Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 02 April 2019
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology

Over-the-Counter Painkillers and Evolutionary Mismatch

  • Psychology Program, SUNY Polytechnic Institute, Utica, NY, United States

People commonly reach for over-the-counter (OTC) pain medications to treat even the slightest physical discomfort, whether at work, at home, or on the go. Acetaminophen (paracetamol, Tylenol) tops the charts among popular painkillers, with estimates in the United States suggesting that nearly one in four adults consume a drug containing acetaminophen each week (Kaufman et al., 2002). As an analgesic, acetaminophen is quite effective in reducing mild to moderate physical pain and ailments, providing relief from headaches, and serving as a fever reducer. However, the accessibility and widespread use of this drug across the lifespan, even beginning in infancy (e.g., Infants' Tylenol), produces a mismatch with our evolved psychology to minimize pain and distress.

An evolutionary mismatch occurs when there is a major divergence between modern contexts and ancestral environments (reviewed by Li et al., 2018). Since changes to the environment drastically outpace the course of biological/genetic evolution, the conditions humans encounter in contemporary life are often vastly different from those of the hunter-gatherer lifestyles that shaped our brains and behavior. Thus, our psychological adaptations that evolved to solve recurrent problems of survival and/or reproduction during 99% of human evolutionary history are now activated by novel inputs of the modern world, which can lead to negative consequences for physical and psychological health (Li et al., 2018).

The experience of physical pain provides a clear example of a psychological adaptation. The unpleasantness of physical pain is highly adaptive and critical for survival. Pain draws our attention and serves to motivate responses to alleviate the discomfort. In ancestral conditions, this would have resulted in behavioral changes that functioned to reduce harm, minimize further damage, and prompt us to learn to avoid situations and actions that led to pain in the past. These benefits of physical pain are clearly witnessed among individuals unable to detect it, as those born with the rare condition of congenital insensitivity to pain often die in childhood due to the failure to notice injuries. Thus, there has been continuous selective pressure during the course of evolution to detect and reduce physical discomfort, and as a result most people possess a strong desire to avoid pain through behavioral changes that reduce exposure to harmful stimuli or events.

Within modern environments, however, individuals suffering from pain can now bypass behavioral changes and simply treat their symptoms with OTC medication. In fact, acetaminophen has been recommended as the first line of treatment for acute lower back pain (reviewed by Koes et al., 2010) and osteoarthritis (reviewed by Zhang et al., 2004), though more recent research has questioned the efficacy of acetaminophen treatment within chronic pain conditions (e.g., Machado et al., 2015; Ennis et al., 2016). People also frequently use painkillers to deal with injuries resulting from physical exercise or sporting competition. In one study of over 500 collegiate athletes, more than half (58%) reported regularly using either OTC or prescribed painkillers throughout the season to alleviate soreness from tough workouts and/or competition (Tricker, 2000). The continued dulling of physical pain from drugs, which undoubtedly feels good in the short-term, could produce maladaptive outcomes in the long-term if nothing is done to address the source of the pain itself. That is, routinely taking acetaminophen or similar OTC drugs to treat pain could exacerbate the physical damage, negatively impact recovery time, and even contribute to the acquisition of additional unnoticed injury or insult.

A distinct, yet somewhat similar, evolutionary mismatch comes from the fever reducing properties of acetaminophen and many other OTC pain medications. Research shows that acetaminophen and other OTC painkillers are effective and well-tolerated in treating febrile illness (McIntyre and Hull, 1996; Purssell, 2002), and as a result, many people reach for these drugs at the first signs of fever. Although unpleasant to endure, fever is our body's adaptive response to combat pathogens and the benefits appear to outweigh the costs (Harden et al., 2015). Taking antipyretics to reduce a fever halts the most effective natural defense against infection, and studies show that it can even produce detrimental health outcomes and increase mortality (Schulman et al., 2005; Eyers et al., 2010). Fever itself should thus not be considered a problem that needs to be remedied, unless of course, it is unusually high and/or contributing to other health problems, but rather it should viewed as an adaptive immune response that has been shaped by natural selection.

