Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 April 2019
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Research on Emotion and Learning: Contributions from Latin America View all 35 articles

Emotion Regulation and Attitudes Toward Conflict in Colombia: Effects of Reappraisal Training on Negative Emotions and Support for Conciliatory and Aggressive Statements

\r\nCamilo Hurtado-Parrado,*Camilo Hurtado-Parrado1,2*Myriam Sierra-PuentesMyriam Sierra-Puentes2Mohammed El HazzouriMohammed El Hazzouri3Alexandra MoralesAlexandra Morales2Diana Gutirrez-VillamarínDiana Gutiérrez-Villamarín2Laura VelsquezLaura Velásquez4Andrea Correa-ChicaAndrea Correa-Chica5Juan Carlos RincnJuan Carlos Rincón2Karen HenaoKaren Henao2Juan Gabriel CastaedaJuan Gabriel Castañeda6Wilson Lpez-LpezWilson López-López4
  • 1Department of Psychology, Troy University, Troy, AL, United States
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 3Mount Royal University, Bissett School of Business, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 4Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Department of Psychology, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 5Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Departamento de Psicoloxía Social, Básica e Metodoloxía, Facultade de Psicoloxía, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 6Corporación Universitaria Minuto de Dios, Dirección de Investigaciones, Bogotá, Colombia

Control of negative emotions (e.g., anger and fear) by political cues perpetuate intractable conflict by mobilizing public support for aggressive actions. Halperin et al. (2013) found that reappraisal – an adaptive form of emotion regulation – decreased negative emotions triggered by anger-inducing information related to the Israeli–Palestinian conflict, and increased support for conciliatory statements. We tested these effects in the context of the conflict between the Colombian government and the Fuerzas Armadas Revolucionarias de Colombia-Ejército del Pueblo (FARC-EP). Reappraisal training reduced negative emotions produced by a presentation that illustrated FARC’s violent actions, and increased support for conciliatory statements (with overall moderate effect magnitudes). We also found that negative emotions mediated the effects of reappraisal on the support for aggressive and conciliatory statements. These findings indicate a high degree of generality of the phenomena, especially considering the differences between the Israeli–Palestinian conflict and the Colombian conflict. Our findings also show promise for replicating these effects on other types of intergroup conflicts and guiding effective public policy.

Introduction

Violent intractable conflicts occupy a central social and political position in the affected societies and require immense material and psychological investment (Kriesberg, 1993; Bar-Tal, 1998; Gross et al., 2013). Individuals living in areas of intractable conflict frequently experience high intensity negative emotions, such as anger, fear, despair, and hatred, irrespective of how directly the events inherent to those conflicts affect them (Halperin, 2014; Rosler et al., 2015). The grief over the loss of known or unknown combatants or civilian casualties, fear from being hurt or having a friend or relative hurt, and despair over failed attempts to achieve peace are the type of intense emotions experienced by members of societies affected by intractable conflict. In this context, political cues gain considerable control over these negative emotions, which in turn, seem to perpetuate the continuation of conflicts by mobilizing public support for aggressive actions, and hindering progress toward conflict resolution (Gross et al., 2013; Halperin et al., 2013; Halperin, 2014).

If negative emotions related to conflict limit the attempts toward a conflict’s peaceful resolution, it seems plausible that efforts to decrease such emotions could result in important outcomes. Research that has extended the field of emotion regulation (Gross, 2013, 2014) to the study of conflict resolution has provided evidence in support of this notion (Gross et al., 2013; Halperin et al., 2013; Halperin, 2014; Halperin and Pliskin, 2015; Rosler et al., 2015; Čehajić-Clancy et al., 2016; Gutentag et al., 2016).

Emotion Regulation and Intractable Conflict: The Effects of Cognitive Reappraisal

Emotion regulation has been defined as the processes by which individuals influence the intensity and valence of their emotions, when they have them, and how they are expressed (Gross, 2014). Individuals implement a wide range of emotion regulation strategies during their day-to-day aversive interactions, including distraction, rumination, wishful thinking, and social sharing (Páez et al., 2013). However, they are not equally adaptive in terms of effectiveness to decrease subjective distress and/or physiological arousal, while maintaining the individual’s ability to pursue meaningful short- and long-term personal and interpersonal goals (Gross, 2014). Cognitive reappraisal is an emotion regulation strategy aimed at reducing or increasing emotional responses by reinterpreting the meaning of the triggering situation (Gross, 2002). This reappraisal process therefore entails detecting and assessing the significance of the environment for wellbeing (Frijda, 2009) - e.g., perceiving the positive aspects of the triggering event, changing its personal relevance, or imagining better or worse outcomes. Over the last decade, mounting research has shown that reappraisal importantly improves physical and psychological well-being (Gross and John, 2003; Webb et al., 2012; Gross, 2014; Hu et al., 2014; Picó-Pérez et al., 2017). However, only recently it has been suggested that it also has a positive role in larger scale phenomena, such as intractable conflicts.

