- 1Centro Nacional de Investigación sobre la Evolución Humana, Burgos, Spain
- 2Fenner School of Environment and Society, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
Domestication and Human Self-Domestication
The term “domestication syndrome” describes a range of correlated trait changes seen in domesticated populations when compared to their wild relatives or ancestors (Jensen, 2006; Wilkins et al., 2014; Zeder, 2015). Controlled experimental breeding has demonstrated rapid emergence of this syndrome in several mammal populations selected for dampened reactive aggression and stress response (Trut, 1999; Jensen, 2006; Kulikov et al., 2016). These results confirm findings of correlated change from longstanding observational research in domesticated lineages (Hemmer, 1990). Known traits include: docile behavior; reduced sexual dimorphism; reduced prognathism; smaller teeth; skeletal gracility; reduced brain sizes; altered oestrus cycles and fertility; floppy ears; elevated vocal communication; and altered pigmentation (Hemmer, 1990; Wilkins et al., 2014; Sánchez-Villagra et al., 2016; Okanoya, 2017). Many of these features are known to appear rapidly, as heterochronic shifts in ontogeny (i.e., paedomorphism or neoteny), rather than as isolated and adaptive mutations (Belyaev, 1979; Trut, 1999; Jensen, 2006; Zeder, 2012, 2015). Heritable hypoplasia of neural crest cell-derived tissues provides the most widely supported proximate explanation for these observed trait correlations (Wilkins et al., 2014).
Interestingly, several traits seen in bonobos (Hare et al., 2012) and in humans (Groves, 1999; Leach, 2003; Cieri et al., 2014; Thomas and Kirby, 2018) suggest intraspecific interactions can drive a process of “self-domestication” via socio-sexual selection for higher social tolerance and less reactive aggression (Cieri et al., 2014; Hare, 2017; Wrangham, 2018). In Homo sapiens, this process is thought to have enabled an expanded cooperative ability, leading to improved language and knowledge-sharing, thereby promoting social complexity and technological advancement (Hare, 2017; Thomas and Kirby, 2018). Humans are also characterized by an outstanding capacity for integration between brain, body and tools, and the evolution of this ability is associated with neuroanatomical changes of the visuospatial association cortex (Bruner, 2018). Whilst current scholarship is yet to address the potential for interaction between self-domestication and body cognition, we hypothesize that there may be value in an examination of any overlap. As such, here, we consider whether and to what extent these phenomena shared common evolutionary factors or reciprocal influences.
The Evolution of the Parietal Cortex in Humans
One of the main goals in evolutionary neurobiology is to identify features and aspects of the human brain that differ from other living and extinct primates (Preuss, 2017). When compared with extant taxa, Homo sapiens is characterized by cerebral features specific to our species, even if, for many of them, it is not clear whether they reflect simple differences in size (due to our peculiarly larger brain) or are entirely novel cerebral traits. Comparison with fossil hominids reveals differences in brain size, but a shared sulcal pattern and overall morphological organization (Bruner, 2017). Notably, there are differences in the cortical proportions of the parietal lobe, which shows dorsal regions that are wider in Neanderthals and generally much larger in modern humans (see Bruner, 2018 for a review). These regions spatially correspond to the precuneus and to the intraparietal sulcus, which have a larger and more complex cortical surface in humans when compared with other primates, including apes.
The parietal cortex is involved in multiple association tasks, but is particularly crucial for visuospatial integration—bridging body and vision, and coordinating eye and hand—and is central to functions like visual imaging, body-centered space and time simulation, and self-awareness (Fletcher et al., 1995; Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Margulies et al., 2009; Freton et al., 2014; Land, 2014). These functions are also involved in relationships between brain and body and between body and environment; key factors that allow offloading and exporting of cognitive functions to external components (especially technology), thereby integrating tools into cognitive schemes of the body (Byrge et al., 2014; Bruner and Iriki, 2016).
