Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 April 2019
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Profiles of Impulsivity in Problematic Internet Users and Cigarette Smokers

  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy and Sociology, Jilin University, Changchun, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, School of Medical Humanitarians, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China
  • 3Department of Sociology, Wuhan University, Wuhan, China

Problematic Internet use (PIU) has been gradually recognized as a mental health issue among adolescents and young students. PIU shows many similarities with substance use disorders, but the shared and distinct mechanisms underlying them are unclear. The purpose of the current study was to explore the relationships between impulsive traits and PIU as well as cigarette smoking behaviors among young adults. Two independent samples of university students (N1 = 1281, N2 = 1034, respectively) over 3 years were assessed with multiple measurements of impulsivity, including the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11), the UPPSP Impulsive Behaviors Scale (UPPSP), and the Delay-discounting Test (DDT). Logistic regression models revealed that across the two independent samples, BIS-11 Attentional Impulsiveness was the common trait positively predicting both PIU and cigarette smoking. While BIS-11 Motor Impulsiveness as well as UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency were the typical traits linked to PIU as positive predictors, UPPSP Sensation Seeking was the unique trait linked to cigarette smoking as a positive predictor. These results suggested that specific dimensions of impulsivity might be concurrently implicated in PIU and cigarette smoking among young adults, putatively representing important trait marks for addictive behaviors.

Introduction

Problematic Internet use or pathological Internet use (PIU), also regarded as Internet addiction (IA), is defined as an inability of individuals to control use of the Internet with various psychological and social problems (Young, 1998; Davis, 2001). Prevalence estimates of PIU in adolescents range from 1.0 to 9.0% among United States and European samples and from 2.0 to 18.0% in Asian samples (Spada, 2014). In the latest edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (i.e., DSM-5), Internet gaming disorder (IGD), a most prevalent form of PIU, has been included as a condition that requires future research in order to be considered a full disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Moreover, together with gambling disorder, IGD has been proposed in the list of addictive conditions in the latest revised version of the International Classification of Diseases (i.e., ICD-11; Potenza, 2018).

As a putative behavioral addiction similar with gambling disorder, PIU shares many clinical manifestations with substance use disorders (SUD), including excessive use of the Internet (e.g., excessive gaming and sexual preoccupations) with a loss of time sense, withdrawal symptoms (e.g., feelings of anger, tension, and depression), tolerance, and negative repercussions (Block, 2008). However, it is highly controversial whether PIU should be formally considered a new clinical disorder (Petry and O’brien, 2013), and more empirical studies are required to detect the shared and distinct aspects and mechanisms between PIU and SUD for a better comprehending of the nature of PIU.

Impulsivity is a multifaceted trait that is known as a characteristic construct of many mental disorders including addictive behaviors (Verdejo-García et al., 2008; De Wit, 2009; Meda et al., 2009), although its underlying neurobiological bases (e.g., impulsive disinhibition/decreased inhibitory control, impulsive inattention, impulsive decision-making, delay discounting) are highly heterogeneous (Winstanley et al., 2006; Reynolds et al., 2008). Measurement of impulsivity includes self-report personality questionnaires and laboratory-based cognitive tasks. For the former, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton et al., 1995) measures the three-dimension model of impulsiveness (i.e., non-planning impulsiveness, attentional impulsiveness, and motor impulsiveness), and the UPPSP Impulsive Behaviors Scale (UPPSP; Whiteside and Lynam, 2001; Smith et al., 2007) figures five distinct pathways to impulsive behavior (i.e., negative urgency, positive urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking). For the latter, the Stop-Signal Test (Logan et al., 1997), Stroop Task (MacLeod, 1991), and analogous Go/No-Go Task mainly tax inhibitory control and response inhibition. Besides, the Delay-discounting Test (DDT; Kirby et al., 1999) assesses the discounting degree of delayed values when individuals make a choice between immediate and delayed rewards. The close connections of impulsivity with addictive behaviors have been well recognized in plentiful previous studies. Higher impulsivity traits measured by self-report questionnaires (e.g., the BIS-11) and impaired inhibitory control measured by cognitive tasks (e.g., the Stop-Signal Test) have been found in different forms of substance use disorders as well as in gambling disorder (Dick et al., 2010; Albein-Urios et al., 2012; Leeman and Potenza, 2012). Consistently, many studies have also revealed dysfunctional impulsivity properties in PIU on the BIS-11 (Cao et al., 2007; Lin et al., 2011; Dalbudak et al., 2013; Ryu et al., 2018), and on the Stop-Signal Test and Go/No-Go Task (Dong et al., 2010; Choi et al., 2014; Ding et al., 2014). Particularly, PIU subjects showed increased impulsivity traits comparable to patients with gambling disorder on the BIS-11 (Lee et al., 2012). These findings signified the candidacy of impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for different addictive disorders, and impulsivity might also serve as an aspect in identifying the nature of PIU as a potential addiction (Lee et al., 2012; Grant and Chamberlain, 2014). However, little research has directly compared PIU with substance use disorders (SUD) on impulsivity, and the shared and distinct mechanisms underlying them remain unclear. The present study thus aimed to investigate the associations of impulsivity traits with PIU and one usual type of SUD, cigarette smoking, among Chinese college students.

In contrast to other segments of society, young adult college students are believed to be more involved in PIU, because of their easy access to the Internet and rapid psychological and biological challenges during the late adolescence and young adulthood (Kandell, 1998; Li et al., 2010). According to the 42nd China Statistical Report on Internet Development 2018 by the China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC), there are 802 million Internet users in China, of which 24.8% were young students and 27.9% were aged 20–29. These youths (so-called post-1990s generation), unlike their predecessors who are considered introverted, submissive, self-restrained, and responsible, have been labeled extroversive, promiscuous, imprudent, and impulsive as the economic wealth exploded prosperously (Rosen, 2009; Yang and Zheng, 2012), which might pave the way for a greater risk of PIU. Indeed, addictive Internet use among Chinese adolescents and college students has been found to be connected with higher levels of impulsivity measured by the BIS-11 (Cao et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2015; Du et al., 2016), the Go/No-Go Task (Dong et al., 2010; Ding et al., 2014), and the DDT (Li et al., 2016; Tian et al., 2018). Interestingly, gender differences on the prevalence of PIU were evident among younger adolescent students, with a higher rate in males (Sun et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2013; Liang et al., 2016); however, this tendency appeared to vanish between males and females in adult college students (Ni et al., 2009; Yan et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2016; Nie et al., 2017). Although females are traditionally recognized as less impulsive and more compliant than males in China, and are less likely being active games players than males (Li and Kirkup, 2007), they could still plunge into impulsive online shopping and network communication activities in college (Wang et al., 2012), which might partly account for the vanished differences.