In the context of the Israeli–Palestinian conflict, Halperin and Gross (2011) found a positive correlation between the implementation of reappraisal and support for providing humanitarian aid to Palestinian citizens by Israeli citizens. More recently, direct evidence of the causal role of emotion regulation on changes to political attitudes in intractable conflicts was obtained. Halperin et al. (2013) tested if reappraisal training could reduce negative intergroup emotions and, in doing so, could decrease support for aggressive actions and increase conciliatory attitudes toward conflict-related events. Israeli participants (college students) first received either reappraisal training or were assigned to a control condition, which in both cases was followed by a presentation of anger-inducing information related to the Israeli–Palestinian conflict (texts, pictures, and music). After the presentation, the participants indicated the extent to which they felt positive and negative emotions, together with their degree of support for different statements reflecting conciliatory policies (e.g., “Regardless of the security situation, Israel needs to transfer food, and medication to Gaza residents”) and aggressive policies (e.g., “If the Israeli Defense Forces detects a terrorist in a building full of civilians, Israel should bomb the building even if most of the civilians will most likely be killed”) toward Palestinians in the Gaza strip. Halperin et al. (2013) found that (a) participants in the reappraisal condition reported lower levels of negative emotions (e.g., anger) compared with control participants; (b) participants in the reappraisal condition expressed more support for conciliatory statements and less support for aggressive statements than participants in the control condition; (c) negative emotions were positively associated with support for aggressive statements and negatively associated with support for conciliatory statements; and (d) negative emotions, specially anger, mediated the effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory and aggressive statements. In our research, we aimed to reproduce these findings in the Colombian context.

The present study was conceived by the time major efforts to end the more than five-decade Colombian conflict between the government and the FARC-EP (Revolutionary Armed Forces of Colombia – Army of the People) were being implemented. Also, with consideration of the potential extension of the findings reported by Halperin et al. (2013) regarding the role of emotion regulation on changing attitudes toward conciliatory and aggressive policies in intractable conflicts.

The Colombian Government-FARC Conflict and the Peace Accord

Fuerzas Armadas Revolucionarias de Colombia (FARC) was officially founded in 1964. It is one of the multiple expressions of agrarian struggles that emerged in Colombian rural areas during the 1930s, and the bipartisan-fueled violence that erupted during the 1950s. FARC shared with other guerrillas the influence of the ideological and political climate of the Cold War and the Cuban Revolution (Thomson, 2011; Centro Nacional de Memoria Histórica, 2013).

FARC proclaimed in the 1980s that their goal was to overthrow the Colombian government. During the same decade, other armed actors emerged, including right-wing paramilitary groups, which had ties to the army, political figures, drug cartels, and large landowners. By the end of the decade, the conflict escalated, including a dramatic upsurge in violent actions affecting the civilian population (e.g., terrorist acts, kidnapping, massacres, landmines, forced recruitment, forced displacement, disappearances, and selective executions; Abello-Llanos et al., 2009; Grajales, 2011).

Drug trafficking became a major funding source for both paramilitary groups and the FARC during the following decades, which resulted in their strengthening. The Colombian state forces (police and army) also grew substantially as response to the consolidation of the other actors. The end result was a modernized and exacerbated conflict, which further affected the civilian population – including frequent human rights’ violations. The estimated number of victims surpassed six million (Human Rights Watch, 2015; Unidad para las Victimas, 2017), including millions of internally displaced and more than 200,000 deaths (near 80% civilians; Historical Memory Group, 2016).

The first step toward the termination of the Government-FARC conflict was the signing of the “General Accord for the Termination of the Conflict and the Construction of a Stable and Lasting Peace” on August 26, 2012 in Havana, Cuba. After 4 years of negotiations, the parties signed on June 23, 2016 the 6-point accord on the end of the conflict, which included agreements related to an integral rural reform, political participation, a bilateral and definite ceasefire and cessation of hostilities, security guarantees for human-rights defenders and social movements, a solution to the problem of illicit drugs, victims’ reparation and compensation, and mechanisms for implementation and verification of the accord (Colombian Government, 2016a; Amnesty International, 2017).

In addition, the parties agreed on a national referendum (plebiscite) as a mechanism for ratification of the signed peace accord, which took place on October 2, 2016. With a 37.4% turnout, 50.2% of Colombians voted against it, and 49.8% voted in favor (Registraduria Nacional del Estado Civil, 2016). This unexpected result obligated the Colombian government and FARC to revise the agreement, this time with the participation of the opposition. The amended version of the agreement was approved by the Colombian Senate and the House of Representatives on November 29 and 30, 2016 (Colombian Government, 2016b).

Emotional factors had an important role in the difficulties that the peace process encountered – especially the results of the national referendum – which continue to be key to the success of ongoing processes of forgiveness and reconciliation (López-López et al., 2013, 2018; Cortés et al., 2015). The success of the controversial campaign ran by the opposition party (Centro Democrático) against the accord supported this notion. Reports indicate that one of its main strategies was triggering negative emotions in the voters, such as anger and fear, by distorting some of the contents of the accord regarding impunity for the FARC members and the negative economic and political implications of accepting the accord (El Espectador, 2016; El País, 2016).

