- 1Département de Psychiatrie de l'Enfant et de l'Adolescent, AP-HP, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France
- 2Sorbonne Université, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France
- 3CRPMS, EA 3522, Université Paris Diderot, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France
- 4Equipe Berthoz, Collège de France, Paris, France
- 5Laboratoire CHArt-THIM, EA4004, Université Paris 8, Saint-Denis, France
Background: Impairments in imitation abilities have been commonly described in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). How motricity in interpersonal coordination impacts imitation, during long lasting semi-ecological conditions, has not been carefully investigated.
Methods: Eighty-five children and adolescents (39 controls with typical development, TD; 29 patients with ASD; 17 patients with developmental coordination disorder, DCD), aged 6 to 20 years, participated to a behavioral paradigm in which participants, standing and moving, interacted with a virtual tightrope walker standing and moving as well. During the protocol, we measured automatically and continuously bodily postures and movements from RGB sensor recording to assess participants' behavioral imitation.
Results: We show that (1) interpersonal synchronization (as evidenced by the synchrony between the participant's and the tightrope walker's bars) and (2) motor coordination (as evidenced by the synchrony between the participant's bar and its own head axis) increased with age and were more impaired in patients with ASD. Also, motor control as evidenced by the movement angle standard deviations of participants' bar and head were significantly impaired in ASD compared to TD or DCD.
Conclusion: Interpersonal synchronization and motor coordination during ecological interaction show both subtle impairment in children with ASD as compared to children with TD or DCD. These results questioned how motricity mature in terms of motor control and proprioception in children with ASD.
Introduction
Imitation plays a critical role in the development of intersubjectivity. It represents a key milestone in early communication (Gopnik and Meltzoff, 1993; Nadel and Potier, 2002; Rogers et al., 2005), a bedrock on which social cognition is built (Meltzoff, 2007) and a prerequisite of the self (Meltzoff, 2005). From an evolutional and developmental perspective, increased self-other distinction occurs through imitation, from motor mimicry (copying the style or the form of bodily movements of a model), to coordination and emulation (sharing the goals or the results of an action, but not the means used to achieve the goals), and to true imitation (reproduction of both the goals and the means of the observed actions) (de Waal, 2008). Motor imitation is a shared experience underlined by the perception that others are “like me” through an innate coupling between observation and execution of human actions, i.e., the existence of a structural congruence between the perception of others and one's own behavior (Meltzoff and Decety, 2003). Spontaneous motor imitation between children reveals a playful dynamic, driven by repetition, challenging the visual-spatial abilities of children (Xavier et al., 2013). It also requires continuous partner reciprocity involving synchrony rooted in rhythmic interpersonal coordination, which is promoted, during joint actions, by dynamic similarities in terms of motor signature (Xavier et al., 2017).
Impairments in imitation abilities have been described in neurodevelopmental disorders such as developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Children with DCD display motor incoordination and visual spatial processing deficits (Mazeau, 2000) which may affect their imitation abilities (Green et al., 2006; Werner et al., 2012). Autism known, as a spectrum disorder because it refers to a wide range of conditions, is defined by impaired social communication function and the presence of restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior or interest as core symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Children with ASD can also display visual spatial difficulties as well as impairments in motor coordination, control of posture, performance of gestures and complex movement sequences (Henderson et al., 1992; Ghaziuddin and Butler, 1998; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Fournier et al., 2010; MacNeil and Mostofsky, 2012). They may also manifest lower interpersonal synchrony (Marsh et al., 2013; Fitzpatrick et al., 2016) and developing deficits in control of movements (Mari et al., 2003; Rinehart et al., 2006; Dowd et al., 2012; Gowen and Hamilton, 2013). Thereon, Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt (2013) support the presence of a disturbance of primary prospective motor control of expressive action in ASD, affecting future social expectation and understanding. Many studies have linked ASD to problems with imitation (for recent reviews see Rogers and Williams, 2006; Nadel, 2014; Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014; Williams, 2018) which could be underlined by a dysfunction in the mirror neuron system (Williams et al., 2001). According to Goldman (2006), this system is involved in simulation theory, as low level simulation-based mindreading, permitting the observer to perform an action like the one being watched, thereby getting the observer “into the mental shoes” of the observed. In this way, problems in the functioning of mirror neuron system could be involved in social cognitive impairments in ASD (Wilkinson and Ball, 2012).