Analogous to the Internet use, cigarette smoking, which is not prohibited by law in most Western countries as well as in China, is accessible for youths and has been increasing among students from middle and high schools to universities (Alexander et al., 2001; Chen et al., 2004; Reed et al., 2007; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2012). The prevalence of cigarette smoking was very high in Chinese adults aged 35–74 years, with 60.2% of men and 6.9% of women being current cigarette smokers (Gu et al., 2004). The Chinese college students are also holding a high risk for cigarette smoking, and the overall prevalence of current smokers is about 29–29.8% (45.1–49% for males and 5–6% for females) (Chen et al., 2004; Zhu et al., 2004; Mao et al., 2008). Similar to PIU, cigarette smoking behavior has also been found to be related to impulsivity. Smokers are typically more impulsive than non-smokers on self-report personality measures (Mitchell, 1999; Reynolds et al., 2007; Flory and Manuck, 2009; Balevich et al., 2013; Schulte et al., 2017), as well as on behavioral choice tasks using the discounting paradigm (Bickel et al., 1999; Baker et al., 2003; Reynolds et al., 2004; Ohmura et al., 2005; Białaszek et al., 2017). A recent meta-analysis of the relationships between specific impulsivity traits and cigarette smoking showed that the UPPSP model of impulsivity was significantly associated with smoking status and severity of nicotine dependence in adults (Kale et al., 2018). Although cigarette smoking is considered a daily-life consuming behavior similar to the Internet use to some degree, it does possess the core addictive features as a typical category of SUD (e.g., intake of substance, toxic effects), which are not necessarily seen in PIU. Therefore, a direct comparison between cigarette smoking and PIU on impulsivity might be conducive to dissociating the effects of substance use (i.e., nicotine) on impulsivity traits from the impulsive vulnerabilities that predispose these problematic behaviors (Verdejo-García et al., 2008). It is assumed that vulnerability-related impulsive aspects could be present in both behaviors, whereas specific impulsivity traits associated with nicotine use might only exist in cigarette smoking but not in PIU.

In the current study, we directly compared the impulsivity profiles between problematic Internet users and cigarette smokers in a large cross-sectional sample of Chinese college students by using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), the UPPSP Impulsive Behaviors Scale, and the Delay Discounting Test (DDT). Moreover, in view of the fast-growing popularity of the Internet information technologies (e.g., gaming, shopping, online payment, and social networking) among the new generation arising in China (i.e., post-1990s), we simultaneously included two independent samples across 3 years (selected in the year 2014 and 2017, respectively), so as to test the possible development of PIU and smoking behaviors among the young college students. In addition, previous reports have been discrepant on the relationships of impulsivity with PIU and smoking behaviors. For instance, some studies revealed robust associations of impulsivity traits (e.g., on the BIS-11 and DDT) with PIU (Saville et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016; Tian et al., 2018), whereas others failed to detect analogous connections (Rømer Thomsen et al., 2018; Zhou et al., 2018). Thus the inclusion of two separate data collections, which took place 3 years apart from each other, was also aimed at verifying the consistency of the relationships between impulsivity with PIU and smoking behaviors in the present study.

The objective of this study was twofold: (1) to facilitate the understanding of the nature of PIU by a straightforward comparison with cigarette smoking on impulsivity facets; and (2) to gather more empirical evidence about specific impulsivity-related traits linked to PIU and/or cigarette smoking as potential vulnerability candidates. We hypothesized that certain aspects of impulsivity (e.g., cognitive impulsiveness) might be commonly associated with PIU and smoking as potential vulnerability-related traits, while some domain-specific traits (e.g., sensation seeking, delay discounting) would be distinctly involved in each behavior, linked to PIU or cigarette smoking separately.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

The participants included two independent sample cohorts (i.e., Sample 1 and Sample 2) of young adult students from a local university located in Guiyang City, the capital of Guizhou Province, China. The Sample 1 consisted of 1,318 students, who were recruited from 12 randomly selected freshman classes at the university in November 2014. Similarly, the Sample 2 consisted of 1,060 students recruited from 10 randomly selected freshman classes at the same university in November 2017. As mentioned before, the purpose of recruiting these two separate samples was to investigate the possible over-time effects on PIU, smoking, and impulsivity.

All of these students were kindly invited to complete a series of questionnaires and provide demographic information by self-report in a psychology class lasting 45 min. The inclusion criteria included: (1) ≥18 years of age, and (2) willingness to participate in our study. The exclusion criteria included: (1) current or past major psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder), (2) a history of brain injury or trauma, neurological diseases, or mental disorders, and (3) ever or current use of psychoactive substance (e.g., opioids, cocaine, marijuana, and amphetamine), which were evaluated by self-report. There were 37 students in the Sample 1 and 26 students in the Sample 2 excluded from this study because of meeting one or more of these exclusion criteria.

Thus finally, 1,281 participants in the Sample 1 (aged 18–23 years, mean age = 19.1 ± 1.1 years; 434 males, 33.9%) and 1,034 participants in the Sample 2 (aged 18–23 years, mean age = 19.2 ± 1.1 years; 405 males, 39.2%) were included in this study. All subjects supplied written informed consent, and they were compensated with a gift equivalent to RMB ¥ 50 for their time. The Human Research Ethics Committee at the Guizhou Medical University thoroughly reviewed and approved this study. Our proposed study design, subject recruitment process, and our plans to compensate the participants were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Group Classification

The Internet use status was classified by employing the Chinese version of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT; Young, 1998), which has been carefully validated and widely used among Chinese students (e.g., Cao et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011; Dong et al., 2012; Tang et al., 2014). The IAT is a self-report questionnaire with 20 items. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (always). The IAT total score ranges from 20 to 100. Higher scores indicate a greater tendency of excessive Internet use symptoms. Scores ≥ 50 indicate potential problematic Internet use (PIU), while scores 20–49 indicate normal Internet use (NIU) (Khazaal et al., 2008). The Cronbach’s α was 0.92 (Sample 1) and 0.90 (Sample 2) in this study.

The smoking status was evaluated by using the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al., 1991), which is a 6-item scale designed for measuring the degree of nicotine dependence. The total score on the FTND is 10, with higher scores indicating more nicotine use symptoms (Fagerström et al., 1996; Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2008). We adopted the Chinese version of the FTND, which has showed acceptable reliability and validity in previous research (e.g., Chen et al., 2002; Yang et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2012). The Cronbach’s α was 0.86 (Sample 1) and 0.87 (Sample 2) in our study. Subjects were divided into subgroups according to their FTND scores and smoking history. Non-smokers were defined as persons who had never smoked cigarettes throughout their lifetime and scored 0 on the FTND. While smokers, including current and past smokers, were defined as persons who smoked at least one cigarette per day during past or current days and scored ≥ 1 on the FTND.

Impulsivity Measures

We used the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11; Patton et al., 1995) to measure three dimensions of impulsivity, including non-planning impulsiveness (a tendency to plan and think carelessly), attentional impulsiveness (refers to difficulties in focusing on a task and cognitive instability such as racing thoughts and thought insertion), and motor impulsiveness (a tendency to act on the spur of the moment). Each of the three dimensions consists of 10 items, with each item rated on a 4-point Likert scale. Sum scores of the three dimensions were obtained for further analyses, respectively, with higher scores indicating higher levels of impulsivity. In our study, the Chinese version of BIS-11 (Li et al., 2011) was adopted. The Cronbach’s α was 0.77–0.89 for the three subscales.

The UPPSP Impulsive Behaviors Scale (UPPSP; Whiteside and Lynam, 2001; Smith et al., 2007) was also used to evaluate five distinct personality traits of impulsive behavior (i.e., negative urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, sensation seeking, and positive urgency). The UPPSP is a 59-item self-report inventory rated on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 4 = strongly disagree). The total score for each dimension was obtained for analyses. Higher scores indicate higher levels of impulsivity. The Chinese version of UPPSP has been used properly among college students in our previous study (Yan et al., 2016), and was employed in the present study. The Cronbach’s α was 0.75–0.84 for the five subscales.

The Delay-discounting Test (DDT; Kirby et al., 1999) was used to evaluate the discounting degree of delayed rewards in monetary choice. Subjects have to make a decision between a smaller but immediate reward and a larger but delayed reward. It has been supposed that delay discounting represents a tendency that subjects prefer the smaller immediate rewards to the larger delayed rewards (Dixon et al., 2003), signifying an impulsive behavior style in decision making. There are a set of 27 coupled choices on the DDT. The delay discounting degree was determined by using the hyperbolic equation V = A/(1 + kD), in which k is a free parameter describing the degree of discounting (see details in Kirby et al., 1999). A higher degree of delay discounting is described by a larger k-value. In our study, we adopted a culturally adapted Chinese version among college students (Sun and Li, 2011), which has been closely reported in our previous study (Yan et al., 2018). The k-values were calculated and log-transformed in order to get a normal distribution.