Morphological changes in the modern human parietal cortex are not described among early Homo sapiens populations (say 100–300 thousand years ago), but are detected in later specimens, roughly at the time the archaeological record begins to show complex tools, projectile technology, and complex graphic culture (Bruner and Pearson, 2013; Neubauer et al., 2018). These developments, and this timeframe, have also been associated with reductions in masculine craniofacial morphology thought to indicate a process of human self-domestication (Cieri et al., 2014). If self-domestication was a crucial process in modern human evolution, and if body-tool extension and visual imaging have been key factors in modern human parietal cortex development, it makes sense to expect some interaction between their relative causes, effects, and functional mechanisms. As such, it appears worthwhile to consider whether these two features (self-domestication and visuospatial cognition) exert reciprocal influences and, further, whether these complex processes may share contributing factors in common (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Diagram of possible relationships between parietal evolution and self-domestication. Neural plasticity (*) can be a key factor, being targeted by selective processes as to enhance cognitive, cultural, and social capacities.
The Domesticated Brain
In general, brain size is substantially reduced in domesticated lineages when compared to non-domesticated forms (Kruska, 1988, 2005), and such reduction is more prominently expressed in more encephalized taxa (Kruska, 1988). Brain size reduction should, therefore, be particularly apparent in modern humans under the effects of domestication. However, although late modern humans display smaller cranial capacity when compared with earlier populations (Henneberg, 1988; McHenry, 1994; Ruff et al., 1997), relative brain size appears to have increased, because of a simultaneous reduction of body size (McHenry, 1994). We can wonder whether the novel expansion of derived areas (like the parietal ones) could have partially contrasted and masked a generalized reduction of brain volume in our species.
During domestication, limbic structures are particularly reduced (Kruska, 1988). This is probably crucial to achieve a lower aggressive reactivity and, accordingly, to promote and extend social bonds. However, presently available evidence suggests that humans have relatively larger—instead of smaller—limbic components (hippocampus, amygdala and orbito-frontal cortex), at least when compared with living apes (Barger et al., 2014). Such structures are, unfortunately, not directly detectable in fossil species, or in early modern humans.
Juvenile Brains, Bodies, and Tools
Many non-human species utilize “objects” to some extent, but only humans use “tools,” as defined in a strict sense. Here, we suggest that to be “a tool,” an object must fulfill at least three crucial conditions. First, it must be integrated within the body schemes of the brain, as a real extension of its space and functions (Maravita and Iriki, 2004; Tunik et al., 2007; Heed et al., 2015). Second, it must be part of a productive chain, in which a propaedeutic sequence of tools is necessary to achieve a final target (Muller et al., 2017). Third, it must not simply assist the ecological and economical behavior of a species, but must be integrated-with, and necessary-to, a cultural niche (Plummer, 2004). Humans achieve these three conditions by integrating technology into cognitive processes, literally as a spider does with its silk web (Kaplan, 2012; Japyassú and Laland, 2017). According to theories in extended cognition, tools are proper functional elements of our cognitive system (Malafouris, 2010, 2013). That is, our cognitive process does not rely only on the neural system, but also on extra-neural components (technology) to which we delegate specific cognitive functions (Overmann, 2015). Such prosthetic capacity can be defined as the capacity to delegate cognitive functions to external elements, offloading and outsourcing information processing to peripheral (out-of-the-body) components. The parietal cortex in humans is involved in tool use and tool making (Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Bzdok et al., 2015; Goldring and Krubitzer, 2017; Kastner et al., 2017), and hence particularly involved in body-tool extension and integration (Bruner and Iriki, 2016). Human prosthetic capacity is largely enhanced by the remarkable plasticity of our cortical system (Sherwood and Gómez-Robles, 2017), and by the high level of creativity and explorative innovation of our species (Kyriacou and Bruner, 2011). Both features (neural plasticity and explorative behavior) are primarily associated with juvenile life stages and have been enhanced by extension of the juvenile period in humans (Bogin, 1990; Pellegrini et al., 2007). Given that animal domestication is broadly associated with a trend toward relative juvenilization (Harvey and Clutton-Brock, 1985; Smith, 1992; Joffe, 1997), aspects of human self-domestication may also contribute to our enhanced technological capacity. In fact, altered timing and stretching of the life-history is implicated in the extension of those ontogenetic stages more sensitive to novelty, the extension of the post-reproductive period, and the extension of life in general (longevity). All of these aspects of human life-history are strictly necessary to generate intergenerational transfer and cultural evolution (Kaplan and Robson, 2002; Lee, 2003), providing a further link between self-domestication and technological extension.
