- 1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
- 2NeuroMI—Milan Center for Neuroscience, University of Milano Bicocca, Milan, Italy
- 3Cognitive Neuropsychology Centre, ASST Grande Ospedale Metropolitano Niguarda, Milan, Italy
Elucidating the nature of consciousness has become one of the most relevant challenges in modern neuroscience. The study of patients with brain damage who exhibit selective impairments in awareness has contributed significantly to redefining the concept of consciousness, shedding light on at least two interesting aspects of its organization: the presence of behavioral and anatomical domain-specific dissociations and the possibility of modulating disturbances in awareness via both central and peripheral stimulations (e.g., caloric vestibular stimulation1—CVS—or transcranial Direct Current Stimulation2—tDCS). Evidence emerging from these lines of research has provided information regarding the nature of conscious processes, their neural substrates, and their associated physiological processes.
Anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP), in which patients with brain damage deny the presence of their contralesional motor deficits (Babinski, 1914), represents how the conscious experience may be finely segmented. Indeed, motor anosognosia can manifest in a limb-specific and modality-specific fashion: For instance, it may affect the upper or lower limbs only (Von Hagen and Ives, 1937; Berti et al., 1996; Moro et al., 2011) and may concern either motor or sensory impairments (i.e., dissociations between AHP and anosognosia for hemianesthesia: AHA; see for example Marcel et al., 2004; Spinazzola et al., 2008; Pia et al., 2014). Moreover, previous studies have reported double dissociations between anosognosia and neglect (Bisiach et al., 1986; Berti et al., 1996; Marcel et al., 2004), revealing that AHP does not depend on the attentional deficits typical of Unilateral Spatial Neglect (Bisiach et al., 1986; Berti et al., 1996; Marcel et al., 2004). The existence of these dissociations suggests that brain lesions selectivity affect conscious processes, rather than inducing general and global impairments in awareness. Thus, patients may be aware of one deficit (e.g., hemianesthesia) yet unaware of another (e.g., hemiplegia).
The use of structured interviews (see for example Bisiach et al., 1986; Berti et al., 1996) and systematic experimental paradigms (Jenkinson and Fotopoulou, 2010) has revealed that motor denial can manifest in various ways. For example, when required to perform a specific action (such as clapping), some patients became aware that they are unable to execute the movement (moderate AHP, score 2/2, in Bisiach et al., 1986; emergent awareness in Moro et al., 2011), while others claim to have performed the action despite their paralysis (severe AHP, score 3/3 in Bisiach et al., 1986). These differences in awareness have been classically conceptualized as different degrees of severity (Bisiach et al., 1986). Alternatively, these two behaviors may be related to different cognitive mechanisms. Among patients who claim they have performed the movement despite demonstrations that no movements have occurred (i.e., those with apparently more severe anosognosia), this obstinate motor delusion seems to be related to the loss of on-going monitoring mechanisms. In other words, this denial is embedded into the neural systems subserving the comparison between the intended and performed movements (Frith et al., 2000; Blakemore et al., 2002). Conversely, among patients who realize they have not moved after failure of the action, the initial denial of impairment may depend on the semantic knowledge that the body segments can move, and on the memory of being able to move prior to the occurrence of the brain lesion (Marcel et al., 2004). Thus, the false belief of preserved motor competence is supported by a left hemisphere “narrator” telling what he knows about bodily functions (Geschwind, 1965).
Previous research has also revealed interesting dissociations between implicit and explicit awareness: Indeed, patients with AHP may have implicit knowledge of their deficit, which is otherwise denied explicitly (see for example Marcel et al., 2004; Fotopoulou et al., 2010; Vocat and Vuilleumier, 2010; Moro et al., 2011). Explicit awareness can be explored directly by asking patients to verbally declare whether they can move, while implicit awareness is generally indirectly inferred based on behavioral observations (Fotopoulou et al., 2010; Moro et al., 2011). Several reports have indicated that different levels of awareness may coexist within the same patient: “Patients who verbally deny their hemiplegia usually do not object of being confined to bed” (Bisiach and Geminiani, 1991). More recently, this dissociation has been explored systematically using either verbal (Fotopoulou et al., 2010) or motor paradigms (Moro et al., 2011). These studies also provide partially convergent anatomical patterns of the dissociation between explicit and implicit awareness.
