Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 16 September 2019
Sec. Eating Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic New Perspectives to Unlock the Current Impasse in Treating Anorexia Nervosa View all 9 articles

Verifying Feighner’s Hypothesis; Anorexia Nervosa Is Not a Psychiatric Disorder

  • Karolinska Institutet, Mandometer Clinics, Huddinge, Sweden

Mental causation takes explanatory priority over evolutionary biology in most accounts of eating disorders. The evolutionary threat of starvation has produced a brain that assists us in the search for food and mental change emerges as a consequence. The major mental causation hypothesis: anxiety causes eating disorders, has been extensively tested and falsified. The subsidiary hypothesis: anxiety and eating disorders are caused by the same genotype, generates inconsistent results because the phenotypes are not traits, but vary along dimensions. Challenging the mental causation hypothesis in Feighner et al. (1972) noted that anorexic patients are physically hyperactive, hoarding for food, and they are rewarded for maintaining a low body weight. In 1996, Feighner’s hypothesis was formalized, relating the patients’ behavioral phenotype to the brain mechanisms of reward and attention (Bergh and Södersten, 1996), and in 2002, the hypothesis was clinically verified by training patients how to eat normally, thus improving outcomes (Bergh et al., 2002). Seventeen years later we provide evidence supporting Feighner’s hypothesis by demonstrating that in 2012, 20 out of 37 patients who were referred by a psychiatrist, had a psychiatric diagnosis that differed from the diagnosis indicated by the SCID-I. Out of the 174 patients who were admitted in 2012, most through self-referral, there was significant disagreement between the outcomes of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem in 110 of the cases. In addition, 358 anorexic patients treated to remission scored high on the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale, but an item response analysis indicated one (unknown) underlying dimension, rather than the three dimensions the scale can dissociate in patients with psychiatric disorders. These results indicate that psychiatric diagnoses, which are reliable and valid in patients with psychiatric disorders, are less well suited for patients with anorexia. The results are in accord with the hypothesis of the present Research Topic, that eating disorders are not always caused by disturbed psychological processes, and support the alternative, clinically relevant hypothesis that the behavioral phenotype of the patients should be addressed directly.

Introduction

Scientific advances often come from uncovering a hitherto unseen aspect of things as a result, not so much of using some new instrument, but rather of looking at objects from a different angle (Jacob, 1977)

A large population-based twin study in Finland reported that most women diagnosed with anorexia nervosa recovered clinically within 5 years and thereafter progressed toward full recovery (Keski-Rahkonen et al., 2007). This interesting study should be revisited, because several reviews of the outcome in anorexia nervosa have concluded the opposite, i.e., although patients may experience symptomatic relief in the short term, remission is uncommon, many patients relapse within a year of discharge, and outcome in the longer term is poor or unknown (Eckert et al., 1995; Ben-Tovim et al., 2001; Zipfel et al., 2015; Södersten et al., 2017; Fisher et al., 2018; Zeeck et al., 2018). Hence, the purpose of this Research Topic, to break the impasse, is appropriate. As the introductory quote from Francois Jacob suggests, it may be useful to view anorexia from a different angle rather than relying on the hypothetical cause-effect relationships that underlie standards of care.

A possible explanation for the limited success of presently used standard treatments was provided 47 years ago when Feighner et al. suggested that anorexia is not caused by a mental disorder (Feighner et al., 1972), questioning the assumption that underlies standards of care (Zipfel et al., 2015; Zeeck et al., 2018). The mental causation hypothesis is expressed in a position paper from the Academy of for Eating Disorders: “eating disorders are … serious mental illnesses … caused by neurobiological disorder(s) of the brain … Anxiety disorders often predate the onset of [anorexia nervosa]” (Klump et al., 2009). It seems likely that challenging the mental causation hypothesis made Feighner’s hypothesis a “sleeping beauty,” i.e., “a paper whose importance is not recognized for several years after publication” (Ke et al., 2015). We suggest that it is time to wake up Feighner’s hypothesis.

This report will present an updated biological theory of the relationship among the mind, starvation and the brain. Outcomes of tests of the mental causation hypothesis are reviewed and original clinical data are presented that support Feighner’s hypothesis. A 23 years old framework for an improved treatment is then re-launched, including a hypothesis on how the brain mediates among eating behavior and mental symptoms.

Biological Theory

Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution (Dobzhansky, 1973)

Forty years after Dobzhansky stated the importance of evolution for understanding biology, most scientists agreed, but this perspective has been neglected outside the biological sciences (Striedter et al., 2012). And most accounts of anorexia nervosa still do not consider the evolutionary significance of starvation. This is surprising because evolutionary psychology and psychiatry are now long established fields (Varga, 2012; Nesse, 2019).

Evolution and the Mind

Humans offer mental causes, including intentionality and agency, to explain purposeful behavior (Bloom, 2008, 2012), i.e., “In many populations with Western European roots, the prevailing belief is that minds and mental states cause behaviors” (McNamara et al., 2019). Over the course of evolution, the psychological process of mental causation, i.e., dualism, developed in parallel with the cognitive systems that are necessary for understanding of the physical properties of our environment (Bloom, 2012). At the psychological level, the idea that everything has a physical basis [for a discussion of physicalism see Stoljar (2017)] normally co-exists with dualism [for a discussion of dualism see Robinson (2017)], but the two are separable, distinct processes (Bloom, 2012).

Bloom suggested that “we are born dualists. we naturally believe in an immaterial soul, in spirits …,” and significantly that we are dualists “for clear adaptive reasons” (Bloom, 2012). Earlier, Darwin raised this possibility by suggesting that “mental powers” emerged over the course of evolution (Jacyna, 2009). Now it has been demonstrated that these dualistic psychological processes have played a significant role in between-group competition and in within-group co-operation and thus in the emergence of shared systems of belief, such as religions; that is to say, dualism and mental causation have facilitated cultural evolution (Norenzayan et al., 2016; Purzycki et al., 2016; Lang et al., 2019). Interestingly, psychological processes of mental causation can emerge after within-group co-operation and engage neural substrates that can be dissociated from the neural substrates engaged in analytical thinking, the two are in competition (Jack et al., 2016; Whitehouse et al., 2019).

However, mental causation is a feeling or an illusion, rather than an explanation; one is reminded of the behaviorist comment: “we talk about free will and choice when we know about behavior but not its cause” (Skinner, 1977; Wegner and Wheatley, 1999; Wegner, 2003, 2004, 2018). Yet, systems of belief, including false beliefs, are often resistant to change even in the face of contradictory scientific evidence, and retrospective examination of the “facts” may be ineffective and can even strengthen the false belief (Lewandowsky et al., 2012; Fazio et al., 2015; Yousif et al., 2019). Today, research on “fake news” has disclosed some of the factors involved, including political and economic interests, denialist campaigns, shared values, maintenance of social networks and so forth (Jost et al., 2018; Lewandowsky et al., 2019). Nevertheless critical examination of the available data is essential for progress in science, for the translation of science into clinical practice, and for guiding public health policies (Jacob, 1977).

Evolution and Starvation, a Brain Designed for the Search for Food

Eating and the search for food have dominated human life in feast and famine alike (Södersten et al., 2008). Using an animal model, Epling and Pierce related the increased physical activity that emerges in starvation and the high physical activity of anorexic patients to the search of food (Epling and Pierce, 1984, 1988). An extensive literature on the evolutionary importance of foraging had accumulated at the time (Stephens and Krebs, 1986). This suggestion was novel because it departed from the mental causation hypothesis as recognized by Guisinger (2003), and today evidence has accumulated that an evolutionary perspective can inform our understanding of anorexia as suggested by Epling and Pierce. Thus, the neural pathways that are activated by the shortage of food assist animals in searching for food, rather than eating, in fact foraging for food can decrease food intake (Ammar et al., 2000; Södersten et al., 2008; Schneider et al., 2013). The neuroendocrine cells in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus that are activated by starvation are involved in “shaping (the) behavioral choices” needed for foraging, and these behaviors outcompete most other behavioral choices (Chen et al., 2015; Dietrich et al., 2015; Burnett et al., 2016; Södersten et al., 2019b). The importance of this behavioral shift is obvious, as starving animals and humans must find food. The role of the brain is permissive, not causal (Södersten et al., 2011).

Anorexia Nervosa and the Mind-Body Problem

The observation that reduced food intake provokes increased physical activity was made a hundred years ago in experimental psychology (Richter, 1922). Shortly thereafter, it was demonstrated experimentally that food intake decreases as a consequence of an increase in physical activity (Skinner, 1938). A comprehensive overview of activity-based anorexia, the animal model for anorexia nervosa that emerged from these initial observations was published more recently (Gutierrez, 2013). The importance of the model for the present discussion of mental causation lies in the reports of the mental effects in healthy humans who participated in a starvation experiment (Keys et al., 1950). While physical activity was an experimental variable rather than a measure of outcome in that study, the increase in physical activity in the search for food has been documented in hundreds of thousands of people during enforced starvation (Södersten et al., 2008).