Overview of the Study

The present study aimed to test whether the effects of cognitive reappraisal training reported by Halperin et al. (2013) could be reproduced in the context of the Colombian government-FARC conflict and the efforts of these actors to end the conflict. The experiment was conducted during the week before the national referendum on the acceptance of the peace agreement took place (October 2, 2016). The methodology of Halperin et al.’s (2013) study was adapted to systematically replicate, as close as possible, the conditions of their experiments (e.g., selection of relevant images and texts for the anger-inducing presentation, measurement of negative emotions, and selection of the conciliatory and aggressive statements toward FARC). Analogous to Halperin et al. (2013) study, college students received cognitive reappraisal training, or a control condition, followed by a presentation of anger-inducing information related to FARC’s violent actions during the conflict. Immediately after the presentation, the participants indicated the extent to which they felt positive and negative emotions. They were also asked to rate their degree of support for different conciliatory or aggressive statements toward FARC, which were prepared by taking excerpts from the peace accord already signed between the involved parts (to be voted on the national referendum; Colombian Government, 2016a) or quotes of public statements made by political opponents to that peace agreement (published in national newspapers). Based on Halperin et al.’s (2013) findings, we predicted that reappraisal training would have resulted in (a) lower levels of negative emotions triggered by anger-inducing information related to FARC’s violent actions, and (b) more support for conciliatory statements and less for aggressive statements. In addition, we expected that (c) negative emotions, including anger, would mediate the effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory and aggressive statements.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One-hundred eight college students voluntarily participated in the study (69 women and 39 men, Mage = 20.3 years; SDage = 1.79), which were recruited from the following undergraduate psychology programs in Bogotá (Colombia): Konrad Lorenz Fundación Universitaria (n = 24), Universidad San Buenaventura (n = 24), Pontificia Universidad Javeriana (n = 22), and Corporación Universitaria Minuto de Dios – Soacha (n = 38). These institutions were selected to capture the wide range of socioeconomic statuses that characterize the Colombian population. Students received a refreshment at the end of the study in return for their participation.

This study was carried out in accordance with the ethical guidelines and recommendations of the American Psychological Association – APA (2002) and the Colombian Board of Psychologists (2016). All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Research and Ethics Committee at Konrad Lorenz Fundación Universitaria.

Procedure

Students were invited to the laboratory to participate in a study related to human emotion. Initially, they completed a trauma inventory (Bremner et al., 2007) aimed at screening their direct experience with the events that were portrayed in subsequent phases of the experiment (e.g., they were asked if they had witnessed a murder or violent acts). As per recommendation of the ethics committee that oversaw the study, participants who reported any of the traumatic experiences listed in the inventory did not continue to further stages of the experiment (n = 31, different from the 108 participants of the study). The different phases of the experiment are described below, which were applied in a single session (approximately 15 min). The participants were individually tested.

Reappraisal Training

Students were randomly assigned to a reappraisal-training condition (n = 54) or a control condition (n = 54). The protocol for the reappraisal intervention replicated that reported by Halperin et al. (2013), who adapted it from that implemented by Richards and Gross (2000). Each participant was exposed to his/her corresponding condition individually. Using a Microsoft PowerPoint®presentation (available on request) projected in a computer screen, the participant observed five anger-inducing pictures related to the Colombian conflict (used from an ongoing project aimed at validating the emotional properties of conflict-related images), and was asked to “respond to them like scientists, objectively and analytically trying to think about them in a cold and detached manner” (Halperin et al., 2013, p. 2). The experimenter explained how to use the reappraisal technique in the presence of the first image, and the participant was asked to apply the technique in the presence of four additional pictures. During all the trials, the experimenter ensured that the participant applied the technique appropriately. Participants in the control condition were exposed to the same set of pictures, but they were only asked to respond to them naturally.

Presentation of Anger-Inducing Information

After the reappraisal or control manipulation, all participants watched a 4-min PowerPoint presentation that portrayed FARC’s violent acts (e.g., displacement, forced disappearance, and sexual violence – presentation available on request) with pictures, text, and music. This presentation was adapted to resemble, as close as possible, the presentation implemented by Halperin et al. (2013) – e.g., number of pictures, music, and length and content of the texts. Prior to the projection of the presentation, participants in the reappraisal condition were asked to apply the technique that they had learned in the previous phase, whereas those in the control group were reminded to respond naturally.

Negative/Positive Emotion Assessment

After the anger-inducing presentation, the participants indicated the degree to which they felt positive (interested, optimistic, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, and active) and negative (angry, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery, and afraid) emotions using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS – Watson et al., 1988 – adapted to Spanish by Dufey and Fernández, 2012; αpositive = 0.78; αnegative = 0.87). Participants indicated to what extent they presently felt each emotion toward FARC using a Likert-style rating scale that ranged from 1 = “very slightly or not at all” to 5 = “Extremely.” Specific scores for each emotion and total scores for negative and positive emotions were analyzed. The sum of the scores on the corresponding individual emotions were used to produce PANAS Negative and PANAS Positive scores for each participant; accordingly, scores in each of these two measures could range between 10 and 50.

Support for Conciliatory and Aggressive Statements

In what was presented as a separate study on attitudes toward the Colombian conflict, the participants indicated their support of three statements reflecting conciliatory statements, and another three items reflecting aggressive statements. Conciliatory statements consisted of excerpts copied verbatim from the peace accord signed between FARC and the Colombian government (Colombian Government, 2016a), which was to be voted on a national referendum days after the experiment took place (e.g., “Reincorporation of the FARC forces to the civil life will be an integral and sustainable process, exceptional and transitory, that will take into account the interests of the community and FARC, including their members and their families, and which will be oriented toward the regional strengthening of the social tissue”). Aggressive statements consisted of verbatim public statements made by political opponents of that peace agreement (e.g., statements in national newspapers – “Although FARC announced 1 year ago a unilateral cease fire, they have not stopped dealing drugs or extorting. These are some of the criminal activities that they have never recognized, which affect the public order and the community” – Interview with Senator E. Macias by Diario del Huila, 2016). The Likert-style rating scale for the conciliatory and aggressive statements ranged from 1 = highly oppose, to 6 = very much in favor. Factor analysis revealed that there were indeed two factors, one measuring conciliatory statements (αconciliatory = 0.65), and another measuring aggressive statements (αaggressive = 0.68). See complete list of aggressive and conciliatory statements on Appendix 1 (see Supplementary Materials). Participants also completed the 33-item Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne and Marlowe, 1960 – adapted to Spanish by Ferrando and Chico, 2000).