Anatomical evidence also supports the modular organization of consciousness. In 2005, Berti et al. (2005) demonstrated that AHP occurs due to lesions of the cerebral regions that monitor motor functions, such as the premotor cortex, rather than damage to a general supramodal neural system. Notably, the brain lesion of a single patient, who exhibited anosognosia without signs of neglect, overlapped with the damage associated with AHP in the between groups comparisons, supporting the hypothesis that motor-monitoring deficits can be disentangled from USN. However, Karnath et al. (2005) reported different results, instead emphasizing the role of the posterior insula in AHP. Further evidence supports the notion that different forms of anosognosia correspond to discrete anatomical substrates depending on the sensory-motor disorders observed. These studies have suggested that AHA is associated with lesions of brain areas such as the putamen, which is well known to subserve sensory processing, contrasting somewhat with the regions involved in AHP (Spinazzola et al., 2008; Pia et al., 2014).
However, more recent studies (Fotopoulou et al., 2010; Vocat et al., 2010; Moro et al., 2011, 2016) have provided a more complex picture of the brain regions involved in awareness, suggesting that AHP is a multi-component disorder caused by lesions of complex and distributed cortical-subcortical anatomical networks, rather than isolated regions (see discussion in Fotopoulou, 2014). Given the available evidence, combined use of the lesion-symptom method and modern neuroanatomical approaches such as fMRI/resting-state fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging, and fiber tractography may allow researchers to overcome limitations associated with defining large-scale networks involved in complex cognitive functions (e.g., body and motor awareness), based on classical clinical anatomical correlations alone (Catani et al., 2012; Thiebaut de Schotten and Foulon, 2018). Elucidating these networks may advance our understanding of the neural and physiological bases of consciousness (see for example Gandola et al., 2014a).
Disorders of consciousness may affect different levels of body representation. AHP is frequently associated with asomatognosia (see Jenkinson et al., 2018) and somatoparaphrenia (SP), which is characterized by the delusion that the paralyzed limb does to not belong to oneself (Gerstmann, 1942). In AHP and SP, impairments in consciousness are associated with bodily functions and body ownership, respectively. Rare cases of SP without AHP have been described (see for example Invernizzi et al., 2012; Moro et al., 2016). Invernizzi et al. (2012) demonstrated that SP is mainly associated with lesions to the right thalamus, basal ganglia, and posterior limb of the internal capsule, sparing the dorsal and premotor regions typically associated with AHP (Berti et al., 2005). The lesional pattern defines the neuropsychological manifestations observed, although the level of interdependence among the different components contributing to mental representation of the body remains unknown. The existence of patients who, surprisingly enough, deny ownership of their paralyzed limb although they recognize that they cannot move it may be explained by the influence of selective awareness modules on unshared senses of agency and body ownership as independent components, supporting an independent rather than additive model of body representation (Tsakiris et al., 2006; Invernizzi et al., 2012). More recently, Moro et al. (2016) revealed that disturbed sensation of limb ownership (DSO) is associated with more medial and subcortical lesions (mainly involving the basal ganglia and surrounding white matter) than those involved in AHP. This evidence supports the existence of a neural dissociation between DSO and AHP in contrast to previous evidence, which suggested a crucial role of the right posterior insula for both the sense of limb ownership and motor anosognosia (Baier and Karnath, 2008).
Extensive research has suggested that central and peripheral stimulation (e.g., CVS, tDCS) can induce transient and selective remission in patients with various disorders of awareness (e.g., USN, anosognosia, and somatoparaphrenia), supporting the notion that conscious experience can be selectively modified. For instance, CVS may restore motor awareness (see for example Vallar et al., 1990, 2003; Bisiach et al., 1991; Rode et al., 1992), the sense of body ownership (Bisiach and Geminiani, 1991; Rode et al., 1992; Salvato et al., 2016), and sensory perception (Vallar et al., 1990, 1993; Bottini et al., 1995, 2005). Moreover, the effects of CVS on these disorders suggest that physiological components play a role in the conscious bodily experience, interacting with cognitive processes. In our recent study, for example, we utilized CVS in a patient with SP, who regained the sense of body part ownership following stimulation. We also observed an increase in body temperature following CVS, which correlated with the temporarily restored sense of limb ownership (Salvato et al., 2018). We speculated that this effect may have been due to partial overlap between the neural correlates of body ownership, thermoregulatory control, and the area stimulated by left-CVS. Alternatively, CVS may modulate interoceptive signals (e.g., body temperature), which may in turn have increased the sense of body ownership. Interestingly, in healthy volunteers, CVS induces a temperature drop in both arms, which is accompanied by bilateral improvements in tactile acuity (Sedda et al., 2016). Other research has indicated that selective remission can be induced in patients with disorders of awareness using simple verbal (Case F. B. in Bottini et al., 2002) or spatial manipulations (Salvato et al., 2016) or using mirror techniques (Jenkinson et al., 2013), highlighting the malleability of conscious processes.
