clavata (Harrison et al., 1989). The reverse, i.e., preference for social information over private information, has been observed in L. humile (Aron et al., 1993), Atta cephalotes, Atta laevigata, and Acromyrmex octospinosus (Vilela et al., 1987) while no clear preference for either is shown in Iridomyrmex purpureus (Middleton et al., 2018). It has been suggested that ants prioritize social or personal information based on the information content of each source and choose the source that provides more detailed, accurate and reliable information about the food source. Thus, a change in the accuracy and reliability of information from one of the sources may cause individuals to switch their choice of information source as has been demonstrated in L. niger (Czaczkes et al., 2019).

Role of Individual and Collective Personality

The field of animal personality – defined as consistent inter-individual differences in behavioral traits across time and/or context – has seen rapid progress in the last two decades and personality traits have been documented in a wide range of taxa (Dingemanse et al., 2010). In social insects, in addition to individual differences in personality traits, groups differ consistently from each other in task performance and regulation of activity, giving rise to group level personality (Webster and Ward, 2011). For example, colonies may vary consistently in the baseline number of foragers that leave the nest to collect food. Group personality, or the particular configuration of behaviors expressed by the group, is likely to emerge from the differential aggregation of individual personalities comprising the colony or by external factors that vary consistently among colonies and affect colony behavior (Pinter-Wollman, 2012). Since the colony is the reproductive unit (Bourke, 2011), consistent behavioral variation among colonies may lead to fitness differences among them (Gordon, 2013). Certain behaviors such as boldness and aggression may be correlated at the population level and such suites of correlated behaviors are defined as behavioral syndromes (Sih et al., 2012). Within the behavioral syndrome expressed at the population level, each individual has a behavioral type; for instance, some individuals may be more bold and aggressive than others (Bell, 2007). Different behavioral types can coexist within a population (Wolf and Weissing, 2010).

Collective Personality

Several studies have looked at variations in behavioral traits at the level of the colony in different species of ants. In P. barbatus, colonies exhibit variation not only in the rate at which scouts leave the nest to search for food but also the ratio of outgoing foragers to returning foragers (Gordon et al., 2011). These differences in foraging activity among colonies persist from year to year (Gordon et al., 2013). Colonies of Pogonomyrmex occidentalis show consistent variation in the temporal pattern of foraging activity and also in the thermal range across which they forage (Cole et al., 2010). Colonies of Temnothorax rugatulus show consistency in their foraging effort and how they respond to different types of resources (Bengston and Dornhaus, 2014). Colonies of Aphaenogaster senilis that are more aggressive, readily explore novel environments and forage at higher temperatures (proactive colonies) are more successful than reactive colonies in retrieving food during intraspecific competition but suffer higher mortality rates (Blight et al., 2016). Colonies of S. invicta show persistent variation in foraging behaviors which is significantly related to colony growth (Bockoven et al., 2015). In L. niger, exploratory activity varies consistently among colonies and colonies with higher levels of exploratory activity discover and exploit food sources faster (Pasquier and Grüter, 2016). Colony personality is influenced by nest structure in Messor andrei and the foraging activity of colonies is consistent as long as they occupy the same nest sites (Pinter-Wollman et al., 2012). Behavioral syndromes have been identified at multiple levels in Myrmica ants where boldness is correlated with aggression at the caste level and with sociability at the colony level (Chapman et al., 2011).

Individual Personality

At the individual level, consistent variations in forager behavior has been observed in several species. In scouts of L. niger, intake of a desired volume of liquid is key to initiation of recruitment. This desired volume is specific to each individual, irrespective of its size, and remains constant over successive trips to a food source. There is also inter-individual difference in the persistence of trail-laying with some foragers never laying trails (Mailleux et al., 2005). Caste-based differences in personality traits have been identified in a few species. In Myrmica rubra, foragers are more active, exploratory, aggressive and attracted to light than workers who worked inside the nest (Pamminger et al., 2014). Foragers of Camponotus aethiops show better learning abilities and higher sucrose responsiveness than the nurses (Perez et al., 2013). However, it is not clear from these studies whether personality is related to the age of workers which determines which tasks they will perform. A few empirical studies have explicitly investigated the influence of personality of individual foragers on their foraging behavior. Learning performance was found to be correlated to exploration behavior in C. aethiops foragers with active explorers being slower to learn a task than less active ones (Udino et al., 2017).