Interestingly, interpreting parietal expansion as an evolutionary novelty may complicate one diagnostic feature of the supposed juvenilization process in humans: that is, the roundedness of our head, which is often explained as a pedomorphic feature, but which could actually represent an apomorphic cortical character, mimicking a juvenile appearance. Apart from parietal bulging, vault globularity in our species is also due to the curvature of the frontal squama, likely to be a secondary structural consequence of having a reduced facial block positioned under the frontal lobes (Pereira-Pedro et al., 2017). This latter feature can indeed be associated with a pedomorphic process, at least if we consider the reduction of the splanchnocranium as a juvenile heterochronic retention.
Association Cortex, Body Perception, and Social Evolution
A further potential locus of association between parietal expansion and self-domestication is increased sociability. The experimentally demonstrated proximate cause of domestication syndrome is selection against reactive, or autonomic, aggressive response (Trut, 1999; Jensen, 2006; Wilkins et al., 2014). This selective mechanism is thought to have facilitated the emergence of language, increased group sizes, and elevated cooperation in humans (Cieri et al., 2014; Hare, 2017; Thomas and Kirby, 2018). In primate species, group size is proportional to brain size and, for humans, it approaches 150 units (i.e., “Dunbar's number”—Dunbar, 2012, 2018). Interestingly, this correlation particularly concerns the association cortex, probably because of a direct relationship with behavioral complexity (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007; Pearce et al., 2013). The parietal cortex is one of the main association regions (Krienen and Buckner, 2017; Mars et al., 2017) and, in this case, its expansion is likely to have a direct effect of social group size. Moreover, the parietal cortex and visuospatial integration are involved in self-recognition, self-other perception, body-centered simulation, and in the management of a “social space” which uses the body as a functional and metric unit (Hills et al., 2015; Maister et al., 2015; Peer et al., 2015). Actually, the precuneus has been hypothesized to be a crucial element of the network involved in mind reading (Heyes and Frith, 2014). These features (increased social group size through increase of association functions, and the capacity to handle a social space based around one's own body) are strictly intermingled with social effects expected from self-domestication and associated juvenilization, namely an increase in the size and complexity of the social network.
Conclusions
We hypothesize that, in humans, changes associated with self-domestication might have influenced, or been influenced by, body cognition, visuospatial integration, technological extension, and the evolution of the parietal cortex. Alternatively, these features may be independent, and might have evolved independently along the human lineage. These two hypotheses should be discussed and evaluated according to a comparative and functional perspective by investigating this possible association in other primates and considering the corresponding relationships between anatomy, development and cognition. Some aspects of these evolutionary features are likely to have interacted, generating reciprocal enhancement. Others may hide common mechanisms, possibly due to ontogenetic communalities and shared developmental components. In this regard, one candidate may be neural plasticity, which is both a crucial consequence of paedomorphic conditions and a feature particularly influencing the development of the parietal cortex because of its sensitivity to sensorial (somatic and visual) inputs. We can wonder whether sociability associated with self-domestication, an extended juvenile period, and increased neural plasticity, could have prompted the expansion of the parietal cortical surface, subsequently triggering retroactive feedback to enhance its functional consequences. Association cortices may be the result of multiple crossing gradients between sensorimotor regions, generating a patchwork of neural combinations in terms of functional properties (Huntenburg et al., 2017). In this case, prolonged or increased plasticity of the body-vision system may be the essential prerequisite for developing a more anatomically and functionally complex prosthetic capacity, as the ability to incorporate tools into body schemes, offloading cognitive processes to external elements. Importantly, it remains to be evaluated whether this process is strictly associated with the evolution of modern humans (Homo sapiens), or can be traced back to the origin of our genus. In any case, it seems important to consider these processes and functions together when attempting to determine a comprehensive evolutionary narrative for our species.