However, considering the heterogeneity of ASD (Xavier et al., 2015) as well as the heterogeneity of imitation performances in ASD (Rogers et al., 2010; Vivanti et al., 2011; Salowitz et al., 2013), inconsistencies and conflicting results exist regarding the nature of this deficit and the presence of a general imitation deficit specific per se in ASD (Vanvuchelen et al., 2011; Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014). Furthermore, the diversity of ages, tasks, and developmental levels of children participating in different studies makes comparison across findings difficult. To date, research has failed to clarify whether differences in imitation reflect a deviance (Rogers et al., 2003) or a delay (Young et al., 2011) from typical development.
Overall, studies found that, in comparison to typically developing (TD) children, children with ASD often have a low propensity to imitate (Vivanti et al., 2014), imitate less precisely, and appear to have more difficulties early in development than later on (Williams et al., 2004). Furthermore, in comparison to TD children, this population was found to display similar performances when imitated actions have a visual goal or meaning (Rogers et al., 1996; Gowen, 2012), and lower performances when imitation tasks are goal-less or meaningless (Williams et al., 2004; Hobson and Hobson, 2008; Rogers et al., 2010; Cossu et al., 2012; Nielsen et al., 2013) and when imitation tasks involve body postures or kinematics (Gowen, 2012; Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014). Contrary to performances in emulation tasks for which autistic participants tend to be proficient, it is likely that children with ASD engage less in mimicry behavior (the means of the action) than TD peers (Edwards, 2014). This suggests a failure to use the kinematic details of the action such as its amplitude, speed, or trajectory (Bekkering et al., 2000; Rumiati and Tessari, 2002; Carpenter et al., 2005; Wild et al., 2010). Several factors related to imitation deficits include poor visual encoding, self-other mapping problems (Williams et al., 2001), and motor, praxis or sensorimotor related disturbances (for a review see Vivanti et al., 2013).
Tasks involved in the abovementioned studies did not investigate imitation involving body movements while an individual is spontaneously interacting with another. Furthermore, comparison to other groups such as children with DCD is infrequent. The developmental aspects of motor imitation in children with ASD and DCD have not yet been addressed in a semi-ecological task such as Thirioux et al. 's experimental setup (2009). This spontaneous motor imitation task during an interaction between a participant and a tightrope walker avatar was first designed to investigate own-body-transformation (OBT) abilities of the participants. In a previous study comparing children with ASD or DCD and typically developing children (TD), we found that (1) OBT in a spatial environment was not possible in this experience before age 11; (2) yet it was possible later for patients with ASD although delayed compared with TD children (Gauthier et al., 2018).
Here we explore behavioral imitation abilities in terms of interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control by means of this interaction paradigm. To get a better understanding of imitation difficulties in children with ASD, we explored the potential alteration of the development of behavioral imitation abilities in children with ASD in comparison with DCD children and TD control children. ASD and DCD have in common motor and visual spatial difficulties. Therefore, comparing these two pathological groups offered the opportunity to disentangle the contribution of visual-spatial and motor coordination impairments in motor imitation difficulties.
The following hypotheses were made: (1) within the TD group, there will be a positive developmental/age effect on interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control. This developmental effect will be explored in the ASD and DCD groups. (2) When compared to TD children, children with ASD will show significant impairments in terms of interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control. (3) When compared to TD children, children with DCD will also show significant impairments in terms of interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control, but to a lesser extent compared to ASD Children.