The humans in the starvation experiment were interviewed not long ago and the mental effects they experienced were recently published (Kalm and Semba, 2005; Tucker, 2007; Case, 2018; Eckert et al., 2018; University of Minnesota, 2018). While “food became an obsession for the participants” (Kalm and Semba, 2005), the mental effects came as a surprise:

What I wasn’t expecting was the effect it would have on the mind; the total feeling of, I guess, depression, the total occupation with the idea of food (participant)

Crow interviewed 19 of the 36 subjects long after the experiment and noted that they had all of the mental symptoms that are characteristic of anorexics:

The similarity between these [effects of starvation] and what’s seen in clinical settings treating people with eating disorders is really quite striking. I knew all about this stuff before I really knew very much about what happened in the study just from working on an eating disorders unit (Crow, 2004)

It is also the case that the mental/behavioral effects of starvation are reversible with appropriate re-feeding (Eckert et al., 2018).

Conclusion

The brain mediates eating-behavior responses to food restriction and has evolved to assist the individual in the search for food. Moreover, healthy humans develop the mental symptoms of anorexia nervosa during food restriction because they are starving, but when the same response is seen in anorexia, humans assume a mental causation of those symptoms by default, rather than assuming that the food restriction is causing that response.

The Mental Causation Hypothesis

Anorexia nervosa is a graphic illustration of the influence of emotions on bodily functions (DuBois, 1949)

Mental factors have long been thought to cause anorexia, rather than thinking that food deprivation causes the mental problems. Given the complexities of comprehending the concept of whether one of these two factors is causative, it is helpful to analyze the concept of co-morbidity.

Co-morbidity

It is not clear what is cause and what is effect among mental symptoms and the eating disorder and much of the problem with understanding this issue concerns the concept of co-morbidity. However, the analysis of causation is complex (Hacker, 2010) and there are at least two problems.

First, the original idea of co-morbidity was of co-existing, but independent, disorders (Feinstein, 1970). From this perspective, co-existing psychiatric symptoms and disordered eating behaviors are not causally related. Yet, the concept of co-morbidity has led to the clustering of psychiatric symptoms that are not independent (Maj, 2005), and the mental causation hypothesis assumes that they are related.

Second, mental disorders are thought to be “risk factors” for anorexia. Two definitions of risk are useful: “the probability of an unwanted event which may or may not occur” and the “cause of an unwanted event which may or may not occur” (Hansson, 2018). Regarding the first definition, one needs to determine why a mental disorder increases the probability of experiencing an eating disorder. There is limited, if any, information that clarifies this matter [see discussion in Södersten et al. (2006)]. We therefore will use the second definition, i.e., risk = cause, to examine the evidence for and against the hypothesis that a mental disorder causes anorexia nervosa, the prototypical eating disorder.

We avoid the distinction between cause, mediator, mechanism, and modulator in psychotherapeutic research (Kazdin, 2007; Tryon, 2018). In any analysis, cause differs from mechanism and mediators and modulators are replaced by mechanisms as research progresses.

The OCD Hypothesis

Childhood anxiety represents one important genetically mediated pathway toward the development of anorexia nervosa… (Kaye et al., 2004)

Anorexia was once thought to be a symptom of anxiety, rather than a separate disorder [reviewed in Wu (2008)], but it is now thought that an anxiety disorder, specifically obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), causes anorexia. We will examine the attempts at verifying this version of the mental causation hypothesis, because it uncovers some problems related to the methods used to assess this hypothesis.

Self-Reported OCD in Anorexia Nervosa

Retrospective interviews were first used to examine the possibility that anxiety disorders cause anorexia nervosa. In support of that hypothesis, 60% of 68 women with anorexia nervosa recalled having an anxiety disorder, and 90% were thought to have had both childhood overanxious disorder and social anxiety disorder before they had anorexia nervosa (Bulik et al., 1997). While these anxiety disorders were considered to be non-specific risk factors for psychopathology later in life, it was suggested that OCD is a specific risk factor for anorexia nervosa, although the presence of OCD before anorexia nervosa was not reported (Bulik et al., 1997).

In another study, 42% of 672 patients with eating disorders reported one or more anxiety disorders in childhood and 23% said that they had OCD before they developed anorexia nervosa (Kaye et al., 2004). But re-calculation of the data in their study showed that 7% of the patients had OCD before they had anorexia nervosa (Södersten and Bergh, 2006). Upon commenting on the re-calculation, the authors stated that at least 14% of the patients had OCD before they had anorexia nervosa and that 36% of the patients in an unpublished study had an anxiety disorder before they had anorexia nervosa (Kaye et al., 2006). They then reported that 39.1% of 647 patients with anorexia nervosa had an anxiety disorders and that 94.4% of the patients had the anxiety disorder before they developed anorexia (Raney et al., 2008). But rather than having OCD, that anxiety disorder that preceded anorexia was childhood overanxious disorder, the disorder that previously was thought to be a non-specific risk factor for psychopathologies in adulthood (Bulik et al., 1997). It was also pointed out that childhood overanxious disorder is no longer a recognized psychiatric disorder (Raney et al., 2008) and the hypothesis that it predates anorexia was subsequently falsified (Buckner et al., 2010).

These discrepancies led others to conclude that the OCD hypothesis was not supported by the existing data (Godart et al., 2006; Swinbourne and Touyz, 2007). It was also pointed out that the causal relationship among anxiety and eating disorders might be the opposite, i.e., that anorexia (or starvation) causes anxiety (Micali et al., 2015). This hypothesis was verified by Keys’ study on starvation (O’Brien and Vincent, 2003). Yet, a recent review re-affirmed that anxiety disorders “typically” precede anorexia nervosa (Zipfel et al., 2015).

Registry Studies That Assess the Role of OCD in Eating Disorders

Recalled disorders produce unreliable estimates of the actual experiences in epidemiological studies (Regier et al., 1984), and it should be obvious that self-reported retrospective data make it difficult “to disentangle causes from effect” (Brander et al., 2016). However, the existence of large national data registries in which the dates of onset for different diagnoses are available allows for a more accurate examination of the putative role of OCD in the development of anorexia.

According to the introductory literature review of the first longitudinal registry study as many as 83% of patients with anorexia nervosa can be diagnosed with anxiety disorders, which precede the onset of anorexia nervosa “in most patients” (Meier et al., 2015). But that study subsequently found that 3.5% of 5,065 patients had actually been diagnosed with a specific anxiety disorder before they were diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, and only 1.9% had been diagnosed with OCD before developing anorexia nervosa (Meier et al., 2015). Hence, OCD did not predate anorexia in 98.1% of the patients, precluding the possibility that it was causal in the development of anorexia nervosa. It should also be noted how much lower the documented diagnoses of anxiety and OCD are compared to the self-reported estimates of these disorders.

Despite the evidence, the OCD hypothesis has been maintained by reporting increases in the risk for anorexia among patients diagnosed with OCD compared to healthy subjects in a national registry study. Thus, 0.7% out of 19,069 men and women with OCD had developed anorexia nervosa compared with 0.2% out of 190,690 healthy men and women (Cederlöf et al., 2015). While the relative risk of OCD preceding anorexia was 3.6 times that of those without OCD, it remains the case that 99.3% of OCD patients did not develop anorexia nervosa.

In the same study, 0.6% out of 19,512 men who had an OCD diagnosis also had an anorexia nervosa diagnosis, compared to 0.02% out of 195,120 healthy men. The relative risk of having anorexia nervosa among men with OCD had increased 37 times (Cederlöf et al., 2015). Rather than verifying the hypothesis that OCD causes anorexia, the results actually supported the hypothesis that anorexia causes OCD (O’Brien and Vincent, 2003), because anorexia nervosa before OCD was several-fold more common that OCD before anorexia, although both cause-effect relationships were uncommon (Cederlöf et al., 2015).

But the increased risk is more apparent than real. To illustrate the problem with these calculations: the probability of winning by drawing a number in a lottery is approximately equal to 0. Drawing ten numbers increases the probability of winning ten times, but the probability remains approximately equal to 0.

Conclusions regarding the increased risk for developing an eating disorder following OCD are further confounded by “… the possibility that subtle eating disorder symptoms were overlooked at initial assessment [of OCD]” (Cederlöf et al., 2015), as had been demonstrated in another longitudinal study (Micali et al., 2011). Also, OCD is a heterogeneous disorder in children (Zijlmans et al., 2017), and diagnosis is not reliable (Mataix-Cols et al., 2007; Rück et al., 2015). The recent suggestion that both patients with anorexia and patients with OCD don’t like to make mistakes (Levinson et al., 2018) stands out as a strained attempt at rescuing the OCD hypothesis in the face of accumulating evidence that argue against the hypothesis. Facing the absence of support from a study on approximately 14,000 children, the OCD hypothesis surrendered just a few months ago, although it was replaced by the equally uncertain hypotheses that childhood overanxious disorder or social anxiety cause anorexia (Kerr-Gaffney et al., 2018; Schaumberg et al., 2019).

Genetics and the OCD Hypothesis

While the history of the heredity of psychiatric disorders goes back several hundred years, the scientific framework for understanding it has remained the same, namely that hereditary factors predispose the individual to mental problems and that environmental factors subsequently cause the disorder (Porter, 2018). This is undoubtedly correct, a pioneer of behavioral genetics pointed out long ago that all behavior is the result of an interaction among genes and environment (Benzer, 1971).