Future Directions

Ant foraging behavior has been the focus of intensive studies for decades, yet much remains to be understood. We highlight a couple of avenues for further research that will give us a more comprehensive understanding of how individual and group level personality may affect the regulation of foraging in ants.

(1) Numerous studies have separately investigated how the use of different sources of information and personality, largely at the colony level, influence foraging. However, the interaction between these two factors, particularly at the level of individual foragers, in giving rise to plasticity in foraging behavior (as shown in Figure 1) remains relatively unexplored. We predict that individuals with different personalities will vary in the manner in which they perceive and use information, prioritize personal and social information, and in their learning abilities (Carere and Locurto, 2011; Sih and del Giudice, 2012). As a result, they will differ in task specializations. For example, individuals who are bolder and show more exploratory activity may more readily become scouts who go out in the initial search for food. These individuals should also have more flexible learning abilities and rely more on personal information. Foragers who vary in their foraging strategies as in Ectatomma ruidum (McGlynn et al., 2015) or in their resource specialization as in Formica aquilonia (Iakovlev and Reznikova, 2019) should also vary in their personality traits and cognitive abilities. Since the task repertoire of individual ants changes with age, further studies are also required to understand whether personality traits of individuals remain constant across their lifetimes and how this affects their task choice and task performance at different stages of their life.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The schematic represents the interplay between social and personal information and personality in regulating foraging behavior of individuals in ant colonies. The solid lines indicate known paths of interactions and the dotted lines indicate expected paths of interactions as discussed in the text. The arrowheads indicate the direction of the interaction. The different sources of information (social and personal) have been listed in the light gray boxes. The foraging decisions of individuals will determine collective foraging at the level of the colony.

(2) In social insects, colony personality is determined by the distribution of individual personalities within the group and differences in the underlying personality distributions will affect collective behavior. Most studies on foraging regulation have been done at the colony level by essentially looking at the average behavior of the group as a whole. Variations in behavioral traits at the individual level are not adequately captured by such studies (for example, Pamir et al., 2011 in honeybees). Thus, exploration of the distribution of individual personality traits within colonies will shed further light on how collective foraging behavior is shaped. For example, a colony with a higher proportion of individuals who are bold and show high exploratory activity should be able to track changing food resources or detect new food sources more efficiently. Such studies can be done by manipulating group compositions as has been done with ants in other contexts (Carere et al., 2018; Neumann and Pinter-Wollman, 2019).

An integrated analysis of personality and information use at the individual and colony levels will give us a more comprehensive understanding of the emergence and maintenance of context-dependent plasticity in ant foraging behavior.

Author Contributions

All authors have made substantial intellectual contribution to the work. SK wrote the manuscript with the help of Pd’E.

Funding

SK received a post-doctoral fellowship from the Fondation Fyssen, France. Pd’E is supported by the Institut Universitaire de France.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Almeida, N. G., De Camargo, R. D. S., Forti, L. C., and Lopes, J. F. S. (2018). Hierarchical establishment of information sources during foraging decision-making process involving Acromyrmex subterraneus (Forel, 1893) (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). Revist. Brasil. Entomol. 62, 36–39. doi: 10.1016/j.rbe.2017.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, S., Beckers, R., Deneubourg, J. L., and Pasteels, J. M. (1993). Memory and chemical communication in the orientation of two mass-recruiting ant species. Insect. Soc. 40, 369–380. doi: 10.1007/BF01253900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, A. M. (2007). Future directions in behavioural syndromes research. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 274, 755–761. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bengston, S. E., and Dornhaus, A. (2014). Be meek or be bold? A colony-level behavioural syndrome in ants. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281, 1791. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beugnon, G., and Fourcassié, V. (1988). How do red wood ants orient during diurnal and nocturnal foraging in a three dimensional system? II. Field experiments. Insect. Soc. 35, 106–124. doi: 10.1007/BF02224142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blight, O., Díaz-Mariblanca, G. A., Cerdá, X., and Boulay, R. (2016). A proactive–reactive syndrome affects group success in an ant species. Behav. Ecol. 27, 118–125. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomquist, G., and Bagneres, A. (2010). “Introduction: history and overview of insect hydrocarbons,” in Insect Hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry, And Chemical Ecology, eds G. Blomquist and A. Bagneres (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 3–18.

Google Scholar

Bockoven, A. A., Wilder, S. M., and Eubanks, M. D. (2015). Intraspecific variation among social insect colonies: persistent regional and colony-level differences in fire ant foraging behavior. PLoS One 10:e0133868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolton, B. (2020). AntWeb: Ants of Bolton World Catalog. Available online at: https://www.antweb.org/project.do?name=worldants (Accessed February 5, 2020).