Method
Participants
A total of 85 children and adolescents, aged 6 to 19 years, were recruited in the department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry of the Pitié-Salpêtrière University hospital. Given the lack of previous developmental study, we included a large control group with typical development. Inclusion criteria for the patients were (1) a diagnosis of DCD or a diagnosis of ASD; (2) the cognitive ability to understand the imitative task which was checked during a motor imitation game with the clinicians involved in the study. Exclusion criteria were ongoing medical conditions (e.g., seizures, sensory deficit) and severe language impairment that can be comorbid with ASD and DCD. For each patient, the diagnoses were based on all available information (including direct interviews, family history data, treatment records) and computed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Each patient was also given a series of clinical assessments: the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) was used to score autism core symptoms (Lord et al., 1994); the cognitive quotient was ascertained by using the WISC-IV (Wechsler Intelligent Scale for Children-IV), or the Psycho-educational Profile—Third Edition (PEP-3) (Schopler et al., 1990) according to age. Developmental age was calculated on this basis. Each child with ASD was individually matched, according to developmental age, with a healthy TD child using chronological age, assuming that for TD children, chronological and developmental age where equal. Children with DCD were evaluated during a psychomotor assessment that included quantitative testing (e.g., the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, M-ABC) (Henderson et al., 1992) performed by an occupational therapist. The TD children were recruited via the staff of the child and adolescent psychiatry department of the Pitié-Salpêtrière hospital. They were matched for age with the patients. Including written informed parental consent, the study was specifically reviewed and approved by an ethics committee, the CERES (Comité d'Ethique de la Recherche en Santé) [N° IRB: 20150700001072]. Among the 85 recruited participants, 5 (4 with ASD and 1 with DCD) did not complete the experiment and were not included in the analysis: 3 participants could not lean like the tightrope walker while not walking like the tightrope walker; 1 participant could partially perform the tasks but decided to stop it too early; 1 participant could not perform the tasks because he wanted to fight with him and to attack his eyes. Demographics and clinical characteristics of the 80 participants with exploitable data are given in Table 1.
The Tightrope Walker Paradigm
The tightrope walker paradigm (Thirioux et al., 2009) is an experimental setup designed to test the ability to change spatial viewpoints i.e., own-body transformations, during a spontaneous motor imitation task (Figure 1). We adapted the paradigm of the “funambule” tightrope walker to children by adjusting the size and giving it a cartoon aspect of a child. We developed a 3D animation where a 3D character is walking on a rope and hold a bar in front of him (Figure 1). The 3D animation was developed under Unity—a 3D engine used for virtual reality. The application runs on a PC under Windows 7. The natural movements of the 3D character come from a series of motion capture with the 12 cameras of VICON system. The animated tightrope walker (TW) walking on a rope was displayed life-sized by a rear-projector onto a large screen (2 * 2 m). It was 0.81 meter-high when standing in the middle part of his rope, 1.13 meter-high when he was the “closest” to the participant. To mimic everyday social encounters and to reinforce interactions giving participants the impression to act in the same spatial environment as the TW, participants stood on a black line (2 /10 cm; length/width) which prolonged on the ground the avatar's rope on the screen (Figure 1A). Before the movie started, we asked participants to find a comfortable position, legs slightly apart and not to shift from their position in response to the moves of the TW. Participants held a wooden bar (length: 1 m) horizontally in front of them. In order to reinforce both the interaction with the TW and the ecological features of the task, the TW's forward and backward movement's duration were randomized.
Figure 1. Principles and set-up of the experiment: (1A) Schematic illustration of the experimental room with the projection on the wall of the tightrope walker avatar; (1B1,2) Tightrope walker avatar's head and bar inclinations in the front-facing orientation.
Protocol and Tasks
First, the tightrope walker (TW) was shown in a front-facing orientation, standing with his right foot in front of the left on the rope for the first 30 s. Then, during 7 following trials, numbered from 1 to 7, the TW, walking successively either forward or backward, was alternatively shown in two orientations: (i) a front-facing orientation when the TW walks forward and a back-facing orientation when, the TW walking backward, the participants saw it from his back. While walking, for each orientation, the TW executed lateral tilts with his bar in random order either to his right or his left (Figure 1,B1,2), with a maximum amplitude of 51° (mean amplitude: 44°) and a maximum duration of 3.2 s (mean duration: 2.7 s). Each trial lasted 35.7 s and was composed of 7 TW's tilts. For half of the participants in each group, the first trial presents the TW front-facing and for the other half the first trial presents the TW back-facing. Participants were instructed to observe the tilts of the TW and to lean when he was leaning. Participants were also asked not to walk even when the TW was walking and to stand still when he was.
Data Recording and Metrics
Participants' bodily postures and movements in the frontal plane were automatically and continuously recorded for offline analysis and labeling. We used a RGB sensor (KINECT), located in front of them, on the wall above the TW, at a height of 1.85 m. The KINECT captured the figure of the participant at a mean rate of about 25 frames per second. The information contained by each frame was accessible through a comma-separated values (csv) file. For each frame, the participant's posture and the TW's posture were recorded as well as the timing of the frame, the participant's and the TW's bar and head inclinations were measured in degrees (Figure 2).