Author Contributions
Both authors have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Antonio Benítez-Burraco for the invitation to contribute to this special issue on self-domestication and human evolution. We thank the reviewers for their comments and suggestions. BG is supported by an Australian Government RTP Scholarship. This article is funded by the Spanish Government (PGC2018-093925-B-C31).
References
Barger, N., Hanson, K. L., Teffer, K., Schenker-Ahmed, N. M., and Semendeferi, K. (2014). Evidence for evolutionary specialization in human limbic structures. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:277. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00277
Belyaev, D. K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. J. Hered. 70, 301–308. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a109263
Bogin, B. (1990). The evolution of human childhood: a unique growth phase and delayed maturity allow for extensive learning and complex culture. BioScience 40, 16–25. doi: 10.2307/1311235
Bruner, E. (2017). “The fossil evidence of human brain evolution,” in Evolution of Nervous Systems 2e, ed J. Kaas (Oxford: Elsevier), 63–92. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804042-3.00105-6
Bruner, E. (2018). Human paleoneurology and the evolution of the parietal cortex. Brain Behav. Evol. 91, 136–147. doi: 10.1159/000488889
Bruner, E., and Iriki, A. (2016). Extending mind, visuospatial integration, and the evolution of the parietal lobes in the human genus. Quat. Int. 405, 98–110. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.05.019
Bruner, E., and Pearson, O. (2013). Neurocranial evolution in modern humans: the case of Jebel Irhoud 1. Anthropol. Sci. 121, 31–41. doi: 10.1537/ase.120927
Byrge, L., Sporns, O., and Smith, L. B. (2014). Developmental process emerges from extended brain-body-behavior networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18,395–403. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.010
Bzdok, D., Heeger, A., Langner, R., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Palomero-Gallagher, N., et al. (2015). Subspecialization in the human posterior medial cortex. Neuroimage 106, 55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.009
Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004
Cieri, R. L., Churchill, S. E., Franciscus, R. G., Tan, J., and Hare, B. (2014). Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 419–443. doi: 10.1086/677209
Dunbar, R. I., and Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317, 1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 1837–1846. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0217
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The anatomy of friendship. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 32–51. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004
Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Baker, S. C., Shallice, T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., and Dolan, R. J. (1995). The mind's eye-precuneus activation in memory-related imagery. Neuroimage 2, 195–200. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1025
Freton, M., Lemogne, C., Bergouignan, L., Delaveau, P., Lehéricy, S., and Fossati, P. (2014). The eye of the self: precuneus volume and visual perspective during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 959–968. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0546-2
Goldring, A. B., and Krubitzer, L. A. (2017). “Evolution of the parietal cortex in mammals: from manipulation to tool use,” in Evolution of Nervous Systems 2e, ed J. Kaas (Oxford: Elsevier), 259–286. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804042-3.00086-5
Grefkes, C., and Fink, G. R. (2005). The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and monkeys. J. Anat. 207, 3–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00426.x
Groves, C. (1999). The advantages and disadvantages of being domesticated. Persp. Hum. Biol. 4, 1–12.