Data Analysis

Data were administered and analyzed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows, Version 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Differences on the categorical data (i.e., home locality, ethnicity, and gender) between groups were analyzed with chi-square tests. Scores on impulsivity measurements were compared between PIU and NIU groups with t-tests. As the group difference on gender was significant between smokers and non-smokers, the 2 (group: smokers, non-smokers) × 2 (gender: male, female) multivariate analysis of variance (mANOVA) model was employed to compare impulsivity scores of smokers and non-smokers. Impulsivity scores between non-smoking NIU, pure PIU, pure smoking, and smoking PIU subgroups were compared using the mANOVA model. Post hoc comparisons were analyzed with Fisher’s least significant differences protected t-tests. Partial correlations were tested between IAT/FTND scores and impulsivity measurements. Multiple linear regressions were employed to analyze the effects of impulsivity traits on IAT and FTND scores. Furthermore, logistic regression analyses were used to test the effects of different traits of impulsivity on PIU and smoking behaviors, controlling for gender. Multicollinearity was tested for all independent variables and outcome variables. Collinearity was not a problem for any variable according to the variance inflation factor (VIF < 10). Statistical significance was set as p < 0.05, two-tailed.

Results

Demographic Characteristics of the Sample 1 and Sample 2

For the purpose of investigating the possible development of problematic Internet use (PIU) and cigarette smoking conditions among young college students, the differences between Sample 1 and Sample 2 were tested on demographics, PIU and smoking status, and impulsivity scores. As seen in Table 1, there were no significant differences on age, ethnicity, and home locality between these two samples (ps > 0.05). However, the proportion of males in Sample 2 was moderately higher than that in Sample 1 (39.2% vs. 33.9%, χ2 = 6.925, p = 0.008). The average IAT and FTND scores, as well as the proportion of problematic Internet users (PIUs), smokers, and smoking PIUs, were comparable in the two samples (ps > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the two samples.

Profiles on Impulsivity of Problematic Internet Users

The demographics and impulsivity scores of the problematic Internet users (PIUs) were compared with the normal Internet users (NIUs) across two samples to present a general impulsivity profile of the PIUs, without excluding the confounding effects of smoking. As depicted in Table 2, no significant between-group differences were detected on age, gender, ethnicity, home locality, or smoking status (i.e., the FTND scores and the proportion of smokers) between PIUs and NIUs (ps > 0.05). On the BIS-11, PIUs scored higher than NIUs in both samples on Motor Impulsiveness (t = 8.494, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.48; t = 8.105, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.50, respectively), Attentional Impulsiveness (t = 9.677, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.54; t = 8.429, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.52, respectively), and Non-planning Impulsiveness (t = 7.114, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.40; t = 6.005, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.37, respectively). On the UPPSP, PIUs scored higher than NIUs in both samples on Lack of Perseverance (t = 7.114, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.40; t = 7.793, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.49, respectively), Lack of Premeditation (t = 3.640, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.20; t = 4.251, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.26, respectively), Negative Urgency (t = 8.601, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.48; t = 7.744, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.48, respectively), and Positive Urgency (t = 6.806, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.38; t = 5.993, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.37, respectively) except on Sensation Seeking (ps > 0.05). On the DDT k-values, PIUs did not differ from NIUs in any of the two samples (ps > 0.05).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic characteristics and impulsivity scores of PIUs and NIUs in each sample.

Profiles on Impulsivity of Cigarette Smokers

The demographics and impulsivity scores of the smokers were compared with the non-smokers across two samples to depict a general impulsivity profile of the smokers, without excluding the confounding effects of PIU. As seen in Table 3, no significant group differences were observed for age, ethnicity, home locality, or Internet use status (i.e., the IAT scores and the proportion of PIUs) between smokers and non-smokers (ps > 0.05). But the proportion of males was significantly higher in smokers than that in non-smokers for both samples (χ2 = 112.910, p < 0.001; χ2 = 62.938, p < 0.001, respectively). Thus in further analyses, gender was controlled as a between-group variable using the 2 (group: smokers, non-smokers) × 2 (gender: male, female) mANOVA models. On the BIS-11, smokers scored higher than non-smokers in both samples on Motor Impulsiveness [F(1,1277) = 6.773, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.018, Cohen’s d = 0.19; F(1,1030) = 11.618, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.021, Cohen’s d = 0.24, respectively] and Attentional Impulsiveness [F(1,1277) = 5.134, p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.017, Cohen’s d = 0.22; F(1,1030) = 4.327, p = 0.038, ηp2 = 0.014, Cohen’s d = 0.19, respectively] except on Non-planning Impulsiveness [F(1,1277) = 0.517, p = 0.472; F(1,1030) = 0.403, p = 0.526, respectively]. On the UPPSP, smokers had higher scores than non-smokers in both samples on Sensation Seeking [F(1,1277) = 5.867, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.040, Cohen’s d = 0.15; F(1,1030) = 4.886, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.049, Cohen’s d = 0.23, respectively], but not on Lack of Perseverance [F(1,1277) = 0.716, p = 0.398; F(1,1030) = 0.166, p = 0.684, respectively], Lack of Premeditation [F(1,1277) = 0.788, p = 0.375; F(1,1030) = 0.088, p = 0.767, respectively], Negative Urgency [F(1,1277) = 0.045, p = 0.832; F(1,1030) = 0.416, p = 0.519, respectively], or Positive Urgency [F(1,1277) = 0.881, p = 0.348; F(1,1030) = 0.001, p = 0.973, respectively]. Besides, no significant main effects of gender or interaction effects of group × gender were found on the BIS-11 and UPPSP scores (ps > 0.05). There were no significant group differences between smokers and non-smokers on the DDT [F(1,1277) = 1.302, p = 0.254; F(1,1030) = 0.072, p = 0.789, respectively], nor main effects of gender or interaction effects of group × gender on the DDT across the two samples (ps > 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Demographic characteristics and impulsivity scores of Smokers and Non-smokers in each sample.

Comparison of Impulsivity Between Pure PIUs and Pure Smokers

In order to directly compare the impulsivity profiles of the pure PIUs and pure smokers by excluding the potential effects of comorbidity between PIU and smoking, we divided the subjects into four subgroups in each sample: non-smoking normal Internet users (NIUs), pure PIUs, pure smokers, and smoking PIUs. Supplementary Tables S1, S2 in the Supplementary Material displayed the demographics and impulsivity scores of these four subgroups in Sample 1 and Sample 2, respectively. Considering the few observations of smoking PIUs in each sample and the similar and comparable primary characteristics (e.g., age, ethnicity, home locality, Internet use status, smoking status, and impulsivity scores) of the two samples, we then pooled them together (N = 2315) for more reliable group comparisons. The Chow test for data pooling showed equal linear regression coefficients between impulsivity traits and IAT/FTND scores [F(9,2297) = 1.305, p < 0.05 for IAT; F(9,2297) = 1.418, p < 0.05 for FTND, respectively] on two data sets (i.e., Sample 1 and Sample 2). The merged data were displayed in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Impulsivity scores of Non-smoking NIUs, pure PIUs, pure Smokers, and Smoking PIUs.