Figure 2. The inclination angles of the head and of the bar in the frontal plane were recorded for the participant and the avatar. The example shows the recording of the inclination in degrees of the participant's head and bar and tightrope walker avatar head and bar, according to the chronological progress of the frames.
The interpersonal synchronization defined as the synchrony between the participant's and the tightrope walker's bars was assessed using the correlation between the bar angles of the TW and that of the participants. For each participant, the motor coordination and the motor control were respectively measured evaluating (i) the correlation between the participant's bar and its head axis angle; (ii) the movement angle's standard deviations of participant's bar and head.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using R Software, Version 2.12.2. For every test, the level of significance, alpha, was fixed at 5%. To assess the variables of interests, we assessed the following dependent variables using Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM; lme4 package): correlation coefficient between the participant's bar and the tightrope walker's bar angles, correlation coefficient between the participant's bar and its head axis angles, standard deviation of the participant's bar movements, standard deviation of the participant's head. The following explicative variables were entered in each model: developmental age, type of group (TD vs. DCD vs. ASD), and the trial number (1 to 7). For each dependent variable, the normal distribution was checked. Variable transformations were conducted to reach normalization when needed.
Results
We have divided the results for two age groups under and above 12 years, based on the results of Gauthier et al. (2018) that used the same sample of participants to study own-body-transformation (OBT) abilities. The authors found that, in the front facing orientation when OBT involved a mental rotation, OBT was very difficult for participants under 12 and the rates of OBT were similar among the three groups (ASD, DCD, and TD). Consequently, we explored imitation abilities regarding the three following aspects: interpersonal synchronization, participant's motor coordination, and control.
Figure 3 shows the interpersonal synchronization of the interactive partners as measured by the correlation coefficients between the participant's bar and the tightrope walker's bar angles during the experiment according to groups and age. The GLMM model found several significant effects: correlation increased with age (β = 0.017, p < 0.001); correlation increased with the number of trials (β = 0.015, p < 0.001). There was also a significant effect according to groups: Correlation was smaller in the ASD group compared to both the TD group (β = −0.173, p < 0.001) and the DCD group (β = −0.124, p < 0.001).
Figure 3. Correlation coefficient between the participant's bar and the tightrope walker's bar during the experiment according to groups and age.
Figure 4 shows the participants' motor coordination as measured by the correlation coefficient between the participant's bar and its head axis angles during the experiment according to groups and age. The GLMM model found several significant differences: the correlation increased with age (β = 0.019, p < 0.001), and with the number of trials (β = 0.006, p < 0.034). There was also a significant effect according to groups. Correlation was smaller in ASD compared to both the TD group (β = −0.27, p < 0.001) and the DCD group (β = −0.20, p < 0.001).
Figure 4. Correlation coefficient between the participant's bar and its head axis during the experiment according to groups and age.
In terms of the participant's motor control, Figure 5 shows the standard deviations of the participant's bar and head angles during the experiment according to groups and age. For the participant's bar angle SD (Figure 4, left panel), the GLMM model found several significant effects. Mean SD decreased with age (β = −0.35, p = 0.029), increased with the number of trials (β = 1.2, p < 0.001). There was also a significant effect according to groups. Mean SD increased in ASD compared to TD (β = 7.14, p < 0.001) or compared to DCD (β = 7.15, p < 0.001). Results were very similar for the standard deviation of the participant's head angle (Figure 4, right panel). The GLMM model found several significant effects. Mean SD decreased with age (β = −0.26, p = 0.028), increased with the number of trials (β = 0.59, p < 0.001). There was also a significant effect according to groups. Mean SD increased in ASD compared to TD (β = 3.97, p < 0.001) or compared to DCD (β = 3.37, p < 0.001).
Figure 5. Mean standard deviations of the participant's bar and head movements during the experiment according to groups and age.
Discussion
In this study, we explored the development of behavioral imitation abilities, in terms of interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control, and their respective potential impairments in ASD and DCD. To elucidate the contribution of visual spatial impairments and motor coordination impairments in ASD, we compared three groups: a group of children with ASD, a group of children with DCD and a group of TD children. We used a quantification of movements to analyze how motor performances evolved during the experimental task. To the best of our knowledge, it is the first clinical study on children assessing quality of interpersonal synchrony and coordination from a semi-ecological point of view through a spontaneous motor imitation task.