Thus, the OCD hypothesis included the subsidiary hypothesis that OCD and anorexia have the same genetic basis (Kaye et al., 2004). However, it was reported that only 0.7% of the patients with OCD develop anorexia nervosa and that “the majority of genetic variance is disorder-specific” (Cederlöf et al., 2015). Paradoxically, a highly significant genetic risk factor for both OCD and anorexia nervosa was then reported in genome-wide-association studies (Brainstorm Consortium, 2018; Yilmaz et al., 2018; Watson et al., 2019). Thus, the subsidiary genetic OCD hypothesis was first falsified and then supported. These contrasting findings may be related to the use of registry data some of the studies versus data from genome-wide association data in the other studies. Note, however, that genome-wide association does not address the question of causation (Pearl, 2019).

Why the OCD Hypothesis Remains Elusive

One reason why tests of the OCD hypothesis have yielded inconsistent results may be because anxiety disorders as well as eating disorders are not distinct categories but vary along overlapping dimensions (Russell, 1979; Bergh et al., 2002; Fairburn et al., 2003; Wu, 2008). For example, an anxious person may have problems with sleep, attention, irritability, concentration, muscle tension, continual arousal, generalized worry, or fear. Thus, a multiple regression analysis using specific anxiety dimensions such as these revealed that OCD did not offer better discriminant validity than “general distress” in predicting eating disorders symptom than symptoms of panic and depression, which have overlapping symptoms with anxiety, questioning a specific OCD-eating disorders relationship (Wu, 2008).

Because attempts at classifying mental disorders into categories based on Mendelian genetics many years ago were unsuccessful, the existence of these categories was questioned (Porter, 2018). This issue was resolved 100 years ago when it was pointed out that with many genes involved, a continuous, normally distributed phenotype should be expected in the population (Fisher, 1918). Recent analyses confirm the involvement of many genes, perhaps the entire genome, in complex traits, including mental disorders (Boyle et al., 2017; Willsey et al., 2018; Wray et al., 2018), confirming the suggestion that psychiatric disorders vary along various symptomatic dimensions (Wu, 2008; Brainstorm Consortium, 2018).

Recent research offers mechanistic explanations for the failure to relate genotype to eating disorders diagnoses. It is long known that cells of different genotypes make up mosaics that constitute the basis for display of behavior (Hotta and Benzer, 1972). Mosaicism was recently demonstrated within brain neurons engaged in the ontogeny and display of behavior (Paquola et al., 2017). This opens for a vast number of gene-neurobiology-environment interactions. Add that behavioral phenotypes can be mediated by different neural networks (Nusbaum et al., 2017), and it will be difficult to verify the subsidiary OCD hypothesis.

The ADHD Hypothesis

it is possible that symptoms of ADHD directly cause [eating disorder] behaviors (Levin and Rawana, 2016)

While the OCD hypothesis has not been tested experimentally, another version of the mental causation hypothesis, the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) hypothesis, has been tested experimentally. Some have argued that research on the ADHD hypothesis has just started (Kaisari et al., 2017), whereas others assume the causal link between ADHD and anorexia nervosa is firmly established (Levin and Rawana, 2016). Not long ago, it was unlikely to have an ADHD diagnosis and anorexia at the same time because the male:female ratio was 80:20 for ADHD and 3:97 for anorexia nervosa (Bao and Swaab, 2011). That has now changed.

The majority of patients diagnosed with ADHD have difficulty paying attention and controlling their impulses (Willcutt, 2012; Willcutt et al., 2012) and women with eating disorders are now diagnosed with ADHD (Seitz et al., 2013; Stulz et al., 2013; Nazar et al., 2016; Svedlund et al., 2017; Yilmaz et al., 2017). Binge eating in bulimia nervosa is thought to reflect impulsivity, along with a lack of response inhibition (Seitz et al., 2013; Bleck et al., 2015; Pearson et al., 2015) and inattention to the physiology of hunger is thought to play a role in anorexia nervosa (Kaye et al., 2009; Yilmaz et al., 2017).

Testing the ADHD Hypothesis

The interview questions and tasks used for estimating and evaluating attention are related mainly to non-food items, including gambling, card sorting and speed of information processing (Rösler et al., 2006; Zastrow et al., 2009; Oltra-Cucarella et al., 2015; Sultson et al., 2016; Tenconi et al., 2016). While these questions may be valid for a diagnosis of ADHD in other subjects, they may be less valid for diagnosing inattention among patients with anorexia nervosa.

Attention is believed to be under top-down control from the prefrontal cortex (Dalley et al., 2011), providing the patients with cognitive control, assisting them in eating only little food. Simultaneously, attenuation of bottom-up control exerted from other brain regions is thought to make the patients inattentive to their starved physiology (Kaye et al., 2009; Brooks et al., 2012a, b). On this framework, one study first noted enhanced activity in the prefrontal cortex in anorexic patients exposed to food items in an fMRI scanner and although bottom-up control was not affected, the patients showed deficiencies in card sorting outside the scanner (Sanders et al., 2015; Sultson et al., 2016). How should these results be interpreted?

As discussed above, the brain has evolved to assist us in searching for food. The activity in the relevant brain neurons outcompetes any activity in neurons engaged in alternative behaviors. Considering that shortage of food, foraging for food, and eating have been the main drivers of evolution (Lieberman, 2011; Ungar, 2017), it would be surprising if someone who had not eaten enough food for a long time, including anorexic patients, was inattentive to food stimuli. Less surprising, a study found that anorexic patients show enhanced attention to food stimuli (Neimeijer et al., 2017), and when examined with a variety of tests of attention, they showed “superior ability to suppress attention to irrelevant information” (Weinbach et al., 2017). Thus, anorexic patients may consider gambling, card sorting and cognitive tests of information processing irrelevant. In fact, activity in their prefrontal cortex may assist them in processing relevant information (Petrovich, 2013; Richard and Berridge, 2013; Ehlers and Todd, 2017).

It is noteworthy that the prefrontal cortex mediates a wide variety of functions, including rational decision making, mentalization, intentionality, and even free will and false beliefs (Denny et al., 2012; Leisman et al., 2012; Funahashi and Andreau, 2013; Mahy et al., 2014; Weele et al., 2018). Even with some compartmentalization within the prefrontal cortex it will be difficult to find a part exclusively devoted to top-down cognitive control of eating.

Genetics and the ADHD Hypothesis

It was recently reported that the genetic correlation among ADHD and anorexia is negative (Brainstorm Consortium, 2018; Demontis et al., 2018), but that ADHD and binge eating are genetically related (Capusan et al., 2017). Given this, it has been suggested that the genetic risk factors for ADHD and eating disorders have not yet been clarified (Willsey et al., 2018) and the most recent reviews do not mention eating disorders among the disorders that co-exist with ADHD (Faraone and Larsson, 2019; Glahn et al., 2019).

Conclusion

The inconsistent outcomes of the many attempts at verifying the mental causation hypothesis have the end result of inadvertently or indirectly supporting Feighner’s hypothesis. Analyses of behavior, cognition and brain function in anorexia nervosa will “make better sense” if they comply with the request for neural plausibility (Crick and Koch, 2003) and if “viewed in the light of evolution” (Dobzhansky, 1973; Jacob, 1977; Striedter et al., 2012). It is surprising that mental disorders, rather that eating behavior, assume priority in most accounts of eating disorders considering that eating behavior has been at the center of animal and human evolution over millennia; chewing is essential for the development of all parts of the head, the brain is not exempt (Lieberman, 2011; Ungar, 2017; Smith, 2018). Keeping this in mind, it seems likely that activities in the brain and other parts of the body assist any starving human, including anorexic patients, in paying attention to food cues. It is suggested that the neglect of the evolutionary perspective rather than methodological and conceptual confusions in neuroimaging research (Donnelly et al., 2018) explains the impasse in the field.

Validating Feighner’s Hypothesis

No known medical illnessor other psychiatric disorder, that could account for the anorexia (Feighner et al., 1972)

The present analysis generates the hypothesis that the diagnosis of mental illness, which is valid and reliable for patients with mental disorders, is less suitable for patients with eating disorders. We offer the following retrospective review of clinical records in support of that hypothesis.

Reliability and Validity of Psychiatric Diagnoses in Eating Disorders

In 2012, 174 patients were admitted to the Mandometer Clinics, most of them through self-referral although some through physician-referral. Following the diagnostic procedures of the clinic (Bergh et al., 2002), 32 were diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, 20 with bulimia nervosa and 122 were diagnosed with an eating disorder not otherwise specified. 37 (21%) of the patients had been referred by a psychiatrist and 20 (54%) of these patients had been diagnosed with a mental disorder, including depression (n = 5), anxiety (n = 5), and ADHD (n = 5).

To examine the reliability and validity of these diagnoses, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) was administered to all 174 patients by clinical staff trained for the purpose (First et al., 1996; Kaye et al., 2004). 120 (69%) of the patients scored high enough to indicate the existence of a psychiatric diagnosis, with anxiety (43.6%) and depression (31.6%) the most common results of the SCID-I interview. However, there was agreement between the diagnosis made by a psychiatrist and the SCID-I results in only 17/37 (46%) cases, indicating that there was actually disagreement, although this effect was not statistically significant (McNemar test, p = 0.26) (Altman, 1990; Table 1). Note that although the SCID-I interview appears sensitive, 26/37 patients (70%) scored high, it failed to confirm the diagnosis made by a psychiatrist in 7/20 (35%) of the cases (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of patients with a psychiatric diagnosis on admission and number of patients with a psychiatric diagnosis indicated by the SCID-I interview.