Moreover, tDCS is effective in modulating disorders of motor awareness. For instance, tDCS over the right premotor cortex has been shown to induce selective remission of AHP (Gandola et al., 2014b). Such improvements manifested only when the patient was requested to perform the action (online judgment) with his eyes open, while motor anosognosia remained when the patient judged non-attempted actions (offline condition) and during the eyes-closed online condition. Selective modulation of motor awareness has also been observed in healthy volunteers, revealing that posterior parietal modulation interferes with non-intentional movement awareness, while premotor cortex modulation interferes with intentional movement awareness (Bolognini et al., 2016; Bruno et al., 2017).
In conclusion, although the neurophysiological organization of awareness into discrete neural systems may explain the multiple dissociations of symptoms observed in patients with brain damage, alternative hypotheses have also been proposed. Recently, the influential theoretical framework, which is based on the free-energy principle and Bayesian inference (Friston, 2005), has been applied to the study of AHP (Fotopoulou, 2014, 2015) and self-recognition (Apps and Tsakiris, 2014). This framework may provide an alternative to highly modular models of motor and self-awareness and may represent a unified explanation of the clinical variability/dissociations of such deficits (Fotopoulou, 2014, 2015).
Author Contributions
GB, FGM, GS, and MG equally contributed in drafting and revising the final version of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
1. ^CVS involves irrigation of the left or right external auricular canal with warm or cold water. This produces effects on several cognitive processes by eliciting neural responses from vestibular neurons, which are strongly interconnected with higher-order brain regions (see review in Bottini and Gandola, 2015).
2. ^By applying a low-intensity electric current to the scalp, tDCS allows for controlled interference with brain activity. Inhibitory (cathodal) or excitatory (anodal) modulation can be induced in the stimulated brain region (Fregni et al., 2015).
References
Apps, M. A., and Tsakiris, M. (2014). The free-energy self: a predictive coding account of self-recognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 41, 85–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.029
Babinski, J. (1914). Contribution à l'étude des troubles mentaux dans l'hémiplégie organique cérébrale (anosognosie). Rev. Neurol. 27, 845–848.
Baier, B., and Karnath, H. O. (2008). Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Stroke 39, 486–488. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.495606
Berti, A., Bottini, G., Gandola, M., Pia, L., Smania, N., Stracciari, A., et al. (2005). Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control. Science 309, 488–491. doi: 10.1126/science.1110625
Berti, A., Làdavas, E., and Della Corte, M. (1996). Anosognosia for hemiplegia, neglect dyslexia, and drawing neglect: clinical findings and theoretical considerations. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2, 426–440. doi: 10.1017/S135561770000151X
Bisiach, E., and Geminiani, G. (1991). “Anosognosia related to hemiplegia and hemianopia,” in Awareness of Deficits After Brain Injury, eds G. P. Prigatano and D. L. Schacter (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 17–39.