There were no significant group differences on age, ethnicity, or home locality between the four subgroups (ps > 0.05), while the proportion of males was significantly higher in pure smokers and smoking PIUs than that in non-smoking NIUs and PIUS (p < 0.001). Therefore, gender was controlled as a covariate in the mANOVA models, with the subgroup as the independent variable and impulsivity scores on the BIS-11, UPPSP, and DDT as the dependent variables. Significant between-group effects were displayed on BIS-11 Motor Impulsiveness [F(3,2307) = 59.928, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.072], Attentional Impulsiveness [F(3,2307) = 69.828, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.083], and Non-planning Impulsiveness [F(3,2307) = 38.507, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.048], as well as on UPPSP Sensation Seeking [F(3,2307) = 17.342, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.027], Lack of Perseverance [F(3,2307) = 41.444, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.051], Lack of Premeditation [F(3,2307) = 13.558, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.017], Negative Urgency [F(3,2307) = 53.526, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.065], and Positive Urgency [F(3,2307) = 31.484, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.039], but not on the DDT (p > 0.05). Post hoc comparisons were then tested on the BIS-11 and UPPSP.

On the BIS-11, pure PIUs scored higher than non-smoking NIUs on Motor Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.76), Attentional Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.81), and Non-planning Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.59). Pure smokers also scored higher than non-smoking NIUs on Motor Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.41), Attentional Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.57), and Non-planning Impulsiveness (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.24). However, pure PIUs did not differ from pure smokers on any of these scores on the BIS-11 (ps > 0.05).

On the UPPSP, pure PIUs scored higher than non-smoking NIUs on Lack of Perseverance (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.63), Lack of Premeditation (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.34), Negative Urgency (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.73), and Positive Urgency (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.57), except on Sensation Seeking (p > 0.05). By contrast, pure smokers scored higher than non-smoking NIUs on Lack of Perseverance (p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.27), Negative Urgency (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.31), Positive Urgency (p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.30), and Sensation Seeking (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.40), except on Lack of Premeditation (p > 0.05). Nevertheless, no significant differences were indicated between pure PIUs and pure smokers on the UPPSP scores (ps > 0.05).

Relationships Between Impulsivity Measures and IAT/FTND Scores

The relationships between IAT/FTND scores and impulsivity measures were tested with partial correlations, controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and home locality. As described in Table 5, IAT scores were positively correlated with the BIS Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness, and UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, Negative Urgency, and Positive Urgency (rp = 0.179–0.420, ps < 0.001), except UPPSP Sensation Seeking and DDT log-transformed k-value (ps > 0.05), across the two samples. FTND scores were positively associated with the BIS Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness, and UPPSP Sensation Seeking, Negative Urgency, and Positive Urgency (rp = 0.059–0.366, ps < 0.05), except UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and DDT log-transformed k-value (ps > 0.05) for both samples.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Partial correlations (rp) between IAT scores, FTND scores and impulsivity measures in each sample.

Predictive Effects of Impulsivity Traits on PIU and Cigarette Smoking

The multiple linear regressions were used to analyze the effects of impulsivity scores (i.e., BIS-11, UPPSP, and DDT) on the IAT and FTND scores. Gender was also entered as a predictor, given the significant group differences on gender. As shown in Table 6, BIS Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, and UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency were positive predictors of IAT scores, whereas gender (male), BIS Attentional Impulsiveness, and UPPSP Sensation Seeking were positive predictors of FTND scores across the two samples.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Multiple linear regression analyses of impulsivity measures on IAT and FTND scores in each sample.

Furthermore, we also adopted logistic regression models to test the effects of impulsivity dimensions on PIU and smoking behaviors, by distinguishing pure PIUs and pure smokers from Non-smoking NIUs with specific impulsive traits (excluding the comorbidity between PIU and smoking). Two binary regression models were conducted to compare the matched subgroups in both samples, that is, pure PIUs vs. Non-smoking NIUs for PIU, and pure smokers vs. Non-smoking NIUs for cigarette smoking, not including the subgroup of smoking PIUs. The impulsivity dimensions and gender were entered as independent variables in the regression models.

Table 7 showed that in both samples, BIS Motor Impulsiveness (OR = 1.100, p < 0.01; OR = 1.133, p < 0.01, respectively), Attentional Impulsiveness (OR = 1.113, p < 0.01; OR = 1.107, p < 0.05, respectively), as well as UPPSP Lack of Perseverance (OR = 1.106, p < 0.01; OR = 1.142, p < 0.001, respectively), Lack of Premeditation (OR = 1.083, p < 0.01; OR = 1.072, p < 0.01, respectively), and Negative Urgency (OR = 1.062, p < 0.01; OR = 1.063, p < 0.01, respectively), positively predicted PIU in the pure PIUs vs. Non-smoking NIUs models (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.202, 0.209, respectively). By contrast, BIS Attentional Impulsiveness (OR = 1.125, p < 0.01; OR = 1.189, p < 0.01, respectively) and UPPSP Sensation Seeking (OR = 1.172, p < 0.001; OR = 1.123, p < 0.001, respectively) positively predicted cigarette smoking in the pure smokers vs. Non-smoking NIUs models (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.368, 0.282, respectively).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Logistic regression analyses of impulsivity traits on PIU and smoking in each sample.

Discussion

This study depicted the profiles of impulsivity in problematic Internet use (PIU) and cigarette smoking across two independent samples of Chinese college students. Our data showed that pure problematic Internet users (PIUs) had elevated scores than non-smoking normal Internet users (NIUs) on the BIS-11 (Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, and Non-planning Impulsiveness) and on the UPPSP (Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, Negative Urgency, and Positive Urgency). Pure cigarette smokers also scored higher than non-smoking NIUs on the BIS-11 (Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, and Non-planning Impulsiveness) and on the UPPSP (Lack of Perseverance, Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, and Sensation Seeking). Significant positive correlations were found between IAT and FTND scores and most impulsivity traits on the BIS-11 and UPPSP. More interesting findings in this study were from the logistic regression models, revealing that BIS Attentional Impulsiveness was the common trait positively predicting both PIU and cigarette smoking. Additionally, although BIS Motor Impulsiveness and UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency characteristically predicted PIU, UPPSP Sensation Seeking predicted cigarette smoking uniquely. In particular, all of these findings were coherently detected across the two separate data collections (i.e., Sample 1 and Sample 2), which occurred 3 years apart from each other. As a whole, these results support our hypotheses that PIU as a candidate of addiction shares some basic mechanisms on impulsivity (Attentional Impulsiveness) with cigarette smoking, although each of these two problem behaviors could be characterized by linking to specific impulsive traits (i.e., Motor Impulsiveness, Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency vs. Sensation Seeking), representing a commonality-specificity profile of impulsivity implicated in addiction.

Despite its multidimensional nature, impulsivity as a core pathological trait of addiction has been considered a vulnerability marker for substance use disorders (Verdejo-García et al., 2008; De Wit, 2009; Ersche et al., 2010; Robbins et al., 2012), playing a critical role in predicting onset and maintenance of drug taking and relapse rates (Pattij and De Vries, 2013). Impulsivity is also being regarded as a candidate for vulnerability to problematic Internet use (PIU) (Ryu et al., 2018), though PIU is controversially viewed as a behavioral addiction. In our study, problematic Internet users (PIUs) showed elevated scores on the BIS model of impulsive traits (i.e., Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, and Non-planning Impulsiveness), in keeping with previous studies (Cao et al., 2007; Lin et al., 2011; Dalbudak et al., 2013; Ryu et al., 2018); and they also exhibited higher levels on the UPPSP model (i.e., Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, Negative Urgency, and Positive Urgency), despite that limited literature failed to find consistent associations between the UPPSP model and problematic Internet gaming/excessive online gaming (Irvine et al., 2013; Nuyens et al., 2016; Deleuze et al., 2017; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2018). Similarly, increased impulsivity traits were observed likewise in our cigarette smokers, both on the BIS model (i.e., Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, and Non-planning Impulsiveness) and on the UPPSP model (i.e., Sensation Seeking, Lack of Perseverance, Negative Urgency, and Positive Urgency), consistent with the literature of impulsivity in cigarette smoking (Kale et al., 2018). These findings indicated that PIUs showed an increased tendency on most impulsivity traits comparable to cigarette smokers (except Lack of Premeditation and Sensation Seeking), demonstrating the likelihood of PIU as a putative non-substance addiction (Block, 2008; Potenza, 2018). However, in our study PIUs and cigarette smokers did not differ from non-smoking NIUs on the DDT, inconsistent with previous reports showing a higher degree of delay discounting in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder (Saville et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2017; Tian et al., 2018) and in university students with cigarette smoking (Białaszek et al., 2017). Nevertheless, smoking PIUs in our study did have higher discounting rates (i.e., k-values) than non-smoking NIUs, PIUs, and smokers (Table 4). Divergence on these data might be partly due to different methodologies and samples, therefore universal measurements should be employed in future to investigate the discrepancy of results from different studies.