In TD children, we found a strong developmental change on behavioral imitation abilities as evidenced by the improvement of interpersonal synchronization (between the participant's bar and the tightrope walker's bar angles), motor coordination (between the participant's bar and its head axis angles), and control (standard deviation of the participant's bar movements, standard deviation of the participant's head), with age. This developmental effect is in line with current knowledge on imitation abilities that increase during child development (Rogers and Williams, 2006; Xavier et al., 2017). We also found a significant increase of interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination and control with the number of trials. This means that children improved during the task which is a common result in semi-ecological continuous task when the duration is short enough to keep child's motivation and avoid fatigability (Chambaron et al., 2006).
In patients with neuro-developmental disorders, the results were quite different. Although all patients were able to perform the task, the ASD group showed significantly lower behavioral imitation abilities compared to the TD group. These results are in line with several studies in literature (Sevlever and Gillis, 2010; Nadel, 2014; Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014). In the current study involving a continuous and meaningless task, our results are concordant with those of several authors arguing for lower performances in comparison to TD (specifically in goalless actions) (Edwards, 2014; Williams et al., 2014) contrary to preserved performances found when imitated actions have a visual goal (Gowen, 2012). Contrary to Young et al. (2011) who argues that imitation abilities are delayed and then could improve with age, our results before and after 12 years old, revealed that imitation in ASD is following a deviant development. Furthermore, the results confirmed those obtained by Gauthier et al. (2018) using the same sample which aimed to explore own-body transformation in ASD. On the basis of a manual annotation of children's movement trajectories, the authors found that the ASD group had a number of imitation responses to the TW movements significantly lower compared to the TD and DCD groups.
A specific input of the current study is the continuous automatic measures of participants and TW avatars that our computational setting allowed. We found lower performances in interpersonal synchrony in the ASD group in comparison with both TD and DCD groups which is concordant with impairments in interpersonal synchrony (Marsh et al., 2013; Fitzpatrick et al., 2016) and kinematic aspects of imitation (Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014) described in this population. In line with several authors (Mari et al., 2003; Schmitz et al., 2003; Rinehart et al., 2006; Fournier et al., 2010; Dowd et al., 2012; MacNeil and Mostofsky, 2012; Wild et al., 2012; Gowen and Hamilton, 2013), we found that the ASD individuals had lower performances in motor control, as shown by the large standard deviation of bar and head angle during the experiment (Figure 4) that significantly differed from that of both TD and DCD groups. Furthermore, motor control and interpersonal synchronization were higher in DCD patients compared to ASD patients independent of the other characteristics entered in the multivariate models.
Based on these results, we can argue that in ASD motor imitation impairments could not entirely be explained by visual spatial impairments and DCD comorbidity that are often described in ASD (Ghaziuddin and Butler, 1998; Pan et al., 2009; Fournier et al., 2010; MacNeil and Mostofsky, 2012). It is likely that the low performances found in ASD are related to impairments concerning the regulation of movement. In an ecological context close to ours, Fitzpatrick et al. (2013) investigated the dynamics of interactional synchronization in children and adolescents with ASD. Using a battery of imitation tasks (action sequences movements), they found that school-age children with ASD had lower social motor synchronization abilities than TD controls. In the same way, using a pendulum coordination paradigm, Fitzpatrick et al. (2016) found that adolescents with ASD performed worst on social motor synchronization tasks than TD controls. The authors argue that synchronization difficulties included in social motor coordination in ASD may be related to motor control impairments which manifested by more jerky and less accurate movements (Cook et al., 2013; Vivanti and Hamilton, 2014).
In our study, impairments in motor control found in ASD group are in accordance with those of several studies exploring motor interaction using robotic platforms. First, in a child/robot interaction built to induce joint attention, Anzalone et al. (2014) found a motor variability in the movements of both head and trunk in children with ASD. Boucenna et al. (2014, 2016) used a human-robot learning paradigm based on imitation of postures. They studied the impact of a human partner on the learning abilities of postures by a humanoid robot (Nao). They found that when Nao was interacting with participants, it was able to learn a social signature at the level of the group (meaning children with ASD as opposed to TD children) (Boucenna et al., 2014) but also at the level of individual recognition (Boucenna et al., 2016). In a seminal study, Guedjou et al. (2017) further showed that when Nao was interacting with children with ASD, the posture recognition was lower than that found after interacting with TD children. Altogether, the authors interpreted the specificity of Nao learning during motor imitation with children with ASD to a variability of the movement trajectories shown by these children.