When the 174 patients were given the opportunity to see a psychiatrist, 60 patients (34%) opted for psychiatric care. However, there was agreement between the outcome of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem in only 64 (37%) of the cases (Table 2). There was significant disagreement between the outcome of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem (McNemar test, p < 0.001) (Altman, 1990).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Number of patients with a psychiatric diagnosis indicated by the SCID-I interview and number of patients taking the chance to visit a psychiatrist.

Are Psychiatric Symptoms Among Anorexic Patients Truly Psychiatric Symptoms?

Between 1993 and 2017, 358 patients with anorexia nervosa were treated to remission at the Mandometer Clinics in a median (quartile range) of 18.5 (10–23.2) months. At admission and remission, they completed the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS), that has been demonstrated reliable and valid for estimating OCD, anxiety and depression in patients with these disorders (Svanborg and Åsberg, 1994; Mattila-Evenden et al., 1996; Hanlon et al., 2008).

Figure 1 shows that the patients rated the level of these symptoms high at admission and low at remission. Please note that the levels of OCD, anxiety and depression were similar on both occasions and that some patients rated their symptoms low at admission and some rated their symptoms high at remission. The similarities in the ratings of OCD, anxiety and depression are verified by the high correlations (Spearman rank correlation) between these symptoms at admission (Figures 24).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Levels of OCD, anxiety (ANX) and depression (DEP) in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa at admission and at remission determined with the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among anxiety and OCD in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among depression and OCD in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among depression and anxiety in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

The CPRS as used for the diagnosis of eating disorders includes 19 items, some of which are specific for OCD, anxiety and depression and some are overlapping between these three domains (Figure 5). A nonparametric Mokken scalability analysis (Sijtsma and Molenaar, 2002) showed that all items contributed to just one underlying variable (all scalabilities except one were >0.4). One item (scalability = 0.2) indicated more noice than information. A good contributing item should show a scalability >0.3. The result is not surprising as there is a set of items thought to contribute to more than one domain. A subsequent automated item selection procedure (Sijtsma and Molenaar, 2002) with the domain overlapping items excluded, should reveal the three intended domains, but the analysis confirmed the first finding of just one underlying domain, with no evidence of any underlying secondary domain. A parametric Rasch model (Sijtsma and Molenaar, 2002) yielded the same result, one dominating domain with high person – item reliabilities of 0.91 and 0.98, and with two items indicating more noice than information. A principal component analysis of the standardized residuals could not find any additional domain (1st and 2nd contrast <2).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Domain specific and overlapping items for anxiety, OCD and depression in the 19 items of the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale.

It is noteworthy that at admission, 78 of the patients had been prescribed at least one psychoactive drug, 46 were given an antidepressant, 24 were given an anxiolytic, and 25 were given a neuroleptic, alone or in combination. Their time to remission was 16.7 (10.4–16) months. In our clinics, psychoactive drugs are gradually reduced as much as possible as part of our treatment, and at remission 7 patients were still taking an antidepressant, 7 were taking an anxiolytic, and 12 were taking a neuroleptic. The CPRS ratings for patients taking psychoactive drugs were as high as for the other patients at admission, and as low as the other patients at remission (data not shown separately), indicating that they appeared to make no difference in the outcomes either of the treatment for eating disorders, or in the outcomes for their psychiatric symptoms.

Discussion and Conclusion

These data bring into question whether the diagnosis of psychiatric symptoms, using the procedures that have been proven effective for patients with psychiatric disorders (Drill et al., 2015; Huprich et al., 2015; Hutsebaut et al., 2016) are helpful in treating patients with anorexia nervosa. The domain-specific items of the CPRS failed to dissociate OCD, anxiety and depression in 358 anorexic patients and instead pointed to one domain, thereby raising the question about what the underlying variable might be. Could it be that the single issue that triggers these symptoms is starvation? It is tempting to speculate that the symptoms assessed by the CPRS do not reflect typical symptoms for OCD, anxiety or depression that is seen in anorexic patients. Indeed, the ineffectiveness of psychoactive drugs targeting these symptoms in anorexic patients is well known (Fineberg et al., 2015). It might be significant in this context that mental symptoms associated with eating disorders are inconsistent over time, identifying some of these symptoms at any particular time may be difficult, as they may be remitting and relapsing over time, rather than running a chronic course (Anckarsäter et al., 2012). Equally significant, by practicing how to eat normally, patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders as well remit from psychiatric symptoms when psychoactive drugs prescribed for treating the same symptoms are withdrawn (Bergh et al., 2002, 2013).

It is concluded that the mental symptoms are effects of eating disorders.

Summary

Very few patients with anxiety develop anorexia nervosa but it is common that starving anorexia patients exhibit anxiety. The risk of developing anorexia nervosa among patients with mental disorders has been overestimated by calculating relative ratios among low probabilities. Reports on inattention in patients with eating disorders are based on tests that may be irrelevant for someone who is starving. When relevant tests are used, anorexic patients show increased attention. There are no convincing data relating the genetics of psychiatric disorders with the genetics of anorexia. All the failed attempts at supporting the mental causation hypothesis, including its subsidiary genetic hypothesis, can be interpreted as supporting Feighner’s hypothesis that the starvation of anorexia causes its mental symptoms.

A word of caution seems appropriate. Thus, most of the literature reviewed is descriptive, putting constraints on cause-effect analyses (Pearl, 2019). While some experiments have tested the hypothesis that cognitive processes maintain the psychopathology of patients with eating disorders, the results are inconclusive (Södersten et al., 2017). Yet, it remains possible that a psychopathology turns out to be a cause of anorexia.

Psychiatric diagnostic procedures appear to be better suited for patients with psychiatric disorders than for patients with anorexia nervosa and perhaps other eating disorders as well. If psychiatric illness does not cause eating disorders we need to consider alternatives such as that starvation causes anxiety, depression and obsession. If this is the case, then we can assume that starvation is the critical problem that needs to be addressed. If we look at this as a behavioral issue and take an evolutionary perspective to understanding the behavior, we can develop treatments that may target the problem. For this purpose a theoretical framework is useful.

Framework

A simple, parsimonious hypothesis is that self-starvation is initially rewarding and subsequently controlled by conditioning to previously neutral stimuli (Bergh and Södersten, 1996)

Feighner et al. (1972) noted that anorexic patients are physically hyperactive and that they are rewarded for maintaining a low body weight. In 1996, Feighner’s hypothesis was formalized, relating the patients’ behavioral phenotype to the brain mechanisms of reward and attention (Bergh and Södersten, 1996). In the context of the present Research Topic, and in the spirit of Occam’s razor, i.e., leaving what is unimportant in the standards of care for eating disorders without notice (Gutiérrez and Carrera, 2018), we would like to re-launch the 23 year old hypothesis cited in the quote. It is actually a framework rather than a hypothesis. Crick and Koch have explained what the term “framework” means in an analysis that we have adapted to understanding eating disorders (Crick and Koch, 2003; Zandian et al., 2007). For example, a framework should be plausible relative to available evolutionary and neurobiological data (Crick and Koch, 2003).

It is helpful to consult Guisinger once more. Thus, she stressed the distinction between proximate (physiological) and ultimate (evolutionary) causes of anorexia (Guisinger, 2003). On the present framework, the ultimate cause of anorexia is the lack of food, that has plagued human evolution, and the proximate cause is the associated neuroendocrinology of reward and attention and of foraging for food. However, “proximate cause” equals “mechanism” and mechanisms are distinct from causes as noted in the last paragraph of section “Co-morbidity.”

Today, women (and some men as well) diet to avoid becoming overweight and Guisinger suggested that “psychological factors” might make some individuals more prone to dieting than others (Guisinger, 2003). However, one of the best studied of these psychological factors, restrained eating, is similar to the mental symptoms discussed in this overview in that it is more likely an effect than a cause of disordered eating behavior (Zandian et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2012).

On the hypothesis that eating disorders are behavioral issues that should be understood on an evolutionary perspective, our framework has been translated into clinical practice over 25 years, the theoretical basis, the treatment, and its outcomes have been described in detail elsewhere (Bergh et al., 2002, 2013, Södersten et al., 2008, 2014, 2017, 2019a). In brief, a reduction in food intake causes release of dopamine in the forebrain terminals of the mesolimbic dopamine neurons concerned with reward, encouraging the patient to continue dieting (Bergh and Södersten, 1996). Hence, dieting “causes” anorexia nervosa via dopamine release in the ventral striatal terminals of the neural network of “reward.” The hypothesized rewarding role of dopamine has now been confirmed (Södersten et al., 2016). In addition, it was originally hypothesized that because dieting also activates the brainstem noradrenalin neurons concerned with attention, anorexia is maintained by conditioning to the situations which provide reward (Bergh and Södersten, 1996). This long established line of research has been confirmed and extended in detail over the years (ibib).

The main intervention, teaching patients how to eat using real time visual feedback on how to eat and how much food to eat during the meal, was recently published in a video (Esfandiari et al., 2018). In addition, the mechanism whereby eating behavior affects mental symptoms (although the nature of these symptoms needs to be determined), has been outlined engaging brainstem relays for chewing as parts of a neural network that connects to limbic forebrain areas, including the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex (Ioakimidis et al., 2011).