More importantly in this study, we found that BIS Attentional Impulsiveness positively predicted both problematic Internet use (PIU) and cigarette smoking in the logistic regression models, after excluding the confounding effects of comorbidity between PIU and smoking behaviors as well as demographic variables (i.e., gender). To our best knowledge, this finding presented the first direct evidence that specific trait of impulsivity (i.e., Attentional Impulsiveness) is overtly increased as a predictive indicator in both PIU and one typical form of substance use disorders (i.e., cigarette smoking) among young adult populations. This data, together with previous preliminary evidence revealing parallel increased impulsivity characteristics on the BIS model between PIU and gambling disorder (Lee et al., 2012), suggest that specific traits of impulsivity might serve as important vulnerability candidates for PIU (Grant and Chamberlain, 2014). These results support our hypothesis that certain impulsivity trait (i.e., Attentional Impulsiveness) is shared by both PIU and cigarette smoking behaviors, putatively representing the basic mechanism and marker for PIU and other addictive disorders. Furthermore, the logistic regression models showed that BIS Motor Impulsiveness and UPPSP Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency predicted PIU, while UPPSP Sensation Seeking predicted cigarette smoking distinguishingly. This finding demonstrated a domain-specific tendency of impulsivity traits linked to PIU and cigarette smoking behaviors. Sensation Seeking refers to the liability to enjoy and pursue exciting activities that may be dangerous (Smith et al., 2007). It has been clearly showed that both male and female smokers were higher in Sensation Seeking than their non-smoking counterparts among adolescents and young adults (Carton et al., 1994; Martin et al., 2002), and Sensation Seeking had strong predictive value for cigarette use as well as marijuana and alcohol use in high school students (Crawford et al., 2003). Therefore, our data further support the notion that Sensation Seeking is an important factor in identifying youths at increased risk for smoking behaviors (Case et al., 2017). By contrast, Motor Impulsiveness (a tendency to act on the spur of the moment), Lack of Perseverance (refers to an ability to remain focused on a task), Lack of Premeditation (a tendency to think and reflect on the consequences of an act), and Negative Urgency (a tendency to experience strong impulses under the condition of negative affect) were indicated as the positive predictors for PIU separately. These results might potentially reflect the heightened risk of involving in PIU activities (e.g., gaming and shopping) for young adult students, who are dysfunctional in behavioral inhibition, myopic for the future, or having maladaptive emotion-focused coping styles (Hetzel-Riggin and Pritchard, 2011; Li et al., 2016; Nikolaidou et al., 2016).

Several limitations should be noted in this study. Firstly, the nature of our study design was a cross-sectional investigation essentially, despite the fact that we included two independent sample cohorts across 3 years (i.e., Sample 1 selected in 2014 and Sample 2 selected in 2017). Thus, the present study was not able to determine causal relationships of impulsivity traits with PIU and cigarette smoking behaviors and future longitudinal studies are warranted. Secondly, the Internet use and smoking status as well as impulsivity traits were primarily evaluated by using self-report questionnaires, which might be liable to bring subjective bias into data analyses. Therefore the results should be explained carefully, and more objective assessments should be incorporated in future research. Additionally, the self-report measurements (e.g., IAT and FTND) and our exclusion criteria for participants (e.g., current or past major psychiatric disorders) evaluated by self-report could suffer from social desirability bias, although we have tried to adopt anonymity and confidentiality methods to partially avoid this problem (e.g., the fear of stigma and stigmatization because of a history of psychiatric disorders among the young students). Thus, future similar research should include various methods (e.g., standard measures such as the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale) to reduce social desirability bias and improve the reliability of data collected by self-reports. Thirdly, our samples mainly consisted of young adult university students, so the findings could not be generalized to the other populations of PIUs and smokers (e.g., community samples, treatment-seeking samples), neither to other age groups (e.g., middle and late adulthood). Moreover, the college students in our study were randomly selected from a single university located in the city of Guiyang, China, so that they could not be regarded as one of the nationally representative samples. Simultaneously, the specific Chinese context of our college students is quite different from that of most Western countries, which might also limit the generalizability of the study findings, thus future cross-cultural studies are warranted and should be of help to draw more universal conclusions on the relationship of impulsivity with PIU and smoking behaviors.

In despite of these limitations, our results indicated that problematic Internet use (PIU) as a potential addictive behavior shared the basic and common mechanism with smoking behavior on certain impulsivity trait (i.e., Attentional Impulsiveness), which is essentially relevant to maladaptive cognitive inhibition, including attention deficit (refers to difficulties in focusing on a task at hand) and cognitive instability (refers to thought insertion and racing thoughts) (Patton et al., 1995; Nigg, 2000). This finding provided the direct converged evidence in PIU and cigarette smoking, supporting the notion that impulsivity is a vulnerability marker for addictive disorders (Verdejo-García et al., 2008; De Wit, 2009; Ersche et al., 2010; Robbins et al., 2012; Pattij and De Vries, 2013). In addition, specific aspects of impulsivity were distinctively linked to PIU (i.e., Motor Impulsiveness, Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, and Negative Urgency) and smoking (i.e., Sensation Seeking), representing a domain specificity of impulsive traits implicated in substance and non-substance addictions.