Data Analyses

Halperin et al. (2013) tested the effects of reappraisal on overall negative and positive emotion scores (PANAS positive and PANAS negative) and scores on specific emotions (e.g., anger). Accordingly, we conducted one-tailed independent-samples t tests to assess directional effects of reappraisal on PANAS negative and PANAS positive and on each of the ten positive and ten negative emotions (e.g., based on Halperin et al.’s (2013) findings we expected significant decrements in negative emotions resulting from reappraisal training).

Also following Halperin et al. (2013) approach and findings, we (a) assessed whether reappraisal increased support for conciliatory statements and reduced support for aggressive statements using directional t tests; and (b) tested the mediating effects of negative emotions on support for aggressive and conciliatory statements using PROCESS procedure for SPSS with model 4 and 5000 bootstrap resamples (Hayes, 2013).

The potential effect of reappraisal on the scores of the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale were assessed with a two-tailed independent-samples t test. Cohen-s’d effect-size calculations were conducted (when applicable).

Results

Effects of Reappraisal Training on Negative Emotions and Conciliatory and Aggressive Statements

A comparison of the overall negative emotions scores (PANAS negative) indicated that participants exposed to reappraisal training prior to the presentation of the anger-inducing information reported less intense negative emotions (M = 19.76, SD = 7,24) than participants in the control condition (M = 23.46, SD = 8,46), t(106) = 2.445, p = 0.008, Cohen’s d = 0.47. No significant differences in the overall scores of positive emotions (PANAS positive) were found between reappraisal (M = 24,81, SD = 6.80) and control (M = 24.31, SD = 5.87) groups; t(106) = 0.409, p = 0.342.

Similar to Halperin et al.’s (2013), we tested the effects of reappraisal on each individual positive and negative emotion using one-tailed independent-samples t tests. As shown in Table 1, reappraisal significantly reduced scores on anger, distress, fear, irritability, and uneasiness, with moderate-to-high effect magnitudes (Gignac and Szodorai, 2016), whereas it had no significant effect on participants’ reports of nervousness, shame, guiltiness, and hostility (ps > 0.115) or in any of the positive emotions assessed (ps > 0.145).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. One-tailed independent-samples t tests and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for scores on specific negative emotions of participants in the reappraisal training (RT) and control condition (CC).

Participants in the reappraisal condition expressed more support for conciliatory statements (M = 4.88, SD = 0.93) when compared with participants in the control group (M = 4.56, SD = 0.96), t(106) = 1.735, p = 0.043, d = 0.33. The obtained Cohen’s d indicated a moderate effect of reappraisal on increasing support for conciliatory statements.

No significant differences between control (M = 3.42, SD = 1.14) and reappraisal groups (M = 3.36, SD = 1.32) were found regarding support for aggressive statements, t(106) = 0.234, p = 0.407. No interactions between condition and gender on support for conciliatory and aggressive statements were observed.

Negative Emotions as Mediators of the Effect of Reappraisal on Support for Aggressive and Conciliatory Statements

Halperin et al. (2013) found that global score on negative emotions (i.e., PANAS Negative) and anger mediated the effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory and aggressive statements. Here we tested whether these measures had the same mediating role. In addition, based on the reports that the campaign against the peace accord between government and FARC was successful by triggering fear in the population (El Espectador, 2016; El País, 2016), we assessed if this emotion had a mediating effect between reappraisal and support for conciliatory and aggressive statements.

All procedures were conducted using Model 4 of the SPSS macro PROCESS (Hayes, 2013) and 5,000 bootstrap resamples. We followed Hayes and Rockwood’s (2016) approach to determine whether a predicted variable M mediated the effect of an independent variable (X) on a dependent variable (Y). To that aim, we estimated the indirect effect of X (e.g., reappraisal) on Y (e.g., support for aggressive statements) operating through M (e.g., anger), and conducted an inference about that effect using a 95% bootstrap confidence interval (CI). A resulting CI that was entirely above or below zero (i.e., did not include zero) was considered evidence of a mediation effect, whereas a CI that included zero indicated no mediated effect of X on Y through M. Though in this approach the significance of the effects of X on M (e.g., reappraisal on anger) and M on Y (e.g., anger on support for aggressive statements) is not relevant in establishing mediation, as recommended by Hayes and Rockwood (2016) we included information on these effects to supplement the analysis – e.g., checking the consistency of the signs of the X-M and M-Y relationships with the predicted indirect effect.

The Mediating Effect of Negative Emotions on Support for Aggressive Statements

The prediction that negative emotions mediated the relationship between reappraisal and support for aggressive statements was confirmed with the observation that the indirect effect of reappraisal on support for aggressive statements through negative emotions was significant [95% CI did not include zero; (−0.354, −0.001)]. The signs of the associations between reappraisal and negative emotions (negative; B = −3.72, p = 0.016), and between negative emotions and support for aggressive statements (positive, B = 0.03, p = 0.05) were consistent with the predicted mediation effect.