Bisiach, E., Rusconi, M. L., and Vallar, G. (1991). Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 29, 1029–1031. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90066-H
Bisiach, E., Vallar, G., Perani, D., Papagno, C., and Berti, A. (1986). Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 24, 471–482. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90092-8
Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 237–242. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1
Bolognini, N., Zigiotto, L., Carneiro, M. I., and Vallar, G. (2016). “How Did I Make It?”: uncertainty about own motor performance after inhibition of the premotor cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1052–1061. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00950
Bottini, G., Bisiach, E., Sterzi, R., and Vallar, G. (2002). Feeling touches in someone else's hand. Neuroreport 13, 249–252. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200202110-00015
Bottini, G., and Gandola, M. (2015). Beyond the non-specific attentional effect of caloric vestibular stimulation: evidence from healthy subjects and patients. Multisens. Res. 28, 591–612. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002504
Bottini, G., Paulesu, E., Gandola, M., Loffredo, S., Scarpa, P., Sterzi, R., et al. (2005). Left caloric vestibular stimulation ameliorates right hemianesthesia. Neurology 65, 1278–1283. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000182398.14088.e8
Bottini, G., Paulesu, E., Sterzi, R., Warburton, E., Wise, R. J., Vallar, G., et al. (1995). Modulation of conscious experience by peripheral sensory stimuli. Nature 376, 778–781. doi: 10.1038/376778a0
Bruno, V., Fossataro, C., Bolognini, N., Zigiotto, L., Vallar, G., Berti, A., et al. (2017). The role of premotor and parietal cortex during monitoring of involuntary movement: a combined TMS and tDCS study. Cortex 96, 83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.001
Catani, M., Dell'acqua, F., Bizzi, A., Forkel, S. J., Williams, S. C., Simmons, A., et al. (2012). Beyond cortical localization in clinico-anatomical correlation. Cortex 48, 1262–1287. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.07.001
Fotopoulou, A. (2014). Time to get rid of the ‘Modular’ in neuropsychology: a unified theory of anosognosia as aberrant predictive coding. J. Neuropsychol. 8, 1–19. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12010
Fotopoulou, A. (2015). The virtual bodily self: mentalisation of the body as revealed in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Conscious. Cogn. 33, 500–510. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.09.018
Fotopoulou, A., Pernigo, S., Maeda, R., Rudd, A., and Kopelman, M. A. (2010). Implicit awareness in anosognosia for hemiplegia: unconscious interference without conscious re-representation. Brain 133 (Pt 12), 3564–3577. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq233
Fregni, F., Nitsche, M. A., Loo, C. K., Brunoni, A. R., Marangolo, P., Leite, J., et al. (2015). Regulatory considerations for the clinical and research use of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS): review and recommendations from an expert panel. Clin Res Regul Aff. 32, 22–35. doi: 10.3109/10601333.2015.980944
Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 360, 815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 355, 1771–1788. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0734
Gandola, M., Bottini, G., Zapparoli, L., Invernizzi, P., Verardi, M., Sterzi, R., et al. (2014a). The physiology of motor delusions in anosognosia for hemiplegia: Implications for current models of motor awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 24C, 98–112. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.01.001
Gandola, M., Sedda, A., Manera, M., Pingue, V., Salvato, G., Spitoni, G. F., et al. (2014b). Selective improvement of anosognosia for hemiplegia during transcranial direct current stimulation: a case report. Cortex 61, 107–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.007
Gerstmann, J. (1942). Problem of imperception of disease and of impaired body territories with organic lesions. Relation to body scheme and its disorders. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 48, 890–913.
Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man II. Brain 88, 585–644. doi: 10.1093/brain/88.3.585
Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., Romano, D., Zapparoli, L., Bottini, G., and Paulesu, E. (2012). What is mine? Behavioral and anatomical dissociations between somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia for hemiplegia. Behav. Neurol. 26, 139–150. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2012-110226
Jenkinson, P. M., and Fotopoulou, A. (2010). Motor awareness in anosognosia for hemiplegia: experiments at last! Exp. Brain Res. 204, 295–304. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1929-8
Jenkinson, P. M., Haggard, P., Ferreira, N. C., and Fotopoulou, A. (2013). Body ownership and attention in the mirror: insights from somatoparaphrenia and the rubber hand illusion. Neuropsychologia 51, 1453–1462. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.029
Jenkinson, P. M., Moro, V., and Fotopoulou, A. (2018). Definition: asomatognosia. Cortex 101, 300–301. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.001