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Ann. Rev. Psycho. 68, 155–186. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201
Hare, B., Wobber, V., and Wrangham, R. W. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Anim. Behav. 83, 573–585. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.007
Harvey, P. H., and Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1985). Life history variation in primates. Evolution 39, 559–581. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb00395.x
Heed, T., Buchholz, V. N., Engel, A. K., and Röder, B. (2015). Tactile remapping: from coordinate transformation to integration in sensorimotor processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 251–258. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.001
Hemmer, H. (1990). Domestication: the Decline of Environmental Appreciation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heyes, C. M., and Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science 344:1243091. doi: 10.1126/science.1243091
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., Lazer, D., Redish, A. D., and Couzin, I. D. (2015). Exploration versus exploitation in space, mind, and society. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19,46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.004
Huntenburg, J. M., Bazin, P. L., and Margulies, D. S. (2017). Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.002
Japyassú, H. F., and Laland, K. N. (2017). Extended spider cognition. Anim. Cogn. 20, 375–395. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1069-7
Jensen, P. (2006). Domestication-From behaviour to genes and back again. App. Animal Behav. Sci. 97, 3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.11.015
Joffe, T. H. (1997). Social pressure have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 32, 593–605. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1997.0140
Kaplan, D. M. (2012). How to demarcate the boundaries of cognition. Biol. Philos. 27, 545–570. doi: 10.1007/s10539-012-9308-4
Kaplan, H. S., and Robson, A. J. (2002). The emergence of humans: the coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 10221–10226. doi: 10.1073/pnas.152502899
Kastner, S., Chen, Q., Jeong, S. K., and Mruczek, R. E. B. (2017). A brief comparative review of primate posterior parietal cortex: a novel hypothesis on the human toolmaker. Neuropsychology 105, 123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.034
Krienen, F. M., and Buckner, R. L. (2017). “Human association cortex: expanded, untethered, neotenous, plastic,” in Evolution of Nervous Systems 2e, ed J. Kaas (Oxford: Elsevier), 169–183. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804042-3.00126-3
Kruska, D. (1988). “Mammalian domestication and its effect on brain structure and behavior” in Intelligence and Evolutionary Biology, eds H. J. Jerison and I. Jerison (Heidelberg; Berlin: Springer), 211–250. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-70877-0_13
Kruska, D. (2005). On the evolutionary significance of encephalization in some eutherian mammals: effects of adaptive radiation, domestication, and feralization. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 73–108. doi: 10.1159/000082979
Kulikov, A. V., Bazhenova, E. Y., Kulikova, E. A., Fursenko, D. V., Trapezova, L. I., Terenina, E. E., et al. (2016). Interplay between aggression, brain monoamines and fur color mutation in the American mink. Genes Brain Behav. 15, 733–740. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12313
Kyriacou, A., and Bruner, E. (2011). Brain evolution, innovation, and endocranial variations in fossil hominids. PaleoAnthropology 2011, 130–143. doi: 10.4207/PA.2011.ART47
Land, M. F. (2014). Do we have an internal model of the outside world? Phil. Trans. R Soc. B 369, 20130045–20130045. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0045
Leach, H. M. (2003). Human domestication reconsidered. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 349–368. doi: 10.1086/368119
Lee, R. D. (2003). Rethinking the evolutionary theory of aging: transfers, not births, shape senescence in social species. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 9637–9642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1530303100
Maister, L., Slater, M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., and Tsakiris, M. (2015). Changing bodies changes minds: owning another body affects social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.001
Malafouris, L. (2010). The brain-artefact interface (BAI): a challenge for archaeology and cultural neuroscience. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 264–273. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp057
Malafouris, L. (2013). How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Maravita, A., and Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body, (schema). Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008
Margulies, D. S., Vincent, J. L., Kelly, C., Lohmann, G., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., et al. (2009). Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 20069–20074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106