We tested whether fear mediated the relationship between reappraisal and support for aggressive statements. The test of the indirect effect of reappraisal on support for aggressive statements through fear was significant [95% CI did not include zero; (−0.333, −0.002)], which confirmed that fear mediated the effects of reappraisal on support for aggressive statements. The signs of the associations between reappraisal and fear (negative, B = −0.43, p = 0.058), and between fear and support for aggressive statements (positive, B = 0.26, p = 0.011) were consistent with the predicted mediation effect.

Following Halperin et al. (2013), we tested whether anger mediated the relationship between reappraisal and support for negative statement. We found no significant indirect effect of reappraisal on support for aggressive statements through anger [95% CI did include zero; (−0.181, 0.056)].

The Mediating Effect of Negative Emotions on Support for Conciliatory Statements

The test for the indirect effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory statements through negative emotions was significant [95% CI did not include zero; (0.01, 0.20)]; thus, results indicated that negative emotions do mediate the relationship between reappraisal and support for conciliatory statements. The signs of the associations between reappraisal and participants’ negative emotions (negative, B = −3.72, p = 0.016), and between negative emotions and support for conciliatory statements (negative, B = −1.69, p = 0.094) were consistent with the predicted mediation effect.

Regarding fear, the test for an indirect effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory statements through fear was significant [95% CI did not include zero; (0.001, 0.229)], thus indicating that fear mediated the effects of reappraisal on support for conciliatory statements. The signs of the associations between reappraisal and fear (negative, B = −0.43, p = 0.058), and between fear and support for conciliatory statements (negative, B = −0.18, p = 0.015) were consistent with the predicted mediation effect.

Similar to Halperin et al. (2013), we tested whether anger mediated the relationship between reappraisal and support for conciliatory statements. We found that the indirect effect of reappraisal on support for conciliatory statements through anger was significant [95% CI did not include zero; (0.002, 0.189)], thus confirming the predicted mediating role of anger on the relationship between reappraisal and support for conciliatory statements. The signs of the associations between reappraisal and anger (negative, B = −0.44, p = 0.066), and between anger and support for conciliatory statements (negative, B = −0.13, p = 0.057) were consisted with the predicted mediation effect.

Social Desirability

A two-tailed independent-samples t test on the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale scores (Crowne and Marlowe, 1960 – adapted to Spanish by Ferrando and Chico, 2000) indicated no significant differences between reappraisal (M = 17.94, SD = 5.40) and control groups (M = 17.54, SD = 5.25), t(106) = 0.397, p = 0.692. The fact that these scores were between the average reported in Spanish (Ferrando and Chico, 2000) and Mexican (Lara-Cantú, 1990) populations (MSpanish = 15.83, SD = 5.15; MMexican = 19.10, SD = 5.57) indicated that participants in the present study showed an average degree of concern for the social desirability of their responses.

Discussion

The present study tested whether the effects of reappraisal training reported by Halperin et al. (2013) could be reproduced in the context of the armed conflict between the Colombian government and FARC. Reappraisal was expected to decrease the intensity of negative emotions evoked by anger-inducing information related to FARC’s violent actions, and produce more support for conciliatory statements and less support for aggressive statements. Also, we expected that negative emotions would mediate the relationship between reappraisal and support for conciliatory and aggressive statements.

Reappraisal training reduced the levels of negative emotions – anger, irritability, fear, and uneasiness – reported by the participants, with moderate-to-high effect magnitudes. It also significantly increased participants’ support for conciliatory statements related to the peace accord signed between FARC and the Colombian government (Colombian Government, 2016a), which was voted on a national referendum few days after the present experiment took place (e.g., acceptance of FARC’s members’ reincorporation to civil life). Also aligned with the findings reported by Halperin et al. (2013), our results indicated that negative emotions mediated the effects of reappraisal on the participants’ support for aggressive and conciliatory statements.

These findings add to those of previous studies (Gross et al., 2013; Halperin et al., 2013; Halperin, 2014; Rosler et al., 2015; Čehajić-Clancy et al., 2016; Gutentag et al., 2016) that support the notion that negative emotional experiences related to intractable conflicts limit attempts toward peaceful resolution of these conflicts (Gross et al., 2013; Halperin, 2014; Halperin and Pliskin, 2015). The replication of most of the effects observed in previous studies related to the Israeli–Palestinian conflict (Halperin and Gross, 2011; Halperin et al., 2013) suggest a high degree of generality of these phenomena, especially considering the differences between the conflicts approached, e.g., the Colombian conflict is internal, has entailed both state and non-state violence, several actors have been involved (other guerrillas, paramilitary groups, and drug cartels), and diverse types of violence were implemented on the civilian population, including terrorist acts, kidnapping, massacres, landmines, forced recruitment, forced displacement, disappearances, and selective executions (Historical Memory Group, 2016; Unidad para las Victimas, 2017). This aspect shows promise for further research that not only continue testing the generality of these effects on other types of intergroup conflicts across the world, but also attempt their application to generate socially relevant behavioral change (e.g., voting, donating, or volunteering). Also, related studies could explore the specific pathways through which these effects manifest depending on contextual factors of the conflict – e.g., why fear, and not anger, showed more evidence of mediating the relationship between reappraisal and support for conciliatory and aggressive statements in the Colombian context.