Google Scholar

Bourke, A. F. G. (2011). Principles of Social Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Carere, C., Audebrand, C., Rödel, H. G., and d’Ettorre, P. (2018). Individual behavioural type and group performance in Formica fusca ants. Behav. Process. 157, 402–407. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carere, C., and Locurto, C. (2011). Interaction between animal personality and animal cognition. Curr. Zool. 57, 491–498. doi: 10.1093/czoolo/57.4.491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, B. B., Thain, H., Coughlin, J., and Hughes, W. O. H. (2011). Behavioural syndromes at multiple scales in Myrmica ants. Anim. Behav. 82, 391–397. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, B. J., Smith, A. A., Huber, Z. J., and Wiernasz, D. C. (2010). The structure of foraging activity in colonies of the harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex occidentalis. Behav. Ecol. 21, 337–342. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collignon, B., and Detrain, C. (2010). Distributed leadership and adaptive decision-making in the ant Tetramorium caespitum. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 1267–1273. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conradt, L., and List, C. (2009). Group decisions in humans and animals: a survey. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 719–742. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., Beckwith, J. J., Horsch, A.-L., and Hartig, F. (2019). The multi-dimensional nature of information drives prioritization of private osver social information in ants. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 286, 20191136. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., Grüter, C., Ellis, L., Wood, E., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2013a). Ant foraging on complex trails: route learning and the role of trail pheromones in Lasius niger. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 188–197. doi: 10.1242/jeb.076570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., Grüter, C., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2013b). Negative feedback in ants: crowding results in less trail pheromone deposition. J. R. Soc. Interf. 10:81. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.1009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., Grüter, C., Jones, S. M., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2011). Synergy between social and private information increases foraging efficiency in ants. Biol. Lett. 7, 521–524. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., Grüter, C., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2015). Trail pheromones: an integrative view of their role in social insect colony organization. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 60, 581–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-010814-020627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czaczkes, T. J., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2012). Pheromone trails in the Brazilian ant Pheidole oxyops: extreme properties and dual recruitment action. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 1149–1156. doi: 10.1007/s00265-012-1367-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dall, S. R. X., Giraldeau, L. A., Olsson, O., McNamara, J. M., and Stephens, D. W. (2005). Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol. and Evol. 20, 187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Detrain, C., and Prieur, J. (2014). Sensitivity and feeding efficiency of the black garden ant Lasius niger to sugar resources. J. Insect Physiol. 64, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2014.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dingemanse, N. J., Kazem, A. J. N., Réale, D., and Wright, J. (2010). Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 81–89. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2009.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duarte, A., Weissing, F. J., Pen, I., and Keller, L. (2011). An evolutionary perspective on self-organized division of labor in social insects. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 42, 91–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-102710-145017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dussutour, A., Fourcassié, V., Heibing, D., and Deneubourg, J. L. (2004). Optimal traffic organization in ants under crowded conditions. Nature 428, 70–73. doi: 10.1038/nature02345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dussutour, A., Nicolis, S. C., Shephard, G., Beekman, M., and Sumpter, D. J. T. (2009). The role of multiple pheromones in food recruitment by ants. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 2337–2348. doi: 10.1242/jeb.029827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farji-Brener, A. G., Amador-Vargas, S., Chinchilla, F., Escobar, S., Cabrera, S., Herrera, M. I., et al. (2010). Information transfer in head-on encounters between leaf-cutting ant workers: food, trail condition or orientation cues? Anim. Behav. 79, 343–349. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourcassié, V., and Beugnon, G. (1988). How do red wood ants orient when foraging in a three dimensional system? I. Laboratory experiments. Insect. Soc. 35, 92–105. doi: 10.1007/BF02224141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, E. L. (2014). The journey of tandem running: the twists, turns and what we have learned. Insect. Soc. 61, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s00040-013-0325-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, E. L., and Franks, N. R. (2012). Individual and social learning in tandem-running recruitment by ants. Anim. Behav. 84, 361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, E. L., Robinson, E. J. H., Marshall, J. A. R., Sendova-Franks, A. B., and Franks, N. R. (2012). Do ants need to be old and experienced to teach? J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1287–1292. doi: 10.1242/jeb.064618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, D. M. (2013). The rewards of restraint in the collective regulation of foraging by harvester ant colonies. Nature 498, 91–93. doi: 10.1038/nature12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, D. M., Dektar, K. N., and Pinter-Wollman, N. (2013). Harvester ant colony variation in foraging activity and response to humidity. PLoS One 8:63363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, D. M., Guetz, A., Greene, M. J., and Holmes, S. (2011). Colony variation in the collective regulation of foraging by harvester ants. Behav. Ecol. 22, 429–435. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, M. J., and Gordon, D. M. (2003). Cuticular hydrocarbons inform task decisions. Nature 423:32. doi: 10.1038/423032a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, M. J., Pinter-Wollman, N., and Gordon, D. M. (2013). Interactions with combined chemical cues inform harvester ant foragers’ decisions to leave the nest in search of food. PLoS One 8:e52219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grüter, C., Czaczkes, T. J., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2011). Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 141–148. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1020-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, J. F., Fewell, J. H., Stiller, T. M., and Breed, M. D. (1989). Effects of experience on use of orientation cues in the giant tropical ant. Anim. Behav. 37, 869–871. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90076-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holldobler, B., and Wilson, E. O. (1990). The Ants. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Iakovlev, I., and Reznikova, Z. (2019). Red wood ants display natural aversive learning differently depending on their task specialization. Front. Psychol. 10:710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, D. E., and Châline, N. (2007). Modulation of pheromone trail strength with food quality in Pharaoh’s ant, Monomorium pharaonis. Anim. Behav. 74, 463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, D. E., Martin, S. J., Holcombe, M., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2006). Longevity and detection of persistent foraging trails in Pharaoh’s ants, Monomorium pharaonis (L.). Anim. Behav. 71, 351–359. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jandt, J. M., and Gordon, D. M. (2016). The behavioral ecology of variation in social insects. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 15, 40–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2016.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S., Czaczkes, T. J., Gallager, A. J., Oberhauser, F. B., Gourlay, E., and Bacon, J. P. (2019). Copy when uncertain: lower light levels increase trail pheromone depositing and reliance on pheromone trails in ants. Anim. Behav. 156, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Breton, J., and Fourcassié, V. (2004). Information transfer during recruitment in the ant Lasius niger L. (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 242–250. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0704-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailleux, A. C., Buffin, A., Detrain, C., and Deneubourg, J. L. (2011). Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger. Insect. Soc. 58, 559–567. doi: 10.1007/s00040-011-0177-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailleux, A. C., Deneubourg, J. L., and Detrain, C. (2000). How do ants assess food volume? Anim. Behav. 59, 1061–1069. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailleux, A. C., Deneubourg, J. L., and Detrain, C. (2003). Regulation of ants’ foraging to resource productivity. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 270, 1609–1616. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailleux, A. C., Detrain, C., and Deneubourg, J. L. (2005). Triggering and persistence of trail-laying in foragers of the ant Lasius niger. J. Insect Physiol. 51, 297–304. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2004.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailleux, A. C., Detrain, C., and Deneubourg, J. L. (2006). Starvation drives a threshold triggering communication. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 4224–4229. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGlynn, T. P., Graham, R., Wilson, J., Emerson, J., Jandt, J. M., and Jahren, A. H. (2015). Distinct types of foragers in the ant Ectatomma ruidum: typical foragers and furtive thieves. Anim. Behav. 109, 243–247. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mersch, D. P. (2016). The social mirror for division of labor: what network topology and dynamics can teach us about organization of work in insect societies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 1087–1099. doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2104-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Middleton, E. J. T., Reid, C. R., Mann, R. P., and Latty, T. (2018). Social and private information influence the decision making of Australian meat ants (Iridomyrmex purpureus). Insect. Soc. 65, 649–656. doi: 10.1007/s00040-018-0656-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, K. M., and Pinter-Wollman, N. (2019). Collective responses to heterospecifics emerge from individual differences in aggression. Behav. Ecol. 30, 801–808. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arz017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pamir, E., Chakroborty, N. K., Stollhoff, N., Gehring, K. B., Antemann, V., Morgenstern, L., et al. (2011). Average group behavior does not represent individual behavior in classical conditioning of the honeybee. Learn. Mem. 18, 733–741. doi: 10.1101/lm.2232711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pamminger, T., Foitzik, S., Kaufmann, K. C., Schützler, N., and Menzel, F. (2014). Worker personality and its association with spatially structured division of labor. PLoS One 9:e079616. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasquier, G., and Grüter, C. (2016). Individual learning performance and exploratory activity are linked to colony foraging success in a mass-recruiting ant. Behav. Ecol. 27:arw079. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, M., Rolland, U., Giurfa, M., and d’Ettorre, P. (2013). Sucrose responsiveness, learning success, and task specialization in ants. Learn. Mem. 20, 417–420. doi: 10.1101/lm.031427.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinter-Wollman, N. (2012). Personality in social insects: how does worker personality determine colony personality? Curr. Zool. 58, 580–588.