More recent and thought-provoking lines of research show that acetaminophen can produce further mismatch with our evolved psychology by even diminishing our sensitivity to social experiences that are interpreted as painful or distressing (reviewed by Ratner et al., 2018). As it turns out, the neurobiological systems that process physical and social pain overlap in the brain (Eisenberger et al., 2003), which gives credence to the saying “words hurt.” This neurological coupling makes sense from an evolutionary perspective given the critical importance of group membership and standing for survival and reproduction in our species. The biological response of social exclusion, for example, should be nearly as attention grabbing and aversive as a painful physical stimulus, and similarly motivate us to respond adaptively to mitigate damage.

In a radical set of studies, DeWall et al. (2010) found that after taking typical doses of acetaminophen daily for a period of 3 weeks, participants reported significantly lower levels of social pain in their daily lives and showed reduced cortical activity (i.e., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula) to social rejection scenarios produced in an experimental setting. Incredibly, subsequent studies have shown that even one-time use of acetaminophen in the laboratory can significantly reduce one's empathy for both physical and social pain experienced by others (Mischkowski et al., 2016). Furthermore, it has been found that acetaminophen blunts the conflict and pain of decision-making (DeWall et al., 2015) and dampens overall emotional perception and subjective arousal (Durso et al., 2015). Collectively, these effects could produce a variety of unintended and maladaptive psychological outcomes in the real world. For example, individuals that routinely take acetaminophen to treat physical pain or headaches in their daily lives may fail to adequately process negative interpersonal interactions, lack typical and appropriate responses to affective cues, show reduced empathy and affiliation toward partners and close friends in times of distress, and suffer from impaired forms of judgement or decision-making.

Future research should aim to examine the full range of psychoactive effects produced by acetaminophen and other OTC painkillers. Based on evidence supporting a general cortical dampening produced by acetaminophen (DeWall et al., 2010), one potentially fruitful area to examine would be whether this drug also modulates the processing of subcortical and more prewired psychological adaptations. Among primates, for example, a group of neurons in the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus has been shown to selectively fire in response to images of snakes (Van Le et al., 2013), and this occurs even among individuals reared in captivity that have had no prior experience with these dangerous animals. Determining how acetaminophen alters the psychological processing of distressing stimuli and situations that vary in ancestral and evolutionary relevance, and evoke distinct patterns of neural activation within the brain, would be important for determining the magnitude and scope of the blunting effects produced by this drug. This type of research could also provide a more systematic approach to assessing how OTC painkillers contribute to mismatches with our evolved psychology.

To date, research examining the psychological effects of acetaminophen and other OTC painkillers is still in its infancy (Ratner et al., 2018), so it remains unknown how the typical consumption of these drugs influence the real world interactions and varied interpersonal relationships among the millions of the people taking them on a daily basis. While the analgesic properties of these drugs appear quite effective and well-tolerated, the potential negative consequences abound, and thus we should be wary of frequent or routine usage of these medications. A little pain and distress, whether physical or social, is good for us, and we should therefore embrace its abrasiveness.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

The author received funding from a Faculty SEED Grant at SUNY Polytechnic Institute (#81825).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I am thankful to Omar Tonsi Eldakar and Gordon G. Gallup Jr. for providing feedback on an earlier draft of the paper. I am also grateful to David Sloan Wilson and the online magazine This View of Life for motivating me to write this paper.