Our findings should be beneficial for future studies and probably throw light on the pathways to develop potential prevention methods and early interventions of different forms of addictive behaviors. For one thing, some cognitive training and behavioral interventions aimed at improving cognitive inhibition capacities that are related to Attentional Impulsiveness, such as working memory training and inhibitory control training, might be ecologically valid not only in pathological and clinical addictions (e.g., stimulants and alcohol dependence) (Houben et al., 2011; Wexler, 2011; Wiers et al., 2011; Wesley and Bickel, 2014), but also in less severe, non-clinical forms of problematic behaviors (e.g., PIU and cigarette smoking). Besides, more precise and effective training arrangements could be exploited by identifying specific targets of impulsivity (e.g., Attentional Impulsiveness) in further preclinical trials, especially for the youths who are also holding a high risk for other attention impulsivity-related disorders (e.g., attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder) (Evans et al., 2014). For another thing, the distinct patterns of impulsivity traits linked to PIU and cigarette smoking call for differentiated and personalized training schedules for individuals with varying types of addictions (i.e., substance use disorders vs. non-substance induced behavioral addictions) (Verdejo-García et al., 2008; Robbins et al., 2012; Yan et al., 2014). In this respect, typical facets of impulsivity should be targeted separately for the interventions in each condition (e.g., Negative Urgency for PIU, Sensation Seeking for cigarette smoking) among youths, who are not fully developed in brain cognition, and as such in emotional functioning. Especially in the Chinese cultural context, most children seem to be overindulged by their parents under the national one-child policy that is implemented since the 1980s and peaked in the 1990s in mainland China (Greenhalgh, 2003; Zhang and Goza, 2006); and school students in China are always suffering a heavy burden from the urgent expectancy of parents for their success in the college entrance examination. But paradoxically, most of these students could easily indulge themselves at university, staying away from parents and entrance exams. As a result, some mental disorders and behavioral disturbances (e.g., depression, anxiety, excessive gaming, smoking, shopping spree, binge eating and drinking) have been more and more salient in Chinese adolescents and young college students in recent decades (Liu and Zhou, 2002; Leung et al., 2008; Lian and Lin, 2008; Cao et al., 2011; Kieling et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2018). Our findings of the specific impulsivity traits involved in PIU (e.g., Negative Urgency) and cigarette smoking (e.g., Sensation Seeking), therefore, potentially designate a peculiar pathway to the prevention and intervention of certain problematic behaviors among Chinese college students. For instance, some school-based interventions (e.g., teaching techniques for coping and regulating negative affects) might help to reduce inappropriate catharsis of stress and prevent certain impulsive behaviors (e.g., PIU) related to negative urgency in these students (Berkman et al., 2012). By comparison, other behavioral trainings for resolving ambivalence and reducing the high levels of excitement pursuing, such as contingency management and motivational interviewing, could change subjective representations of rewards/losses and reduce the preference of college students for dangerous exciting activities connected with sensation seeking (e.g., smoking and drinking) (Hettema and Hendricks, 2010; Rohsenow et al., 2017). Last but not the least, different measures and models of impulsivity were utilized in our study, including a three-dimension model (i.e., the BIS-11) that mainly highlights cognitive-behavioral disinhibition facets (i.e., Attentional, Non-Planning, and Motor Impulsiveness) (Patton et al., 1995; Meda et al., 2009) and a five-pathway model (i.e., the UPPSP) that contains more diverse and complex traits (i.e., Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Premeditation, Sensation Seeking), as well as a delay discounting task (i.e., the DDT). However, because of the multi-dimensions and heterogeneity of these impulsivity traits, we failed to detect universal relationships between them with PIU and smoking, even though some traits seem to be similar in concept (e.g., Non-Planning Impulsiveness vs. Lack of Premeditation). Thus, our findings prompt that more integrated and refined models of impulsivity are quite necessary for future similar studies. As an example, a superior model that encompasses the all-round facets of impulsivity, including inhibitory control (e.g., cognitive inhibition related to Attentional Impulsiveness and Perseverance, behavioral inhibition related to Motor Impulsiveness), forward-looking planning (e.g., Delay Discounting, Premeditation, and Non-planning Impulsiveness), novelty/reward sensitivity (e.g., Sensation Seeking), and emotional urgency (e.g., Negative/Positive urgency), should be extremely beneficial to sniff out core marker traits of impulsivity implicated in addictive disorders, paving the way for better treating addiction in future (Ersche et al., 2010).

Ethics Statement

The Human Research Ethics Committee at the Guizhou Medical University thoroughly reviewed and approved this study. Our proposed study design, subject recruitment process, and plans to compensate the participants were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

S-JL, LW, and W-SY designed the study, wrote the protocols, and directed the study. S-JL and W-SY wrote a first draft of the manuscript. S-JL and YL performed assessments, data collection, and main data analysis. All authors contributed to the writing and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

Funding for this study was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31560284) and the Humanity and Social Science Research Project (Youth Foundation) of Ministry of Education of China (14YJC190022) to W-SY, and by the China National Social Science Foundation (13CSH041) to LW. These funding sources had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mr. R. Tossell and Dr. Y. Ren for their proofreading on our manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00772/full#supplementary-material

References

Albein-Urios, N., Martinez-González, J. M., Lozano,Ó., Clark, L., and Verdejo-García, A. (2012). Comparison of impulsivity and working memory in cocaine addiction and pathological gambling: implications for cocaine-induced neurotoxicity. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 126, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, C., Piazza, M., Mekos, D., and Valente, T. (2001). Peers, schools, and adolescent cigarette smoking. J. Adolesc. Health 29, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00210-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., and Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 382–392. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balevich, E. C., Wein, N. D., and Flory, J. D. (2013). Cigarette smoking and measures of impulsivity in a college sample. Subst. Abuse 34, 256–262. doi: 10.1080/08897077.2012.763082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkman, E. T., Graham, A. M., and Fisher, P. A. (2012). Training self-control: a domain-general translational neuroscience approach. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 374–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00248.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Białaszek, W., Marcowski, P., and Cox, D. J. (2017). Differences in delay, but not probability discounting, in current smokers, E-cigarette users, and never smokers. Psychol. Rec. 67, 223–230. doi: 10.1007/s40732-017-0244-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., and Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology 146, 447–454. doi: 10.1007/PL00005490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J. J. (2008). Issues for DSM-V: internet addiction. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 306–307. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, F., Su, L., Liu, T., and Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 466–471. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, H., Sun, Y., Wan, Y., Hao, J., and Tao, F. (2011). Problematic Internet use in Chinese adolescents and its relation to psychosomatic symptoms and life satisfaction. BMC Public Health 11:802. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carton, S., Jouvent, R., and Widlöcher, D. (1994). Sensation seeking, nicotine dependence, and smoking motivation in female and male smokers. Addict. Behav. 19, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90026-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Case, K. R., Loukas, A., Harrell, M. B., Wilkinson, A. V., Springer, A. E., Pérez, A., et al. (2017). The association between sensation seeking and e-cigarette use in Texas young adults: a cross-sectional study. J. Am. Coll. Health 65, 277–285. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2017.1282487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] (2012). Current tobacco use among middle and high school students–United States, 2011. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly Rep. 61, 581–585.