Google Scholar

Pinter-Wollman, N., Gordon, D. M., and Holmes, S. (2012). Nest site and weather affect the personality of harvester ant colonies. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1022–1029. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ars066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, S. C., Brooks, S. E., and Franks, N. R. (2001). The use of edges in visual navigation by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Ethology 107, 1125–1136.

Google Scholar

Ravary, F., Lecoutey, E., Kaminski, G., Châline, N., and Jaisson, P. (2007). Individual experience alone can generate lasting division of labor in ants. Curr. Biol. 17, 1308–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2008). Experimental paradigms for studying cognition and communication in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol. News 11, 201–214.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Ryabko, B. (2011). Numerical competence in animals, with an insight from ants. Behaviour 148, 405–434. doi: 10.1163/000579511X568562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, T. O., Sleeman, P. A., McNamara, J. M., Houston, A. I., and Franks, N. R. (2007). Teaching with evaluation in ants. Curr. Biol. 17, 1520–1526. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E., Jackson, D. E., Holcombe, M., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2005). “No entry” signal in ant foraging. Nature 438:442.

Google Scholar

Robinson, E. J. H., Feinerman, O., and Franks, N. R. (2009a). Flexible task allocation and the organization of work in ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 4373–4380. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E. J. H., Richardson, T. O., Sendova-Franks, A. B., Feinerman, O., and Franks, N. R. (2009b). Radio tagging reveals the roles of corpulence, experience and social information in ant decision making. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 627–636. doi: 10.1007/s00265-008-0696-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih, A., Cote, J., Evans, M., Fogarty, S., and Pruitt, J. (2012). Ecological implications of behavioural syndromes. Ecol. Lett. 15, 278–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01731.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih, A., and del Giudice, M. (2012). Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: a behavioural ecology perspective. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2762–2772. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanner, C. J. (2009). Individual experience-based foraging can generate community territorial structure for competing ant species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 591–603. doi: 10.1007/s00265-008-0694-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Udino, E., Perez, M., Carere, C., and d’Ettorre, P. (2017). Active explorers show low learning performance in a social insect. Curr. Zool. 63, 555–560. doi: 10.1093/cz/zow101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilela, E. F., Jaffé, K., and Howse, P. E. (1987). Orientation in leaf-cutting ants (Formicidae: Attini). Anim. Behav. 35, 1443–1453. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80017-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Thienen, W., Metzler, D., and Witte, V. (2016). How memory and motivation modulate the responses to trail pheromones in three ant species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 393–407. doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2059-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, D., Tissot, M., and Gordon, D. (2001). Task-related environment alters the cuticular hydrocarbon composition of harvester ants. J. Chem. Ecol. 27, 1805–1819. doi: 10.1023/A:1010408725464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, M. M., and Ward, A. J. W. (2011). Personality and social context. Biol. Rev. 2, 759–773. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00169.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1962). Chemical communication among workers of the fire ant Solenopsis saevissima (Fr. Smith) 1. The organization of mass-foraging. Anim. Behav. 10, 134–147. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90141-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, V., and Maschwitz, U. (2002). Coordination of raiding and emigration in the ponerine army ant Leptogenys distinguenda (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Ponerinae): a signal analysis. J. Insect Behav. 15, 195–217. doi: 10.1023/A:1015484917019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, M., and Weissing, F. J. (2010). An explanatory framework for adaptive personality differences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 3959–3968. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: behavioral plasticity, collective behavior, foraging, recruitment, social insects

Citation: Kolay S, Boulay R and d’Ettorre P (2020) Regulation of Ant Foraging: A Review of the Role of Information Use and Personality. Front. Psychol. 11:734. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00734

Received: 10 December 2019; Accepted: 25 March 2020;
Published: 28 April 2020.

Edited by:

Mathieu Lihoreau, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Reviewed by:

Tomer J. Czaczkes, University of Regensburg, Germany
Zhanna Reznikova, Institute of Systematics and Ecology of Animals (RAS), Russia

Copyright © 2020 Kolay, Boulay and d’Ettorre. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Swetashree Kolay, kolay@leec.univ-paris13.fr

Deceased

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.