Limitations and Further Research

The fact that we could only obtain evidence of an indirect effect of reappraisal on aggressive statements against FARC may relate to the characteristics of the texts that were presented to the participants—i.e., excerpts from the peace accord signed between FARC and Colombian government and aggressive statements from national newspapers. For instance, Correa et al. (2018) found that linguistic aspects of the Colombian government-FARC peace accord made its legibility very low, which adds to the more general finding that legal and political texts are not easily comprehended by lay persons (Charrow and Charrow, 1979; Jones et al., 2012; Mandic et al., 2012). Pilot testing of the different statements used in the present study was not possible due to the close date established for the referendum on the signed peace accord (less than 2 months), which may have resulted in the moderate levels of internal consistency observed for both sets of statements (αconciliatory = 0.65; αaggressive = 0.68). It is recommended that further research addresses these issues, including securing the legibility of the textual stimuli prior to their presentation to the participants (e.g., using data mining to identify the ideal or especially difficult fragments in a given text—Correa et al., 2018—and conducting pilot tests).

Another potential factor that may explain the limited effect of reappraisal on the participants’ attitudes toward the aggressive statements, is the fact that support for these ideas was overall low (scores typically ranged between 2 and 3 in the 6-pt Likert scale, irrespective of group assignment). This floor effect may have resulted from having chosen participants from a target population that was reported to be in favor of the peace accord (urban young adults; Caracol Radio, 2016), and thus against actions that may have compromised it. In addition, different to Halperin et al.’s (2013) experiment, which had a clearly identifiable outgroup, in the present study some participants may not have perceived FARC as an outgroup. Accordingly, further research could test the effects of reappraisal on populations with a different relationship to the conflict and its actors —e.g., victims and members of the armed forces—and thus other attitudes toward the peace process.

The assessment of changes in emotional states is an aspect that needs to be considered for future research. Same as in Halperin et al.’s (2013) study, participants’ emotions were only measured after receiving the cognitive reappraisal training (or control instructions) and the anger-inducing information. This limited the assessment of the effect of reappraisal, as participants’ emotional state at baseline was unknown. Though the strategy of randomly assigning participants to the reappraisal condition provided some degree of control over this potential confound, future studies could implement a pre and post intervention measurement.

Regarding the limited mediational role of anger, which contrasted with Halperin et al.’s (2013) findings, it seems plausible that this outcome relates to the overall low emotional level reported by the participants (below theoretical mean). This may have resulted from exclusion of participants that experienced traumatic events and/or the fact that Colombian young urban population characterizes for absence of strong emotions related to conflict-related information. Ongoing research in our lab is precisely testing the latter notion (Hurtado-Parrado et al., unpublished).

Lastly, the results of the national referendum on the peace accord (50.02% of the votes were against it) indicated a highly polarized public opinion across the Colombian population. This outcome suggests other lines of research that could entail (a) testing whether the effects reported in the present study could be reproduced in the population that voted against the accord, and (b) analyzing the contents and effects of the controversial campaign ran by Colombian opposition party (Centro Democrático) against the accord, which apparently was successful by triggering negative emotions in the voters (e.g., by distorting some of the contents of the accord regarding impunity for the FARC members and the negative economic and political implications of accepting the accord – El Espectador, 2016; El País, 2016). Exploring the role of fear in this context seems particularly relevant, since it was seemingly triggered by the opposition campaign and our data indicates a mediating role for supporting conciliatory and aggressive statements.

Conclusion

Our findings add to those of previous research in showing the potential that resides in efforts to constructively cope with negative emotions that emerge during intractable conflicts. Future research should not only address the limitations of the present study, but also extend it by testing the long-term effects of the reappraisal intervention and its potential influence on actual political behavior (e.g., voting, donating money, or volunteering) and guiding public policy on areas affected by conflict. This is specially relevant in places in which efforts toward conflict resolution are currently being undertaken—as is the Colombian case.

Lastly, the positive effects of cognitive reappraisal training show promise to enhance the processes entailed in the development of peace cultures (Bar-Tal, 2000). This is particularly the case for the Colombian context, in which the much-needed processes of forgiveness and reconciliation seem heavily affected by negative emotions (e.g., feelings of hatred or distrust associated to a construed image of an enemy – Alzate et al., 2009; López-López et al., 2013, 2018; Cortés et al., 2015).

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the ethical guidelines and recommendations of the American Psychological Association – APA (2002) and the Colombian Board of Psychologists (2016). All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Research and Ethics Committee at Konrad Lorenz Fundación Universitaria.

Author Contributions

CH-P, MS-P, MEH, AM, DG-V, JC, WL-L, and KH contributed to the conception, design, and implementation of the study. CH-P, MS-P, AM, DG-V, LV, and AC-C conducted the field work and organized the database. CH-P, MEH, and JR performed the statistical analysis. CH-P wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MEH, MS-P, WL-L revised and extended the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, and read and approved the final version.

Funding

Financial support for this research was provided by Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz – grant number 9IN10181.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank ABA Colombia for the support during the different stages of the project, and Dr. Eran Halperin and Dr. Roni Porat for kindly providing us all the information and materials they used in their study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00908/full#supplementary-material

References

Abello-Llanos, R., Amaris-Macías, M., Blanco-Abarca, A., Madariaga-Orozco, C., Manrique-Palacio, K., Martínez-González, M., et al. (2009). Bienestar y trauma en personas adultas desplazadas por la violencia política. Univ. Psychol. 8, 455–470. doi: 10.11606/S1518-8787.2018052017094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzate, M., Durán, M., and Sabucedo, J. M. (2009). Población civil y transformación constructiva de un conflicto armado interno: aplicación al caso colombiano. Univ. Psychol. 8, 703–720.