Google Scholar

Chen, X., Li, X., Stanton, B., Mao, R., Sun, Z., Zhang, H., et al. (2004). Patterns of cigarette smoking among students from 19 colleges and universities in Jiangsu Province, China: a latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 76, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Zheng, H., Sussman, S., Gong, J., Stacy, A., Xia, J., et al. (2002). Use of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire for measuring nicotine dependence among adolescent smokers in China: a pilot test. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 16, 260–263. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.16.3.260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. S., Park, S. M., Roh, M. S., Lee, J. Y., Park, C. B., Hwang, J. Y., et al. (2014). Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in Internet addiction. Psychiatry Res. 215, 424–428. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, A. M., Pentz, M. A., Chou, C. P., Li, C., and Dwyer, J. H. (2003). Parallel developmental trajectories of sensation seeking and regular substance use in adolescents. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 17, 179–192. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.3.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalbudak, E., Evren, C., Topcu, M., Aldemir, S., Coskun, K. S., Bozkurt, M., et al. (2013). Relationship of Internet addiction with impulsivity and severity of psychopathology among Turkish university students. Psychiatry Res. 210, 1086–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Wit, H. (2009). Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict. Biol. 14, 22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deleuze, J., Nuyens, F., Rochat, L., Rothen, S., Maurage, P., and Billieux, J. (2017). Established risk factors for addiction fail to discriminate between healthy gamers and gamers endorsing DSM-5 Internet gaming disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 6, 516–524. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 17, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, D. M., Smith, G., Olausson, P., Mitchell, S. H., Leeman, R. F., O’malley, S. S., et al. (2010). Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict. Biol. 15, 217–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00190.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, W. N., Sun, J. H., Sun, Y. W., Chen, X., Zhou, Y., Zhuang, Z. G., et al. (2014). Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a Go/No-Go fMRI study. Behav. Brain Funct. 10:20. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. R., Marley, J., and Jacobs, E. A. (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 36, 449–458. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., DeVito, E. E., Du, X., and Cui, Z. (2012). Impaired inhibitory control in ‘internet addiction disorder’: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 203, 153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., Lu, Q., Zhou, H., and Zhao, X. (2010). Impulse inhibition in people with Internet addiction disorder: electrophysiological evidence from a Go/No Go study. Neurosci. Lett. 485, 138–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, X., Qi, X., Yang, Y., Du, G., Gao, P., Zhang, Y., et al. (2016). Altered structural correlates of impulsivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:4. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ersche, K. D., Turton, A. J., Pradhan, S., Bullmore, E. T., and Robbins, T. W. (2010). Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 770–773. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. W., Owens, J. S., and Bunford, N. (2014). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 43, 527–551. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.850700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagerström, K. O., Kunze, M., Schoberberger, R., Breslau, N., Hughes, J. R., Hurt, R. D., et al. (1996). Nicotine dependence versus smoking prevalence: comparisons among countries and categories of smokers. Tob. Control 5, 52–56. doi: 10.1136/tc.5.1.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flory, J. D., and Manuck, S. B. (2009). Impulsiveness and cigarette smoking. Psychosom. Med. 71, 431–437. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181988c2d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, J. E., and Chamberlain, S. R. (2014). Impulsive action and impulsive choice across substance and behavioral addictions: cause or consequence? Addict. Behav. 39, 1632–1639. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhalgh, S. (2003). Science, modernity, and the making of China’s one-child policy. Popul. Dev. Rev. 29, 163–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2003.00163.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, D., Wu, X., Reynolds, K., Duan, X., Xin, X., Reynolds, R. F., et al. (2004). Cigarette smoking and exposure to environmental tobacco smoke in China: the international collaborative study of cardiovascular disease in Asia. Am. J. Public Health 94, 1972–1976. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.94.11.1972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerstrom, K. O. (1991). The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Addiction 86, 1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hettema, J. E., and Hendricks, P. S. (2010). Motivational interviewing for smoking cessation: a meta-analytic review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 78, 868–884. doi: 10.1037/a0021498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hetzel-Riggin, M. D., and Pritchard, J. R. (2011). Predicting problematic Internet use in men and women: the contributions of psychological distress, coping style, and body esteem. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 519–525. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., Wiers, R. W., and Jansen, A. (2011). Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 116, 132–136. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Bolton, S., Harrison, N. A., Bullmore, E. T., and Voon, V. (2013). Impaired decisional impulsivity in pathological videogamers. PLoS One 8:e75914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kale, D., Stautz, K., and Cooper, A. (2018). Impulsivity related personality traits and cigarette smoking in adults: a meta-analysis using the UPPS-P model of impulsivity and reward sensitivity. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 185, 149–167. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandell, J. J. (1998). Internet addiction on campus: the vulnerability of college students. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1, 11–17. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khazaal, Y., Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khan, R., Louati, Y., Scarlatti, E., et al. (2008). French validation of the internet addiction test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 11, 703–706. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kieling, C., Baker-Henningham, H., Belfer, M., Conti, G., Ertem, I., Omigbodun, O., et al. (2011). Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: evidence for action. Lancet 378, 1515–1525. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60827-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., and Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, 78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. W., Choi, J. S., Shin, Y. C., Lee, J. Y., Jung, H. Y., and Kwon, J. S. (2012). Impulsivity in internet addiction: a comparison with pathological gambling. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 15, 373–377. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeman, R. F., and Potenza, M. N. (2012). Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: a focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology 219, 469–490. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, P. W., Hung, S. F., Ho, T. P., Lee, C. C., Liu, W. S., Tang, C. P., et al. (2008). Prevalence of DSM-IV disorders in Chinese adolescents and the effects of an impairment criterion. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 17, 452–461. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-0687-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Zhang, W., Li, X., Zhen, S., and Wang, Y. (2010). Stressful life events and problematic Internet use by adolescent females and males: a mediated moderation model. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1199–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, N., and Kirkup, G. (2007). Gender and cultural differences in Internet use: a study of China and the UK. Comput. Educ. 48, 301–317. doi: 10.1111/appy.12229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Q., Tian, M., Taxer, J., Zheng, Y., Wu, H., Sun, S., et al. (2016). Problematic internet users’ discounting behaviors reflect an inability to delay gratification, not risk taking. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19, 172–178. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. Y., Phillips, M. R., Xu, D., Zhang, Y. L., Yang, S. J., Tong, Y. S., et al. (2011). Reliability and validity of an adapted Chinese version of Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Chin. Ment. Health J. 25, 610–615.