Development of motor control requires forming an internal model of action relying on the coupling between action (motor commands) and perception (sensory feedback). Critical to the development of social, communicative, and motor coordination behaviors, internal model of action accurately predicts the sensory consequences of motor commands (Krakauer and Shadmehr, 2007). Thereupon, considering human brain organized by principles of Bayesian inferences and predictive coding, several authors recently proposed that autism may be a disorder of prediction (Van Boxtel and Lu, 2013; Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2015; Bolis and Schilbach, 2017). This hypothesis of impaired prediction could potentially account for several significant correlates of autism as a reduced motor anticipation (see Sinha et al., 2014). In addition, interpersonal predictive coding incorporates time aspects (Manera et al., 2013) specifically highlighted in our setting using an ecologically valid real-time interaction. In that respect and according to Von der Lühe et al. (2016), impairments in imitation abilities with difficulties in interpersonal synchrony found in the ASD group are consistent with this hypothesis of predictive coding impairment in ASD.
Our results should be interpreted taking into consideration some limitations. First, the number of participants in the three groups was somewhat restricted, in particular in the DCD group compared to the two others. Second, visual attention abilities of the participants were not assessed despite their importance in performing the task. Finally, further studies could assess motor skills of ASD participants in accordance with DCD participants which would add consistency to the study confirming that, at the same motor abilities level, ASD group had more behavioral imitation impairments. However, it should be noted that, in the current study, motor control and motor coordination abilities involved in the imitation task correspond to milestones in very early child development, i.e., postural development in TD infants (André-Thomas and Ajuriaguerra, 1948; Lindsay et al., 2013).
Conclusion
Behavioral imitation abilities during an ecological interaction showed subtle impairment in children with ASD as compared to children with TD or DCD, both in terms of interpersonal synchrony and motor coordination. These results questioned how motricity matures in terms of motor control and proprioception in children with ASD. Exploring motor control from a developmental point of view through a dynamic process like imitation poses significant pragmatic challenges for researchers and clinicians alike. In this regard, computational modeling involving human-machine interaction may be promising.
Author Contributions
All co-authors have participated in writing this manuscript. JX, DC, FV, MC, AB, and SA were involved in the introduction, discussion, and results parts of the manuscript. SG, SA, and MZ carried out the experimental part of our work (testing procedure and material). They participated in the writing of the description of the methodology of our work.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by the IHU-A-ICM (Institut des Neurosciences Translationnelles de Paris). The sponsor had no involvement in the study design, data analysis, or interpretation of the results.
References
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Publishing Incorporated.
André-Thomas, A. S., and Ajuriaguerra, J. D. (1948). L'axe Corporel, Musculature et Innervation. Paris: Masson.
Anzalone, S. M., Tilmont, E., Boucenna, S., Xavier, J., Jouen, A. L., Bodeau, N., et al. (2014). How children with autism spectrum disorder behave and explore the 4-dimensional (spatial 3D+time) environment during a joint attention induction task with a robot. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 8, 814–826. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.03.002
Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., and Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 53, 153–164. doi: 10.1080/713755872
Bolis, D., and Schilbach, L. (2017). Observing and participating in social interactions: action perception and action control across the autistic spectrum. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.009
Boucenna, S., Anzalone, S., Tilmont, E., Cohen, D., and Chetouani, M. (2014). Learning of social signatures through imitation game between a robot and a human partner. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 6, 213–225. doi: 10.1109/TAMD.2014.2319861
Boucenna, S., Cohen, D., Meltzoff, A. N., Gaussier, P., and Chetouani, M. (2016). Robots learn to recognize individuals from imitative encounters with people and avatars. Sci. Rep. 6:19908. doi: 10.1038/srep19908
Carpenter, M., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve-and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Dev. Sci. 8, 13–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x
Chambaron, S., Ginhac, D., Ferrel-Chapus, C., and Perruchet, P. (2006). Implicit learning of a repeated segment in continuous tracking: a reappraisal. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 845–854. doi: 10.1080/17470210500198585
Cook, J., Blakemore, S., and Press, C. (2013). Atypical basic movement kinematics in autism spectrum conditions. Brain 136, 2816–2824. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt208
Cossu, G., Boria, S., Copioli, C., Bracceschi, R., Giuberti, V., Santelli, E., et al. (2012). Motor representation of actions in children with autism. PLoS ONE 7:e44779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044779
de Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625