References

DeWall, C. N., Chester, D. S., and White, D. S. (2015). Can acetaminophen reduce the pain of decision-making? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 56, 117–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeWall, C. N., MacDonald, G., Webster, G. D., Masten, C. L., Baumeister, R. F., Powell, C., et al. (2010). Acetaminophen reduces social pain: behavioral and neural evidence. Psychol. Sci. 21, 931–937. doi: 10.1177/0956797610374741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durso, G. R., Luttrell, A., and Way, B. M. (2015). Over-the-counter relief from pains and pleasures alike: acetaminophen blunts evaluation sensitivity to both negative and positive stimuli. Psychol. Sci. 26, 750–758. doi: 10.1177/0956797615570366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennis, Z. N., Dideriksen, D., Vægter, H. B., Handberg, G., and Pottegård, A. (2016). Acetaminophen for chronic pain: a systematic review on efficacy. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 118, 184–189. doi: 10.1111/bcpt.12527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyers, S., Weatherall, M., Shirtcliffe, P., Perrin, K., and Beasley, R. (2010). The effect on mortality of antipyretics in the treatment of influenza infection: systematic review and meta-analyis. J. R. Soc. Med. 103, 403–411. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2010.090441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harden, L. M., Kent, S., Pittman, Q. J., and Roth, J. (2015). Fever and sickness behavior: friend or foe? Brain Behav. Immun. 50, 322–333. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, D. W., Kelly, J. P., Rosenberg, L., Anderson, T. E., and Mitchell, A. A. (2002). Recent patterns of medication use in the ambulatory adult population of the United States: the slone survey. JAMA 287, 337–344. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.3.337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koes, B. W., van Tulder, M., Lin, C. W. C., Macedo, L. G., McAuley, J., and Maher, C. (2010). An updated overview of clinical guidelines for the management of non-specific low back pain in primary care. Eur. Spine J. 19, 2075–2094. doi: 10.1007/s00586-010-1502-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, N. P., van Vugt, M., and Colarelli, S. M. (2018). The evolutionary mismatch hypothesis: implications for psychological science. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 27, 38–44. doi: 10.1177/0963721417731378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machado, G. C., Maher, C. G., and Ferreira, M. L. (2015). Lack of efficacy of paracetamol (acetaminophen) for low back pain and osteoarthritis. J. Pioneer. Med. Sci. 5, 142–143.

Google Scholar

McIntyre, J., and Hull, D. (1996). Comparing efficacy and tolerability of ibuprofen and paracetamol in fever. Arch. Dis. Child. 74, 164–167. doi: 10.1136/adc.74.2.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischkowski, D., Crocker, J., and Way, B. M. (2016). From painkiller to empathy killer: acetaminophen (paracetamol) reduces empathy for pain. Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. 11, 1345–1353. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purssell, E. (2002). Treating fever in children: paracetamol or ibuprofen?. Br. J. Community Nurs. 7, 316–320. doi: 10.12968/bjcn.2002.7.6.10477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratner, K. G., Kaczmarek, A. R., and Hong, Y. (2018). Can over-the-counter pain medications influence our thoughts and emotions? Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 82–89. doi: 10.1177/2372732217748965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulman, C. I., Namias, N., Doherty, J., Manning, R. J., Li, P., Elhaddad, A., et al. (2005). The effect of antipyretic therapy upon outcomes in critically ill patients: a randomized, prospective study. Surg. Infect. 6, 369–375. doi: 10.1089/sur.2005.6.369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tricker, R. (2000). Painkilling drugs in collegiate athletics: knowledge, attitudes, and use of student athletes. J. Drug Educ. 30, 313–324. doi: 10.2190/N1K3-V8BK-90GH-TTHU

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Nguyen, M., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., et al. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19000–19005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312648110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Jones, A., and Doherty, M. (2004). Does paracetamol (acetaminophen) reduce the pain of osteoarthritis? A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 63, 901–907. doi: 10.1136/ard.2003.018531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, acetamination, evolutionary mismatch, social pain, fever

Citation: Gallup AC (2019) Over-the-Counter Painkillers and Evolutionary Mismatch. Front. Psychol. 10:686. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00686

Received: 12 December 2018; Accepted: 12 March 2019;
Published: 02 April 2019.

Edited by:

Phil Kavanagh, ISN Psychology Pty Ltd., Australia

Reviewed by:

Norman Li, Singapore Management University, Singapore

Copyright © 2019 Gallup. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrew C. Gallup, a.c.gallup@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.