Hypothesis

You are how you eat (Zandian, 2009; Lieberman, 2011)

Chewing is at the heart of human evolution, promoting brain development and many aspects of physical and mental health (Lieberman, 2011; Kumar et al., 2018). In addition, chewing exerts an anxiolytic effect (Hollingworth, 1939; Ioakimidis et al., 2011). Thermoregulation also played a key role in evolution (Jastroch et al., 2018), warmth promotes health (Heinonen and Laukkanen, 2018) and recovery from activity-based anorexia in experimental animals (Carrera and Gutiérrez, 2018). In addition, warmth exerts an anxiolytic effect in anorexic patients (Zandian et al., 2017) and warmth has been used to treat anorexic patients over 145 years (Gull, 1874; Bergh et al., 2002; Carrera and Gutiérrez, 2018).

Both chewing and warmth exert their anxiolytic effect by engaging the serotonin neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus in the brainstem that project to the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex (Lowry et al., 2009; Ioakimidis et al., 2011). These limbic brain regions are influenced by chewing within 30 min (Svensson et al., 2006), the time it takes for warmth to reduce anxiety (Zandian et al., 2017). This limbic cortical plasticity may extend to striatal dopamine neurons, which are also essential for mental health (Kita and Kita, 2011). The hypothesis emerges that chewing and warmth exert their anxiolytic effect by engaging the prefrontal cortex.

Testing the Hypothesis

Transcortical magnetic stimulation and recording of evoked motor potentials in peripheral muscles (Kumar et al., 2018) combined with chewing and standard methods for examining anxiety and administering heat in the clinic (Zandian et al., 2017) make testing of the hypothesis feasible.

Conclusion

the underlying assumption that [anorexia nervosa] is always caused by disturbed psychological processes may not always be true (This Research Topic)

The mental causation hypothesis, formalized as the OCD- and ADHD-hypothesis, has not been supported possibly because mental disorders vary along continuous dimensions and cannot be divided into discrete categories. This was realized 100 years ago (Fisher, 1918) and is now re-realized (Wu, 2008; Insel et al., 2010; Adam, 2013; Gillan et al., 2017; Brainstorm Consortium, 2018).

Biological hypotheses make sense only if viewed in the light of evolution, the mental causation hypothesis cannot be supported when examined from an evolutionary perspective. The commonly held assumption that individuals with anorexia do not pay attention to their physiological signals is undermined when one takes into account the broader impact of starvation. When appropriate stimuli are used, individuals with anorexia pay more, rather than less, attention to hunger cues. Starvation was the major evolutionary threat and humans have evolved to eat large meals whenever offered the chance, not as a result of lack of response inhibition. The high level of physical activity, the difficulty in obtaining food, and the constant low body weight exemplify the meaning of the term “homeostasis” as this concept emerged from the ideas of Bernard subsequently tested by Cannon (Södersten et al., 2006, 2008, 2011).

Interestingly, it was recently suggested that the genes related to anxiety were adaptively selected for at a crucial period of human development, during human migration “Out-of-Africa” about 100, 000 years ago, and have been maintained in the population (Sato and Kawata, 2018). Furthermore, the same genotype may have offered reproductive advantage over the course of evolution (Alvergne et al., 2010).

We have pointed out the inconsistencies in the mental causation hypothesis continuously since 1996 and in line with the present Research Topic we have offered an alternative framework and translated it into clinical practice over the years. The main intervention of our treatment is to teach patients to eat normally, because “eating disorders are eating disorders,” not mental disorders. As noted repeatedly in the present account, eating behavior, chewing in particular, has been of major importance in evolution (Lieberman, 2011; Ungar, 2017; Smith, 2018), and it is unsurprising that disordered eating is associated with health problems. Interestingly, this framework has been extended to the general hypothesis that inappropriate use of our muscles is a major cause of several health problems (Lieberman, 2014).

The hypothesis offered here is but one of several possible hypotheses that emerges from our framework and aims at establishing the link among eating behavior, the brain, and psychological epiphenomena. Obviously, another question comes to mind: if psychiatric symptoms in patients with mental disorders are different from the “psychiatric symptoms” of anorexic patients, what is the dimension(s) underlying these symptoms in anorexia nervosa?

Personal Insights and Opinions

Normalization of eating behavior was demonstrated to be effective in a pragmatic randomized controlled trial (Bergh et al., 2002) and treatment outcomes were subsequently reported for 1,428 patients, including all eating disorders diagnoses, from six clinics in four countries (Bergh et al., 2013). The outcomes of this treatment was then shown to be better than the outcomes of cognitive behavioral therapy for the treatment of eating disorders, the treatment that is thought to rest on the best evidence (Södersten et al., 2017). A recent analysis of the data in the National Quality Registry for the Treatment of Eating Disorders in Sweden found that outcomes of this eating-normalization treatment has been better over several years than the outcomes at the other clinics in Sweden (Södersten et al., 2019a). The differences in outcomes were not the result of a difference in patient characteristics at admission. In fact, our patients are more severely ill than patients admitted to other clinics (Södersten et al., 2016).

The success of our treatment in comparison to that in the published literature supports the tenets upon which it is built/was developed. Clearly, it is time to reconsider the mental causality hypothesis and consider eating behavior and its underlying evolutionary adaptiveness. Once we focus on the effects of starvation on eating behavior, the treatment of anorexia and other eating disorders can be improved. Interestingly, the first step in cognitive behavioral therapy for eating disorders is similar in that “the prescription of a pattern of regular eating (italics in original) is probably the single most effective procedure in the treatment,” which “in the great majority of cases results in a marked … decrease in the level of general psychiatric symptoms” (Fairburn et al., 1993).

Ethics Statement

The analysis of the data in the clinical files, i.e., the registry of the Mandometer Clinics (point 4), was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of Stockholm. The data has been collected since 1993 and is continuously collected. The patients in the registry did not participate in the analysis. All patients entering treatment are informed verbally and in writing that their data might be used in research and if their data are used in research it will be anonymized. Written consent by the patients is not required for analysis of data collected in registries over long periods of time. The patients are also informed that they can request that their data is not used and that they can leave the treatment any time without giving a reason.

Author Contributions

PS, MZ, and CB collected the data. UB performed the statistical analyses. CB supervised the clinical work since 1993. All authors conceived the idea of the manuscript, analyzed the data, reviewed the several versions of the manuscript, and approved the final version of the manuscript. PS wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Mando Group AB.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Stephan Lewandowsky for providing the supporting information.

References

Adam, D. (2013). Mental health: on the spectrum. Nature 496, 416–418.

Google Scholar

Altman, D. G. (1990). Practical Statistics for Medical Research. London: Chapman & Hall.

Google Scholar

Alvergne, A., Jokela, M., and Lummaa, V. (2010). Personality and reproductive success in a high-fertility human population. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 11745–11750. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001752107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ammar, A. A., Sederholm, F., Saito, T. R., Scheurink, A. J., Johnson, A. E., and Södersten, P. (2000). NPY-leptin: opposing effects on appetitive and consummatory ingestive behavior and sexual behavior. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 278, R1627–R1633.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Anckarsäter, H., Hofvander, B., Billstedt, E., Gillberg, I. C., Gillberg, C., Wentz, E., et al. (2012). The sociocommunicative deficit subgroup in anorexia nervosa: autism spectrum disorders and neurocognition in a community-based, longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 42, 1957–1967. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, A.-M., and Swaab, D. F. (2011). Sexual differentiation of the human brain: relation to gender identity, sexual orientation and neuropsychiatric disorders. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 32:214–226. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Tovim, D. I., Walker, K., Gilchrist, P., Freeman, R., Kalucy, R., and Esterman, A. (2001). Outcome in patients with eating disorders: a 5-year study. Lancet Lond. Engl. 357, 1254–1257. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)04406-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benzer, S. (1971). From the gene to behavior. JAMA 218, 1015–1022.

Google Scholar

Bergh, C., Brodin, U., Lindberg, G., and Södersten, P. (2002). Randomized controlled trial of a treatment for anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 9486–9491.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bergh, C., Callmar, M., Danemar, S., Hölcke, M., Isberg, S., Leon, M., et al. (2013). Effective treatment of eating disorders: results at multiple sites. Behav. Neurosci. 127, 878–889.

Google Scholar

Bergh, C., and Södersten, P. (1996). Anorexia nervosa, self-starvation and the reward of stress. Nat. Med. 2, 21–22. doi: 10.1038/nm0196-21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleck, J. R., DeBate, R. D., and Olivardia, R. (2015). The comorbidity of ADHD and eating disorders in a nationally representative sample. J. Behav. Health Serv. Res. 42, 437–451. doi: 10.1007/s11414-014-9422-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloom, P. (2008). This is Your Brain. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vg01Q1BI4WM (accessed September 3, 2019).