Mars, R. B., Passingham, R. E., Neubert, F. X., Verhagen, L., and Sallet, J. (2017). “Evolutionary specializations of the human association cortex,” in Evolution of Nervous Systems 2e, ed J. Kaas (Oxford: Elsevier), 185–205. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804042-3.00118-4
McHenry, H. M. (1994). Tempo and mode in human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 6780–6786. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.15.6780
Muller, A., Clarkson, C., and Shipton, C. (2017). Measuring behavioural and cognitive complexity in lithic technology throughout human evolution. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 48, 166–180. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2017.07.006
Neubauer, S., Hublin, J. J., and Gunz, P. (2018). The evolution of modern human brain shape. Sci. Adv. 4:eaao5961. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aao5961
Okanoya, K. (2017). Sexual communication and domestication may give rise to the signal complexity necessary for the emergence of language: an indication from songbird studies. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 106–110. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1165-8
Overmann, K. A. (2015). Teeth, tools and human becoming. J. Anthropol. Sci. 93, 163–167. doi: 10.4436/jass.93012
Pearce, E., Stringer, C., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2013). New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 280:20130168. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0168
Peer, M., Salomon, R., Goldberg, I., Blanke, O., and Arzy, S. (2015). Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 11072–11077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504242112
Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D., and Smith, P. K. (2007). Play in evolution and development. Develop. Rev. 27, 261–276. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.09.001
Pereira-Pedro, A. S., Masters, M., and Bruner, E. (2017). Shape analysis of spatial relationships between orbito-ocular and endocranial structures in modern humans and fossil hominids. J. Anat. 231, 947–960. doi: 10.1111/joa.12693
Plummer, T. (2004). Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. Yrb. Phys. Anthropol. 47, 118–164. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20157
Preuss, T. M. (2017). “The human brain: evolution and distinctive features,” in On Human Nature, eds M. Tibayrenc and F. J. Ayala (New York, NY: Academic Press), 125–149. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-420190-3.00008-9
Ruff, C., Trinkaus, E., and Holliday, T. W. (1997). Body mass and encephalization in pleistocene homo. Nature 387, 173–176. doi: 10.1038/387173a0
Sánchez-Villagra, M. R., Geiger, M., and Schneider, R. A. (2016). The taming of the neural crest: a developmental perspective on the origins of morphological covariation in domesticated mammals. Open Sci. 3:160107. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160107
Sherwood, C. C., and Gómez-Robles, A. (2017). Brain plasticity and human evolution. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 46, 399–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev-anthro-102215-100009
Smith, B.H. (1992). Life history and the evolution of human maturation. Evol. Anthropol. 1, 134–142. doi: 10.1002/evan.1360010406
Thomas, J., and Kirby, S. (2018). Self-domestication and the evolution of language. Biol. Philos. 33:9. doi: 10.1007/s10539-018-9612-8
Trut, L. N. (1999). Early canid domestication: the farm-fox experiment. Am. Sci. 87:160. doi: 10.1511/1999.2.160
Tunik, E., Rice, N. J., Hamilton, A., and Grafton, S. T. (2007). Beyond grasping: representation of action in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage 36, T77–T86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.026
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., and Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2014). The “domestication syndrome” in mammals: a unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics 197, 795–808. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.165423
Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 245–253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713611115
Zeder, M. A. (2012). The domestication of animals. J. Anthropol. Res. 68, 161–190. doi: 10.3998/jar.0521004.0068.201
Keywords: brain evolution, parietal lobes, spatial cognition, association cortex, life-history, social evolution, extended cognition
Citation: Bruner E and Gleeson BT (2019) Body Cognition and Self-Domestication in Human Evolution. Front. Psychol. 10:1111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01111
Received: 13 February 2019; Accepted: 29 April 2019;
Published: 21 May 2019.
Edited by:
Antonio Benítez-Burraco, University of Seville, SpainReviewed by:
Matt Joseph Rossano, Southeastern Louisiana University, United StatesAaron Jonas Stutz, Independent Researcher, Sweden
Copyright © 2019 Bruner and Gleeson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Emiliano Bruner, ZW1pbGlhbm8uYnJ1bmVyQGNlbmllaC5lcw==