Karnath, H. O., Baier, B., and Nägele, T. (2005). Awareness of the functioning of one's own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? J. Neurosci. 25, 7134–7138. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005
Marcel, A. J., Tegnér, R., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (2004). Anosognosia for plegia: specificity, extension, partiality and disunity of bodily unawareness. Cortex 40, 19–40. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70919-5
Moro, V., Pernigo, S., Tsakiris, M., Avesani, R., Edelstyn, N. M., Jenkinson, P. M., et al. (2016). Motor versus body awareness: voxel-based lesion analysis in anosognosia for hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia following right hemisphere stroke. Cortex 83, 62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.001
Moro, V., Pernigo, S., Zapparoli, P., Cordioli, Z., and Aglioti, S. M. (2011). Phenomenology and neural correlates of implicit and emergent motor awareness in patients with anosognosia for hemiplegia. Behav. Brain Res. 225, 259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.010
Pia, L., Spinazzola, L., Garbarini, F., Bellan, G., Piedimonte, A., Fossataro, C., et al. (2014). Anosognosia for hemianaesthesia: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study. Cortex 61, 158–166. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.006
Rode, G., Charles, N., Perenin, M. T., Vighetto, A., Trillet, M., and Aimard, G. (1992). Partial remission of hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia through vestibular stimulation in a case of unilateral neglect. Cortex 28, 203–208. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80048-2
Salvato, G., Gandola, M., Veronelli, L., Agostoni, E. C., Sberna, M., Corbo, M., et al. (2016). The spatial side of somatoparaphrenia: a case study. Neurocase 22, 154–160. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2015.1077257
Salvato, G., Gandola, M., Veronelli, L., Berlingeri, M., Corbo, M., and Bottini, G. (2018). “The vestibular system, body temperature and sense of body ownership: a potential link? Insights from a single case study”. Physiol Behav. 194, 522–526. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.008
Sedda, A., Tonin, D., Salvato, G., Gandola, M., and Bottini, G. (2016). Left caloric vestibular stimulation as a tool to reveal implicit and explicit parameters of body representation. Conscious. Cogn. 41, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.012
Spinazzola, L., Pia, L., Folegatti, A., Marchetti, C., and Berti, A. (2008). Modular structure of awareness for sensorimotor disorders: evidence from anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianaesthesia. Neuropsychologia 46, 915–926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.015
Thiebaut de Schotten, M., and Foulon, C. (2018). The rise of a new associationist school for lesion-symptom mapping. Brain 141, 2–4. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx332
Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G., and Haggard, P. (2006). Having a body versus moving your body: how agency structures body-ownership. Conscious. Cogn. 15, 423–432. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.004
Vallar, G., Bottini, G., Rusconi, M., and Sterzi, R. (1993). Exploring somatosensory hemineglect by vestibular stimulation. Brain 116, 71–86. doi: 10.1093/brain/116.1.71
Vallar, G., Bottini, G., and Sterzi, R. (2003). Anosognosia for left-sided motor and sensory deficits, motor neglect, and sensory hemiinattention: is there a relationship? Prog. Brain Res. 142, 289–301. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42020-7
Vallar, G., Sterzi, R., Bottini, G., Cappa, S., and Rusconi, M. L. (1990). Temporary remission of left hemianesthesia after vestibular stimulation. A sensory neglect phenomenon. Cortex 26, 123–131. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80078-0
Vocat, R., Staub, F., Stroppini, T., and Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Anosognosia for hemiplegia: a clinical-anatomical prospective study. Brain 133 (Pt 12), 3578–3597. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq297
Vocat, R., and Vuilleumier, P. (2010). “Neuroanatomy of impaired body awareness in anosognosia and hysteria: a multi-component account,” in The Study of Anosognosia, ed G. Prigatano (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 359–403.
Keywords: awareness, anosognosia for hemiplegia, somatoparaphrenia, caloric vestibular stimulation, consciousness
Citation: Bottini G, Magnani FG, Salvato G and Gandola M (2018) Multiple Dissociations in Patients With Disorders of Body Awareness: Implications for the Study of Consciousness. Front. Psychol. 9:2068. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02068
Received: 27 July 2018; Accepted: 08 October 2018;
Published: 26 October 2018.
Edited by:
Cecilia Guariglia, Università degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza, ItalyReviewed by:
Paul Mark Jenkinson, University of Hertfordshire, United KingdomCopyright © 2018 Bottini, Magnani, Salvato and Gandola. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Gabriella Bottini, Zy5ib3R0aW5pQHVuaXB2Lml0; Z2FicmllbGxhLmJvdHRpbmlAZ21haWwuY29t