Google Scholar

Lian, J. W., and Lin, T. M. (2008). Effects of consumer characteristics on their acceptance of online shopping: comparisons among different product types. Comput. Hum. Behav. 24, 48–65. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, L., Zhou, D., Yuan, C., Shao, A., and Bian, Y. (2016). Gender differences in the relationship between internet addiction and depression: a cross-lagged study in Chinese adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 63, 463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, M. P., Ko, H. C., and Wu, J. Y. W. (2011). Prevalence and psychosocial risk factors associated with Internet addiction in a nationally representative sample of college students in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 741–746. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., and Zhou, H. (2002). Sleep duration, insomnia and behavioral problems among Chinese adolescents. Psychiatry Res. 111, 75–85. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00131-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., and Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol. Sci. 8, 60–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00545.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mao, R., Li, X., Stanton, B., Wang, J., Hong, Y., Zhang, H., et al. (2008). Psychosocial correlates of cigarette smoking among college students in China. Health Educ. Res. 24, 105–118. doi: 10.1093/her/cyn002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C. A., Kelly, T. H., Rayens, M. K., Brogli, B. R., Brenzel, A., Smith, W. J., et al. (2002). Sensation seeking, puberty, and nicotine, alcohol, and marijuana use in adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 1495–1502. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meda, S. A., Stevens, M. C., Potenza, M. N., Pittman, B., Gueorguieva, R., Andrews, M. M., et al. (2009). Investigating the behavioral and self-report constructs of impulsivity domains using principal component analysis. Behav. Pharmacol. 20, 390–399. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32833113a3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 146, 455–464. doi: 10.1007/PL00005491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, X., Yan, H., Chen, S., and Liu, Z. (2009). Factors influencing internet addiction in a sample of freshmen university students in China. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 327–330. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nie, J., Zhang, W., and Liu, Y. (2017). Exploring depression, self-esteem and verbal fluency with different degrees of internet addiction among Chinese college students. Compr. Psychiatry 72, 114–120. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 126, 220–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikolaidou, M., Fraser, D. S., and Hinvest, N. (2016). Physiological markers of biased decision-making in problematic Internet users. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 510–517. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuyens, F., Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., and Billieux, J. (2016). Impulsivity in multiplayer online battle arena gamers: preliminary results on experimental and self-report measures. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 351–356. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., and Kitamura, N. (2005). Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology 182, 508–515. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattij, T., and De Vries, T. J. (2013). The role of impulsivity in relapse vulnerability. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 700–705. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M., and O’brien, C. P. (2013). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5. Addiction 108, 1186–1187. doi: 10.1111/add.12162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potenza, M. N. (2018). Do gaming disorder and hazardous gaming belong in the ICD-11? Considerations regarding the death of a hospitalized patient that was reported to have occurred while a care provider was gaming. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 206–207. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, M. B., Wang, R., Shillington, A. M., Clapp, J. D., and Lange, J. E. (2007). The relationship between alcohol use and cigarette smoking in a sample of undergraduate college students. Addict. Behav. 32, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, B., Patak, M., Shroff, P., Penfold, R. B., Melanko, S., and Duhig, A. M. (2007). Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behavior in adolescent daily smokers and nonsmokers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 15, 264–271. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.3.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, B., Penfold, R. B., and Patak, M. (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: laboratory behavioral assessments. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 16, 124–131. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.2.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, B., Richards, J. B., Horn, K., and Karraker, K. (2004). Delay discounting and probability discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults. Behav. Process. 65, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00109-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ríos-Bedoya, C. F., Snedecor, S. M., Pomerleau, C. S., and Pomerleau, O. F. (2008). Association of withdrawal features with nicotine dependence as measured by the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND). Addict. Behav. 33, 1086–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., and Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohsenow, D. J., Martin, R. A., Tidey, J. W., Colby, S. M., and Monti, P. M. (2017). Treating smokers in substance treatment with contingent vouchers, nicotine replacement and brief advice adapted for sobriety settings. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 72, 72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2016.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rømer Thomsen, K., Callesen, M. B., Hesse, M., Kvamme, T. L., Pedersen, M. M., Pedersen, M. U., et al. (2018). Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 317–330. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, S. (2009). Contemporary Chinese youth and the state. J. Asian Stud. 68, 359–369. doi: 10.1017/S0021911809000631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryu, H., Lee, J. Y., Choi, A., Park, S., Kim, D. J., and Choi, J. S. (2018). The relationship between impulsivity and internet gaming disorder in young adults: mediating effects of interpersonal relationships and depression. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:E458. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15030458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saville, B. K., Gisbert, A., Kopp, J., and Telesco, C. (2010). Internet addiction and delay discounting in college students. Psychol. Rec. 60, 273–286. doi: 10.1007/BF03395707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte, M. H., Kaag, A. M., Wiers, R. W., Schmaal, L., van den Brink, W., Reneman, L., et al. (2017). Prefrontal Glx and GABA concentrations and impulsivity in cigarette smokers and smoking polysubstance users. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 179, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Annus, A. M., Spillane, N. S., and McCarthy, D. M. (2007). On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment 14, 155–170. doi: 10.1177/1073191106295527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spada, M. M. (2014). An overview of problematic Internet use. Addict. Behav. 39, 3–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, P., Johnson, C. A., Palmer, P., Arpawong, T. E., Unger, J. B., Xie, B., et al. (2012). Concurrent and predictive relationships between compulsive Internet use and substance use: findings from vocational high school students in China and the USA. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 9, 660–673. doi: 10.3390/ijerph9030660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Y., and Li, S. (2011). Testing the effect of risk on intertemporal choice in the Chinese cultural context. J. Soc. Psychol. 151, 517–522. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2010.503719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, J., Yu, Y., Du, Y., Ma, Y., Zhang, D., and Wang, J. (2014). Prevalence of internet addiction and its association with stressful life events and psychological symptoms among adolescent internet users. Addict. Behav. 39, 744–747. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, M., Tao, R., Zheng, Y., Zhang, H., Yang, G., Li, Q., et al. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting. Comput. Hum. Behav. 80, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdejo-García, A., Lawrence, A. J., and Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Zhou, X., Lu, C., Wu, J., Deng, X., and Hong, L. (2011). Problematic Internet use in high school students in Guangdong Province, China. PLoS One 6:e19660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. L., Jackson, L. A., Zhang, D. J., and Su, Z. Q. (2012). The relationships among the Big Five Personality factors, self-esteem, narcissism, and sensation-seeking to Chinese University students’ uses of social networking sites (SNSs). Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 2313–2319. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Luo, J., Bai, Y., Kong, J., Luo, J., Gao, W., et al. (2013). Internet addiction of adolescents in China: prevalence, predictors, and association with well-being. Addict. Res. Theory 21, 62–69. doi: 10.3109/16066359.2012.690053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Hu, Y., Xu, J., Zhou, H., Lin, X., Du, X., et al. (2017). Dysfunctional prefrontal function is associated with impulsivity in people with internet gaming disorder during a delay discounting task. Front. Psychiatry 8:287. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesley, M. J., and Bickel, W. K. (2014). Remember the future II: meta-analyses and functional overlap of working memory and delay discounting. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 435–448. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wexler, B. E. (2011). Computerized cognitive remediation treatment for substance abuse disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 197–198. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiteside, S. P., and Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30, 669–689. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., and Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychol. Sci. 22, 490–497. doi: 10.1177/0956797611400615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winstanley, C. A., Eagle, D. M., and Robbins, T. W. (2006). Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 379–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, X., Chen, X., Han, J., Meng, H., Luo, J., Nydegger, L., et al. (2013). Prevalence and factors of addictive Internet use among adolescents in Wuhan, China: interactions of parental relationship with age and hyperactivity-impulsivity. PLoS One 8:e61782. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, W., Li, Y., and Sui, N. (2014). The relationship between recent stressful life events, personality traits, perceived family functioning and internet addiction among college students. Stress Health 30, 3–11. doi: 10.1002/smi.2490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, W. S., Zhang, R. R., Lan, Y., Li, Y. H., and Sui, N. (2016). Comparison of impulsivity in non-problem, at-risk and problem gamblers. Sci. Rep. 6:39233. doi: 10.1038/srep39233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, W. S., Zhang, R. R., Lan, Y., Li, Z. M., and Li, Y. H. (2018). Questionnaire-based maladaptive decision-coping patterns involved in binge eating among 1013 college students. Front. Psychol. 9:609. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, L., and Zheng, Y. (2012). Fen qings (angry youth) in contemporary China. J. Contemp. China 21, 637–653. doi: 10.1080/10670564.2012.666834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, T., Shiffman, S., Rockett, I. R., Cui, X., and Cao, R. (2011). Nicotine dependence among Chinese city dwellers: a population-based cross-sectional study. Nicotine Tob. Res. 13, 556–564. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X., Zhu, L., Chen, Q., Song, P., and Wang, Z. (2016). Parent marital conflict and Internet addiction among Chinese college students: the mediating role of father-child, mother-child, and peer attachment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 59, 221–229. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.01.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1, 237–244. doi: 10.1007/s10899-011-9287-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X. Y., Liang, J., Xiu, M. H., He, J., Cheng, W., Wu, Z., et al. (2012). Cigarette smoking in male patients with chronic schizophrenia in a Chinese population: prevalence and relationship to clinical phenotypes. PLoS One 7:e30937. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., and Goza, F. W. (2006). Who will care for the elderly in China?: a review of the problems caused by China’s one-child policy and their potential solutions. J. Aging Stud. 20, 151–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2005.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Mei, S., Li, L., Chai, J., Li, J., and Du, H. (2015). The relationship between impulsivity and internet addiction in Chinese college students: a moderated mediation analysis of meaning in life and self-esteem. PLoS One 10:e0131597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, N., Cao, H., Li, X., Zhang, J., Yao, Y., Geng, X., et al. (2018). Internet addiction, problematic internet use, non-problematic internet use among Chinese adolescents: individual, parental, peer, and socio-demographic correlates. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 32, 365–372. doi: 10.1037/adb0000358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, T., Feng, B., Wong, S., Choi, W., and Zhu, S. H. (2004). A comparison of smoking behaviors among medical and other college students in China. Health Promot. Int. 19, 189–196. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dah206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: problematic internet use, smoking, addiction, impulsivity, personality

Citation: Liu S-J, Lan Y, Wu L and Yan W-S (2019) Profiles of Impulsivity in Problematic Internet Users and Cigarette Smokers. Front. Psychol. 10:772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00772

Received: 21 September 2018; Accepted: 20 March 2019;
Published: 04 April 2019.

Edited by:

Hester Van Herk, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Csilla Horvath, Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands
Symen Brouwers, North-West University, South Africa

Copyright © 2019 Liu, Lan, Wu and Yan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Wu, d2xpbkB3aHUuZWR1LmNu Wan-Sen Yan, eWFud2Fuc2VuQDE2My5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.