Google Scholar

American Psychological Association – APA (2002). Ethical principlesof psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist 57, 1060–1073. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.12.1060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amnesty International (2017). Amnesty International Report 2016-2017. Available at: https://www.amnesty.org/es/countries/americas/colombia/report-colombia/ (accessed August 8, 2017).

Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D. (2000). From intractable conflict through conflict resolution to reconciliation: psychological analysis. Polit. Psychol. 21, 351–365. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D. (1998). Societal beliefs in times of intractable conflict: the Israeli case. Int. J. Confl. Manag. 9, 22–50. doi: 10.1108/eb022803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, J. D., Bolus, R., and Mayer, E. A. (2007). Psychometric properties of the early trauma inventory–self report. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 195, 211–218. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000243824.84651.6c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caracol Radio (2016). El Si en el Plebiscito Lograría el 54% Según Encuesta Sobre Intención de Voto. Available at: http://caracol.com.co/programa/2016/09/23/6am_hoy_por_hoy/1474630703_980326.html (accessed August 8, 2017).

Google Scholar

Čehajić-Clancy, S., Goldenberg, A., Gross, J. J., and Halperin, E. (2016). Social-psychological interventions for intergroup reconciliation: an emotion regulation perspective. Psychol. Inq. 27, 73–88. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2016.1153945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centro Nacional de Memoria Histórica (2013). Guerrilla y Población Civil: Trayectoria de las FARC 1949-2013. Bogotá: Imprenta Nacional.

Google Scholar

Charrow, R. P., and Charrow, V. R. (1979). Making legal language understandable: a psycholinguistic study of jury instructions. Columbia Law Rev. 79, 1306–1374. doi: 10.2307/1121842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombian Board of Psychologists (2006). Article 12 of Law1090 of 2006. Reg. Deontological and Disciplinary Process of Professional Psychology Exercise. The National Congress of Colombia. Available at: http://colpsic.org.co/aym_image/files/LEY_1090_DE_2006.pdf (accessed July 14, 2017).

Google Scholar

Colombian Government (2016a). Acuerdo Final Para la Terminación del Conflicto y la Construcción de una paz Estable y Duradera. Available at: http://www.urnadecristal.gov.co/sites/default/files/acuerdo-final-habana.pdf (accessed August 10, 2017).

Google Scholar

Colombian Government (2016b). Acuerdo Final Para la Terminación del Conflicto y la Construcción de una paz Estable y Duradera. Bogotá: Colombian Government.

Google Scholar

Correa, J. C., García-Chivita, M., and García-Vargas, G. R. (2018). A text mining approach to the text difficulty of Latin American peace agreement. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 50, 61–70.

Google Scholar

Cortés, A., Torres, A., López-López, W., Pérez, D. C., and Pineda-Marin, C. (2015). Comprensiones sobre el perdón y la reconciliación en el contexto del conflicto armado colombiano. Psychos. Interv. 25, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psi.2015.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowne, D. P., and Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J. Consult. Psychol. 24, 349–354. doi: 10.1037/h0047358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diario del Huila (2016). Con lo Firmado en La Habana, Ganan las FARC y Pierde el País: Interview with Colombian Senator Ernesto Macías. Available at: http://www.centrodemocratico.com/?q=articulo/con-lo-firmado-en-la-habana-ganan-las-farc-y-pierde-el-pais-macias (accessed August 10, 2017).

Google Scholar

Dufey, M., and Fernández, A. M. (2012). Validez y confiabilidad del positive affect and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en estudiantes universitarios chilenos. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación Psicológica 34, 157–173.

Google Scholar

El Espectador (2016). La Cuestionable Estrategia de Campaña del “No” [The Questionable Campaign of the “no”]. Available at: http://www.elespectador.com/noticias/politica/cuestionable-estrategia-de-campana-del-no-articulo-658862 (accessed August 10, 2017).

Google Scholar

El País (2016). Las Polémicas Revelaciones de Promotor del No Sobre Estrategia en el Plebiscito [The Polemic Revelations of the “No” Promoter Regarding the Strategy During the Plebiscite]. Available at: http://www.elpais.com.co/proceso-de-paz/las-polemicas-revelaciones-de-promotor-del-no-sobre-estrategia-en-el-plebiscito.html (accessed August 10, 2017).