Google Scholar

Bloom, P. (2012). Religion, morality, evolution. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 179–199. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, E. A., Li, Y. I., and Pritchard, J. K. (2017). An expanded view of complex traits: from polygenic to omnigenic. Cell 169, 1177–1186. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainstorm Consortium. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360:eaa8757. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brander, G., Pérez-Vigil, A., Larsson, H., and Mataix-Cols, D. (2016). Systematic review of environmental risk factors for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a proposed roadmap from association to causation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 65, 36–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. J., O’Daly, O., Uher, R., Friederich, H.-C., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M., et al. (2012a). Thinking about eating food activates visual cortex with reduced bilateral cerebellar activation in females with anorexia nervosa: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e34000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. J., Rask-Andersen, M., Benedict, C., and Schiöth, H. B. (2012b). A debate on current eating disorder diagnoses in light of neurobiological findings: is it time for a spectrum model? BMC Psychiatry 12:76. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, J. D., Silgado, J., and Lewinsohn, P. M. (2010). Delineation of differential temporal relations between specific eating and anxiety disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 781–787. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Fear, J. L., and Joyce, P. R. (1997). Eating disorders and antecedent anxiety disorders: a controlled study. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 96, 101–107. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09913.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnett, C. J., Li, C., Webber, E., Tsaousidou, E., Xue, S. Y., Brüning, J. C., et al. (2016). Hunger-driven motivational state competition. Neuron 92, 187–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capusan, A. J., Yao, S., Kuja-Halkola, R., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L. M., Bendtsen, P., et al. (2017). Genetic and environmental aspects in the association between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and binge-eating behavior in adults: a twin study. Psychol. Med. 47, 2866–2878. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrera, O., and Gutiérrez, E. (2018). Hyperactivity in anorexia nervosa: to warm or not to warm. That is the question (a translational research one). J. Eat. Disord. 6:4. doi: 10.1186/s40337-018-0190-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Case, D. (2018). The Minnesota Starvation Experiment: Starving so that Others can be Better Fed. Avaliable at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=uq62Bgo_AJotttttt

Google Scholar

Cederlöf, M., Thornton, L. M., Baker, J., Lichtenstein, P., Larsson, H., Rück, C., et al. (2015). Etiological overlap between obsessive-compulsive disorder and anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal cohort, multigenerational family and twin study. World Psychiatry Off. J. World Psychiatr. Assoc. 14, 333–338. doi: 10.1002/wps.20251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Lin, Y.-C., Kuo, T.-W., and Knight, Z. A. (2015). Sensory detection of food rapidly modulates arcuate feeding circuits. Cell 160, 829–841. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, F., and Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 119–126.

Google Scholar

Crow, S. (2004). Science and philosophy in the Minnesota experiment. Paper presented at the First International Ancel Keys Symposium on Nutrition and Health, Minneapolis, MA.

Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., et al. (2018). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 51, 63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self- and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1742–1752. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietrich, M. O., Zimmer, M. R., Bober, J., and Horvath, T. L. (2015). Hypothalamic Agrp neurons drive stereotypic behaviors beyond feeding. Cell 160, 1222–1232. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am. Biol. Teach. 35, 125–129. doi: 10.2307/4444260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnelly, B., Touyz, S., Hay, P., Burton, A., Russell, J., and Caterson, I. (2018). Neuroimaging in bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder: a systematic review. J. Eat. Disord. 6:3. doi: 10.1186/s40337-018-0187-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drill, R., Nakash, O., DeFife, J. A., and Westen, D. (2015). Assessment of clinical information: comparison of the validity of a structured clinical interview (the SCID) and the clinical diagnostic interview. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 203, 459–462. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuBois, F. S. (1949). Compulsion neurosis with cachexia (anorexia nervosa). Am. J. Psychiatry 106, 107–115. doi: 10.1176/ajp.106.2.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckert, E. D., Gottesman, I. I., Swigart, S. E., and Casper, R. C. (2018). A 54-year follow-up investigation and review of the Minnesota study on human starvation and its relevance to eating disorders. Arch. Psychol. 2, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Eckert, E. D., Halmi, K. A., Marchi, P., Grove, W., and Crosby, R. (1995). Ten-year follow-up of anorexia nervosa: clinical course and outcome. Psychol. Med. 25, 143–156. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700028166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, M. R., and Todd, R. M. (2017). Genesis and maintenance of attentional biases: the role of the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system. Neural Plast. 2017:6817349. doi: 10.1155/2017/6817349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epling, W. F., and Pierce, W. D. (1984). Activity-based anorexia in rats as a function of opportunity to run on activity wheel. Nutr. Behav. 2, 37–39.

Google Scholar

Epling, W. F., and Pierce, W. D. (1988). Activity-based anorexia: a biobehavioral perspective. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 7, 475–485. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(198807)7:4<475::aid-eat2260070405>3.0.co;2-m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esfandiari, M., Papapanagiotou, V., Diou, C., Zandian, M., Nolstam, J., and Södersten, P., et al. (2018). Control of eating behavior using a novel feedback system. J. Vis. Exp. 135:e57432. doi: 10.3791/57432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., and Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 509–528. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00088-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairburn, C. G., Marcus, M. D., and Wilson, G. T. (1993). “Cognitive-behavioral therapy for binge eating and bulimia nervosa: a comprehensive treatment manual,” in Binge Eating, Nature, Assessment and Treatment, eds C. G. Fairburn and G. T. Wilson (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 361–404.

Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Larsson, H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 562–575. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N. M., Payne, B. K., and Marsh, E. J. (2015). Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144, 993–1002. doi: 10.1037/xge0000098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G., and Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 26, 57–63.

Google Scholar

Feinstein, A. R. (1970). The pre-therapeutic classification of co-morbidity in chronic disease. J. Chronic Dis. 23, 455–468. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(70)90054-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fineberg, N. A., Reghunandanan, S., Simpson, H. B., Phillips, K. A., Richter, M. A., Matthews, K., et al. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): practical strategies for pharmacological and somatic treatment in adults. Psychiatry Res. 227, 114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and William, J. B. W. (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Research Version (SCID-I). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Google Scholar

Fisher, C. A., Skocic, S., Rutherford, K. A., and Hetrick, S. E. (2018). Family therapy approaches for anorexia nervosa. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 10:CD004780. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004780.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Trans R Soc Edinb 52, 399–433. doi: 10.1017/s0080456800012163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funahashi, S., and Andreau, J. M. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and neural mechanisms of executive function. J. Physiol. Paris 107, 471–482. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Fineberg, N. A., and Robbins, T. W. (2017). A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol. Med. 47, 1528–1548. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glahn, D. C., Nimgaonkar, V. L., Raventós, H., Contreras, J., McIntosh, A. M., Thomson, P. A., et al. (2019). Rediscovering the value of families for psychiatric genetics research. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 523–535. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0073-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godart, N., Berthoz, S., Perdereau, F., and Jeammet, P. (2006). Comorbidity of anxiety with eating disorders and OCD. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 326–326. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guisinger, S. (2003). Adapted to flee famine: adding an evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Rev. 110, 745–761. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.4.745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gull, W. (1874). Anorexia nervosa (apepsia hysterica, anorexia hysterica). Trans. Clin. Soc. Lond. 7, 22–28.

Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E. (2013). A rat in the labyrinth of anorexia nervosa: contributions of the activity-based anorexia rodent model to the understanding of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 289–301. doi: 10.1002/eat.22095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez, E., and Carrera, O. (2018). Anorexia nervosa treatments and Occam’s razor. Psychol. Med. 48, 1390–1391. doi: 10.1017/s0033291717003944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hacker, P. M. S. (2010). Human Nature: The Categorial Framework. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Hanlon, C., Medhin, G., Alem, A., Araya, M., Abdulahi, A., Tesfaye, M., et al. (2008). Measuring common mental disorders in women in Ethiopia: reliability and construct validity of the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 43, 653–659. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0342-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansson, S. O. (2018). Risk. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/risk/ (accessed September 3, 2019).

Google Scholar

Heinonen, I., and Laukkanen, J. A. (2018). Effects of heat and cold on health, with special reference to Finnish sauna bathing. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 314, R629–R638. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00115.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollingworth, H. L. (1939). Chewing as a techning of relaxation. Science 90, 385–387. doi: 10.1126/science.90.2339.385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hotta, Y., and Benzer, S. (1972). Mapping of behaviour in Drosophila mosaics. Nature 240, 527–535. doi: 10.1038/240527a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huprich, S. K., Paggeot, A. V., and Samuel, D. B. (2015). Comparing the Personality Disorder Interview for DSM-IV (PDI-IV) and SCID-II borderline personality disorder scales: an item-response theory analysis. J. Pers. Assess. 97, 13–21. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2014.946606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutsebaut, J., Feenstra, D. J., and Kamphuis, J. H. (2016). Development and preliminary psychometric evaluation of a brief self-report questionnaire for the assessment of the DSM-5 level of personality functioning scale: the LPFS brief form (LPFS-BF). Personal. Disord. 7, 192–197. doi: 10.1037/per0000159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioakimidis, I., Zandian, M., Ulbl, F., Bergh, C., Leon, M., and Södersten, P. (2011). How eating affects mood. Physiol. Behav. 103, 290–294. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, A. I., Friedman, J. P., Boyatzis, R. E., and Taylor, S. N. (2016). Why do you believe in God? relationships between religious belief, analytic thinking, mentalizing and moral concern. PLoS One 11:e0149989. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, F. (1977). Evolution and tinkering. Science 196, 1161–1166. doi: 10.1126/science.860134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacyna, S. (2009). The most important of all the organs: darwin on the brain. Brain J. Neurol. 132, 3481–3487. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastroch, M., Oelkrug, R., and Keipert, S. (2018). Insights into brown adipose tissue evolution and function from non-model organisms. J. Exp. Biol. 221(Pt Suppl. 1), jeb169425. doi: 10.1242/jeb.169425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, F., Pratt, M., and Wardle, J. (2012). Dietary restraint and self-regulation in eating behavior. Int. J. Obes. 36, 665–674. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C., and Hardin, C. D. (2018). Ideological asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of misinformation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 23, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaisari, P., Dourish, C. T., and Higgs, S. (2017). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disordered eating behaviour: a systematic review and a framework for future research. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 53, 109–121. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalm, L. M., and Semba, R. D. (2005). They starved so that others be better fed: remembering Ancel Keys and the Minnesota experiment. J. Nutr. 135, 1347–1352. doi: 10.1093/jn/135.6.1347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaye, W. H., Bulik, C., Thornton, L., and Plotnicov, K. (2006). Dr. Kaye and Colleagues reply. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 327–329.