Google Scholar

Ferrando, P. J., and Chico, E. (2000). Adaptación y análisis psicométrico de la escala de deseabilidad social de Marlowe y Crowne. Psicothema 12, 383–389.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Frijda, N. H. (2009). Emotions, individual differences and time course: reflections. Cogn. Emot. 23, 1444–1461. doi: 10.1080/02699930903093276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gignac, G. E., and Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Pers. Individ. Diff. 102, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grajales, J. (2011). The rifle and the title: paramilitary violence, land grab and land control in Colombia. J. Peas. Stud. 38, 771–792. doi: 10.1080/03066150.2011.607701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward. Emotion 13, 359–365. doi: 10.1037/a0032135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2014). “Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 3–20.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., Halperin, E., and Porat, R. (2013). Emotion regulation in intractable conflicts. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 423–429. doi: 10.1177/0963721413495871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutentag, T., Halperin, E., Porat, R., Bigman, Y. E., and Tamir, M. (2016). Successful emotion regulation requires both conviction and skill: beliefs about the controllability of emotions, reappraisal, and regulation success. Cogn. Emot. 31, 1225–1233. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1213704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, E. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and conflict resolution. Emot. Rev. 6, 68–76. doi: 10.1177/1754073913491844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, E., and Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion regulation in violent conflict: reappraisal, hope, and support for humanitarian aid to the opponent in wartime. Cogn. Emot. 25, 1228–1236. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.536081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, E., and Pliskin, R. (2015). Emotions and emotion regulation in intractable conflict: studying emotional processes within a unique context. Polit. Psychol. 36, 119–150. doi: 10.1111/pops.12236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, E., Porat, R., Tamir, M., and Gross, J. J. (2013). Can emotion regulation change political attitudes in intractable conflicts? From the laboratory to the field. Psychol. Sci. 24, 106–111. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., and Rockwood, N. J. (2016). Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: observations, recommendations, and implementation. Behav. Res. Ther. 98, 39–57. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Historical Memory Group (2016). Basta ya! Colombia: Memories of War and Dignity. Bogotá: Centro Nacional de Memoria Histórica.

Google Scholar

Hu, T., Zhang, D., Wang, J., Mistry, R., Ran, G., and Wang, X. (2014). Relation between emotion regulation and mental health: a meta-analysis review. Psychol. Rep. 114, 341–362. doi: 10.2466/03.20.PR0.114k22w4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Human Rights Watch (2015). World Report 2015: Colombia. Available at: https://www.hrw.org/world-report/2015/country-chapters/colombia (accessed July 14, 2017).

Google Scholar

Jones, N., McDavid, J., Derthick, K., Dowell, R., and Spyridakis, J. (2012). Plain language in environmental policy documents: an assessment of reader comprehension and perceptions. J. Techn. Writ. Commun. 42, 331–371. doi: 10.2190/TW.42.4.b

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriesberg, L. (1993). Intractable conflicts. Peace Rev. 5, 417–421. doi: 10.1080/10402659308425753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lara-Cantú, M. A. (1990). Validez y confiabilidad de la escala de deseabilidad social de Marlowe y Crowne en una población de adultos. Salud Mental 13, 35–37.

Google Scholar

López-López, W., Pineda Marín, C., Murcia León, M. C., Perilla Garzón, D. C., and Mullet, E. (2013). Forgiving perpetrators of violence: colombian people’s positions. Soc. Indicat. Res. 114, 287–301. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0146-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-López, W., Sandoval Alvarado, G., Rodríguez, S., Ruiz, C., León, J. D., Pineda Marín, C., et al. (2018). Forgiving former perpetrators of violence and reintegrating them into Colombian civil society: non-combatant citizens’ positions. J. Peace Psychol. 24, 201–215. doi: 10.1037/pac0000295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandic, C. G., Rudd, R., Hehir, T., and Acevedo-Garcia, D. (2012). Readability of special education procedural safeguards. J. Spec. Educ. 45, 195–203. doi: 10.1177/0022466910362774

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Páez, D., Martínez-Sánchez, F., Mendiburo, A., Bobowik, M., and Sevillano, V. (2013). Affect regulation strategies and perceived emotional adjustment for negative and positive affect: a study on anger, sadness and joy. J. Posit. Psychol. 8, 249–262. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2013.786751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picó-Pérez, M., Radua, J., Steward, T., Menchón, J. M., and Soriano-Mas, C. (2017). Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Registraduria Nacional del Estado Civil (2016). Plebiscito 2 Octubre 2016 – Boletín Nacional No. 53. Bogotá: Registraduría Nacional de Estado Civil.

Google Scholar

Richards, J. M., and Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 410–424. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosler, N., Cohen-Chen, S., and Halperin, E. (2015). The distinctive effects of empathy and hope in intractable conflicts. J. Confl. Resol. 61, 114–139. doi: 10.1177/0022002715569772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, F. (2011). The agrarian question and violence in colombia: conflict and development. J. Agrar. Change 11, 321–356. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0366.2011.00314.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unidad para las Victimas (2017). Registro Único de Víctimas en Colombia. Bogotá: Unidad para las victimas.

Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, T. L., Miles, E., and Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychol. Bull. 138, 775–808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion regulation, cognitive reappraisal, conciliation, aggression, peace, intractable conflict, Colombia, FARC-EP

Citation: Hurtado-Parrado C, Sierra-Puentes M, El Hazzouri M, Morales A, Gutiérrez-Villamarín D, Velásquez L, Correa-Chica A, Rincón JC, Henao K, Castañeda JG and López-López W (2019) Emotion Regulation and Attitudes Toward Conflict in Colombia: Effects of Reappraisal Training on Negative Emotions and Support for Conciliatory and Aggressive Statements. Front. Psychol. 10:908. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00908

Received: 14 December 2018; Accepted: 04 April 2019;
Published: 24 April 2019.

Edited by:

Changiz Mohiyeddini, Northeastern University, United States

Reviewed by:

Dario Paez, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Spain
Jonathan Greenberg, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, United States

Copyright © 2019 Hurtado-Parrado, Sierra-Puentes, El Hazzouri, Morales, Gutiérrez-Villamarín, Velásquez, Correa-Chica, Rincón, Henao, Castañeda and López-López. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Camilo Hurtado-Parrado, hhurtadoparrado@troy.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.