Google Scholar

Kaye, W. H., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L., Barbarich, N., and Masters, K. (2004). Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 2215–2221.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kaye, W. H., Fudge, J. L., and Paulus, M. (2009). New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 573–584. doi: 10.1038/nrn2682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ke, Q., Ferrara, E., Radicchi, F., and Flammini, A. (2015). Defining and identifying sleeping beauties in science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 7426–7431. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1424329112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr-Gaffney, J., Harrison, A., and Tchanturia, K. (2018). Social anxiety in the eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 48, 2477–2491. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keski-Rahkonen, A., Hoek, H. W., Susser, E. S., Linna, M. S., Sihvola, E., Raevuori, A., et al. (2007). Epidemiology and course of anorexia nervosa in the community. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1259–1265.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., and Taylor, H. L. (1950). The Biology of Human Starvation. Minneapolis, MN: The University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Kita, H., and Kita, T. (2011). Cortical stimulation evokes abnormal responses in the dopamine-depleted rat basal ganglia. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 31, 10311–10322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0915-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klump, K. L., Bulik, C. M., Kaye, W. H., Treasure, J., and Tyson, E. (2009). Academy for eating disorders position paper: eating disorders are serious mental illnesses. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 42, 97–103. doi: 10.1002/eat.20589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Kothari, M., Grigoriadis, A., Trulsson, M., and Svensson, P. (2018). Bite or brain: implication of sensorimotor regulation and neuroplasticity in oral rehabilitation procedures. J. Oral Rehabil. 45, 323–333. doi: 10.1111/joor.12603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, M., Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C. L., Atkinson, Q. D., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., et al. (2019). Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 286:20190202. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leisman, G., Machado, C., Melillo, R., and Mualem, R. (2012). Intentionality and “free-will” from a neurodevelopmental perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:36. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, R. L., and Rawana, J. S. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and eating disorders across the lifespan: a systematic review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 50, 22–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levinson, C. A., Zerwas, S. C., Brosof, L. C., Thornton, L. M., Strober, M., Pivarunas, B., et al. (2018). Associations between dimensions of anorexia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder: an examination of personality and psychological factors in patients with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 27, 161–172. doi: 10.1002/erv.2635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K. H., Seifert, C. M., Schwarz, N., and Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest J. Am. Psychol. Soc. 13, 106–131. doi: 10.1177/1529100612451018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewandowsky, S., Pilditch, T. D., Madsen, J. K., Oreskes, N., and Risbey, J. S. (2019). Influence and seepage: an evidence-resistant minority can affect public opinion and scientific belief formation. Cognition 188, 124–139. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, D. E. (2011). Evolution of the Human Head. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Lieberman, D. E. (2014). The Story of the Human Body: Evolution, Health and Disease. New York, NY: Random House LLC.

Google Scholar

Lowry, C. A., Lightman, S. L., and Nutt, D. J. (2009). That warm fuzzy feeling: brain serotonergic neurons and the regulation of emotion. J. Psychopharmacol. Oxf. Engl. 23, 392–400. doi: 10.1177/0269881108099956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahy, C. E. V., Moses, L. J., and Pfeifer, J. H. (2014). How and where: theory-of-mind in the brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 68–81. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maj, M. (2005). “Psychiatric comorbidity”: an artefact of current diagnostic systems? Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 186, 182–184. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.3.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mataix-Cols, D., Pertusa, A., and Leckman, J. F. (2007). Issues for DSM-V: how should obsessive-compulsive and related disorders be classified? Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1313–1314. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattila-Evenden, M., Svanborg, P., Gustavsson, P., and Asberg, M. (1996). Determinants of self-rating and expert rating concordance in psychiatric out-patients, using the affective subscales of the CPRS. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 94, 386–396. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb09879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A., and Henrich, J. (2019). Weighing outcome vs. intent across societies: how cultural models of mind shape moral reasoning. Cognition 182, 95–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, S. M., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L. M., Mattheisen, M., Mortensen, P. B., and Petersen, L. (2015). Diagnosed anxiety disorders and the risk of subsequent anorexia nervosa: a Danish population register study. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 23, 524–530. doi: 10.1002/erv.2402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micali, N., Hilton, K., Nakatani, E., Natatani, E., Heyman, I., Turner, C., et al. (2011). Is childhood OCD a risk factor for eating disorders later in life? A longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 41, 2507–2513. doi: 10.1017/S003329171100078X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micali, N., Solmi, F., Horton, N. J., Crosby, R. D., Eddy, K. T., Calzo, J. P., et al. (2015). Adolescent eating disorders predict psychiatric, high-risk behaviors and weight outcomes in young adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 652–659.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nazar, B. P., Bernardes, C., Peachey, G., Sergeant, J., Mattos, P., and Treasure, J. (2016). The risk of eating disorders comorbid with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 1045–1057. doi: 10.1002/eat.22643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neimeijer, R. A. M., Roefs, A., and de Jong, P. J. (2017). Heightened attentional capture by visual food stimuli in anorexia nervosa. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126, 805–811. doi: 10.1037/abn0000275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesse, R. M. (2019). Good Reasons for Bad Feelings. London: Allen Lane.

Google Scholar

Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., et al. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e1. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nusbaum, M. P., Blitz, D. M., and Marder, E. (2017). Functional consequences of neuropeptide and small-molecule co-transmission. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 389–403. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, K. M., and Vincent, N. K. (2003). Psychiatric comorbidity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: nature, prevalence, and causal relationships. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23, 57–74. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00201-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oltra-Cucarella, J., Rojo Moreno, L., Arribas Sáiz, P., Sanguesa García, C., Latorre Paniagua, P., Hidalgo Muñxoz, E., et al. (2015). Impaired executive functioning influences verbal memory in anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 16, 47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paquola, A. C. M., Erwin, J. A., and Gage, F. H. (2017). Insights into the role of somatic mosaicism in the brain. Curr. Opin. Syst. Biol. 1, 90–94. doi: 10.1016/j.coisb.2016.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearl, J. (2019). The Book of Why, the New Science of Cause and Effect. London: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Pearson, C. M., Wonderlich, S. A., and Smith, G. T. (2015). A risk and maintenance model for bulimia nervosa: from impulsive action to compulsive behavior. Psychol. Rev. 122, 516–535. doi: 10.1037/a0039268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrovich, G. D. (2013). Forebrain networks and the control of feeding by environmental learned cues. Physiol. Behav. 121, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, T. M. (2018). Genetics in the Madhouse: The Unknown History of Human Heredity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., et al. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature 530, 327–330. doi: 10.1038/nature16980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raney, T. J., Thornton, L. M., Berrettini, W., Brandt, H., Crawford, S., Fichter, M. M., et al. (2008). Influence of overanxious disorder of childhood on the expression of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 326–332. doi: 10.1002/eat.20508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regier, D. A., Myers, J. K., Kramer, M., Robins, L. N., Blazer, D. G., Hough, R. L., et al. (1984). The NIMH epidemiologic catchment area program. Historical context, major objectives, and study population characteristics. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 41, 934–941.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Richard, J. M., and Berridge, K. C. (2013). Prefrontal cortex modulates desire and dread generated by nucleus accumbens glutamate disruption. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 360–370. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, C. P. (1922). A behavioristic study of the activity of the rat. J. Comp. Psychol. Monogr. 1, 1–55.

Google Scholar

Robinson, H. (2017). Dualism. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2017/entries/dualism (accessed September 3, 2019).

Google Scholar

Rösler, M., Retz, W., Thome, J., Schneider, M., Stieglitz, R.-D., and Falkai, P. (2006). Psychopathological rating scales for diagnostic use in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256(Suppl. 1), i3–i11. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-1001-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rück, C., Larsson, K. J., Lind, K., Perez-Vigil, A., Isomura, K., Sariaslan, A., et al. (2015). Validity and reliability of chronic tic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder diagnoses in the Swedish National Patient Register. BMJ Open 5:e007520. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, G. (1979). Bulimia nervosa: an ominous variant of anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 9, 429–448. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700031974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, N., Smeets, P. A. M., van Elburg, A. A., Danner, U. N., van Meer, F., Hoek, H. W., et al. (2015). Altered food-cue processing in chronically ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:46. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, D. X., and Kawata, M. (2018). Positive and balancing selection on SLC18A1 gene associated with psychiatric disorders and human-unique personality traits. Evol. Lett. 2, 499–510. doi: 10.1002/evl3.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaumberg, K., Zerwas, S., Goodman, E., Yilmaz, Z., Bulik, C. M., and Micali, N. (2019). Anxiety disorder symptoms at age 10 predict eating disorder symptoms and diagnoses in adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 686–696. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J. E., Wise, J. D., Benton, N. A., Brozek, J. M., and Keen-Rhinehart, E. (2013). When do we eat? Ingestive behavior, survival, and reproductive success. Horm. Behav. 64, 702–728. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitz, J., Kahraman-Lanzerath, B., Legenbauer, T., Sarrar, L., Herpertz, S., Salbach-Andrae, H., et al. (2013). The role of impulsivity, inattention and comorbid ADHD in patients with bulimia nervosa. PLoS One 8:e63891. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sijtsma, K., and Molenaar, I. W. (2002). Introduction to Nonparametric Item Response Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.

Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York, NY: D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc.

Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Smith, T. M. (2018). The Tales Teeth Tell. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Södersten, P., and Bergh, C. (2006). Comorbidity of anxiety with eating disorders and OCD. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 327–329.

Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Bergh, C., Leon, M., Brodin, U., and Zandian, M. (2017). Cognitive behavior therapy for eating disorders versus normalization of eating behavior. Physiol. Behav. 174, 178–190. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Bergh, C., Leon, M., and Zandian, M. (2016). Dopamine and anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 60, 26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Bergh, C., and Zandian, M. (2006). Psychoneuroendocrinology of anorexia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 1149–1153. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Bergh, C., Zandian, M., and Ioakimidis, I. (2011). Obesity and the brain. Med. Hypotheses 77, 371–373.

Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Bergh, C., Zandian, M., and Ioakimidis, I. (2014). Homeostasis in anorexia nervosa. Front. Neurosci. 8:234. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Brodin, U., Sjöberg, J., Zandian, M., and Bergh, C. (2019a). Treatment outcomes for eating disorders in Sweden: data from the national quality registry. BMJ Open 9:e024179. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Brodin, U., Zandian, M., and Bergh, C. (2019b). Eating behavior and the evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Front. Neurosci. 13:596. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Södersten, P., Nergårdh, R., Bergh, C., Zandian, M., and Scheurink, A. (2008). Behavioral neuroendocrinology and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 29:445–462. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephens, D. W., and Krebs, J. (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Stoljar, D. (2017). Physicalism. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2017/entries/physicalism (accessed September 3, 2019).

Google Scholar

Striedter, G. F., Avise, J. C., and Ayala, F. J. (2012). In the light of evolution VI: brain and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109(Suppl. 1), 10607–10611.

Google Scholar

Stulz, N., Hepp, U., Gächter, C., Martin-Soelch, C., Spindler, A., and Milos, G. (2013). The severity of ADHD and eating disorder symptoms: a correlational study. BMC Psychiatry 13:44. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sultson, H., van Meer, F., Sanders, N., van Elburg, A. A., Danner, U. N., Hoek, H. W., et al. (2016). Associations between neural correlates of visual stimulus processing and set-shifting in ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 255, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svanborg, P., and Åsberg, M. (1994). A new self-rating scale for depression and anxiety states based on the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 89, 21–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb01480.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svedlund, N. E., Norring, C., Ginsberg, Y., and von Hausswolff-Juhlin, Y. (2017). Symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) among adult eating disorder patients. BMC Psychiatry 17:19. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1093-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., Romaniello, A., Wang, K., Arendt-Nielsen, L., and Sessle, B. J. (2006). One hour of tongue-task training is associated with plasticity in corticomotor control of the human tongue musculature. Exp. Brain Res. 173, 165–173. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0380-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swinbourne, J. M., and Touyz, S. W. (2007). The co-morbidity of eating disorders and anxiety disorders: a review. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 15, 253–274. doi: 10.1002/erv.784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Clementi, M., Collantoni, E., Pinato, C., Forzan, M., et al. (2016). Clinical and genetic correlates of decision making in anorexia nervosa. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 38, 327–337. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1112878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tryon, W. W. (2018). Mediators and mechanisms. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 6, 619–628.

Google Scholar

Tucker, T. (2007). The Great Starvation Experiment: Ancel Keys and the Men Who Starved for Science. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Ungar, P. S. (2017). Evolution’s Bite: A Story of Teeth, Diet, and Human Origins. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

University of Minnesota (2018). The Minnesota Semistarvation Experiment. Available at: http://www.epi.umn.edu/cvdepi/video/the-minnesota-semistarvation-experiment/ (accessed September 3, 2019).

Google Scholar

Varga, S. (2012). Evolutionary psychiatry and depression: testing two hypotheses. Med. Health Care Philos. 15, 41–52. doi: 10.1007/s11019-010-9305-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, H. J., Yilmaz, Z., Thornton, L. M., Hübel, C., Coleman, J. R. I., Gaspar, H. A., et al. (2019). Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat. Genet. 51, 1207–1214. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0439-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weele, C. M. V., Siciliano, C. A., Matthews, G. A., Namburi, P., Izadmehr, E. M., Espinel, I. C., et al. (2018). Dopamine enhances signal-to-noise ratio in cortical-brainstem encoding of aversive stimuli. Nature 563, 397–401. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0682-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (2003). The mind’s best trick: how we experience conscious will. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 65–69. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00002-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (2004). Précis of the illusion of conscious will. Behav. Brain Sci 27, 649–659.

Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (2018). The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M., and Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. Am. Psychol. 54, 480–492. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinbach, N., Sher, H., Lock, J. D., and Henik, A. (2017). Attention networks in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27, 343–351. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1057-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehouse, H., François, P., Savage, P. E., Currie, T. E., Feeney, K. C., Cioni, E., et al. (2019). Complex societies precede moralizing gods throughout world history. Nature 568, 226–229. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1043-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurother. J. Am. Soc. Exp. Neurother. 9, 490–499. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 991–1010. doi: 10.1037/a0027347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willsey, A. J., Morris, M. T., Wang, S., Willsey, H. R., Sun, N., Teerikorpi, N., et al. (2018). The psychiatric cell map initiative: a convergent systems biological approach to illuminating key molecular pathways in europsychiatric disorders. Cell 174, 505–520. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wray, N. R., Wijmenga, C., Sullivan, P. F., Yang, J., and Visscher, P. M. (2018). Common disease is more complex than implied by the core gene omnigenic model. Cell 173, 1573–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.05.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, K. D. (2008). Eating disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a dimensional approach to purported relations. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 1412–1420. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, Z., Halvorsen, M., Bryois, J., Yu, D., Thornton, L. M., Zerwas, S., et al. (2018). Examination of the shared genetic basis of anorexia nervosa and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Mol. Psychiatry doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0115-4 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, Z., Javaras, K. N., Baker, J. H., Thornton, L. M., Lichtenstein, P., Bulik, C. M., et al. (2017). Association between childhood to adolescent attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom trajectories and late adolescent disordered eating. J. Adolesc. Health Off. Publ. Soc. Adolesc. Med. 61, 140–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yousif, S. R., Aboody, R., and Keil, F. C. (2019). The Illusion of consensus: a failure to distinguish between true and false consensus. Psychol. Sci. 30, 1195–1204. doi: 10.1177/0956797619856844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandian, M. (2009). You Are How You Eat. Ph.D thesis, Karolinska Institutet, Solna.

Google Scholar

Zandian, M., Holmstedt, E., Larsson, A., Bergh, C., Brodin, U., and Södersten, P. (2017). Anxiolytic effect of warmth in anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 135, 266–267. doi: 10.1111/acps.12691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandian, M., Ioakimidis, I., Bergh, C., and Södersten, P. (2007). Cause and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Physiol. Behav. 92, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandian, M., Ioakimidis, I., Bergh, C., and Södersten, P. (2009). Linear eaters turned decelerated: reduction of a risk for disordered eating? Physiol. Behav. 96, 518–521. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zastrow, A., Kaiser, S., Stippich, C., Walther, S., Herzog, W., Tchanturia, K., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 608–616. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeeck, A., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Friederich, H.-C., Brockmeyer, T., Resmark, G., Hagenah, U., et al. (2018). Psychotherapeutic treatment for anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 9:158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijlmans, J., Marhe, R., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., Popma, A., Tiemeier, H., et al. (2017). Children with obsessive-compulsive symptomology in the general population: different subtypes? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. JDBP 38, 476–482. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zipfel, S., Giel, K. E., Bulik, C. M., Hay, P., and Schmidt, U. (2015). Anorexia nervosa: aetiology, assessment, and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry 2, 1099–1111. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00356-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anorexia nervosa, mental causation, evolution, starvation, psychiatric diagnosis, treatment

Citation: Södersten P, Brodin U, Zandian M and Bergh CEK (2019) Verifying Feighner’s Hypothesis; Anorexia Nervosa Is Not a Psychiatric Disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:2110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02110

Received: 30 December 2018; Accepted: 30 August 2019;
Published: 16 September 2019.

Edited by:

Emilio Gutierrez, Universidad de Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Reviewed by:

Shan Guisinger, The University of Montana, United States
Stafford Lightman, University of Bristol, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Södersten, Brodin, Zandian and Bergh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Per Södersten, cGVyLnNvZGVyc3RlbkBraS5zZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.