Dowd, A. M., McGinley, J. L., Taffe, J. R., and Rinehart, N. J. (2012). Do planning and visual integration difficulties underpin motor dysfunction in autism? A kinematic study of young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1539–1548. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1385-8
Edwards, L. A. (2014). A meta-analysis of imitation abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: meta-analysis of imitation in ASD. Autism Res. 7, 363–380. doi: 10.1002/aur.1379
Fitzpatrick, P., Diorio, R., Richardson, M. J., and Schmidt, R. C. (2013). Dynamical methods for evaluating the time-dependent unfolding of social coordination in children with autism. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:21. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00021
Fitzpatrick, P., Frazier, J. A., Cochran, D. M., Mitchell, T., Coleman, C., and Schmidt, R. C. (2016). Impairments of social motor synchrony evident in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 7:1323. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01323
Fitzpatrick, P., Mitchell, T., Frazier, J., Cochran, D, Coleman, C., and Schmidt, R. C. (2016). “Exploring the behavioral and neural processes underlying social synchronization of individuals with and without social deficits,” in Studies in Perception and Action XIII: Eighteenth International Conference on Perception and Action, eds J. Weast-Knapp, M. Malone, and D. Abney (New York, NY: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis Group).
Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3
Gauthier, S., Anzalone, S. M., Cohen, D., Chetouani, M., Villa, F., Berthoz, A., et al. (2018). Behavioral own-body-transformations in children and adolescents with typical development, autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Front. Psychol. 9:676. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00676
Ghaziuddin, M., and Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: a further report. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 42, 43–48. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.1998.00065.x
Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating Minds: the Philosophy, Psychology and Neuroscience of Mindreading.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Chennu, S., Bekinschtein, T. A., Rattazzi, A., Beraudi, A., Trippichio, P., et al. (2015). Predictive coding in autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 2625–2636. doi: 10.1152/jn.00543.2015
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. (1993). Imitation, cultural learning and the origins of theory of mind. Behav. Brain Sci. 16, 521–523.
Gowen, E. (2012). Imitation in autism: why action kinematics matter. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:117. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00117
Gowen, E., and Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: a review using a computational context. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 323–344. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0
Green, D., Baird, G., and Sugden, D. (2006). A pilot study of psychopathology in developmental coordination disorder. Child Care Health Dev. 32, 741–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00684.x
Guedjou, H., Boucenna, S., Xavier, J., Cohen, D., and Chetouani, M. (2017). “The influence of individual social traits on robot learning in a human-robot interaction,” in 26th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN).
Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., and Barnett, A. L. (1992). Movement Assesment Battery for Children. London: Psychological Corporation.
Hobson, R. P., and Hobson, J. A. (2008). Dissociable aspects of imitation: a study in autism. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 170–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.007
Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., Newschaffer, C. J., Denckla, M. B., Landa, R., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome from controls. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 613–621. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0109-y
Krakauer, J. W., and Shadmehr, R. (2007). “Towards a computational neuropsychology of action,” in Progress in Brain Research (Elsevier), 383–394. Available online at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0079612306650243 (Accessed November 13, 2017).
Lindsay, S., Proulx, M., Thomson, N., and Scott, H. (2013). Educators' challenges of including children with autism spectrum disorder in mainstream classrooms. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 60, 347–362. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2013.846470
Lord, C., Rutter, M., and Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145
MacNeil, L. K., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2012). Specificity of dyspraxia in children with autism. Neuropsychology 26, 165–171. doi: 10.1037/a0026955
Manera, V., Schouten, B., Verfaillie, K., and Becchio, C. (2013). Time will show: real time predictions during interpersonal action perception. PLoS ONE 8:e54949. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054949
Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., and Prior, M. (2003). The reach–to–grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 393–403. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1205
Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Verbalis, A. D., Schmidt, R. C., et al. (2013). Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:4. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00004
Mazeau, M. (2000). Troubles neuroviuels et praxiques: un élément déterminant du pronostic à long terme. Méd. Thér. Pédiatr. 3, 273–280.
Meltzoff, A. (2005). “Imitation and other minds: the ‘like me’ hypothesis,” in Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science: Vol. 2: Imitation, Human Development, and Culture, eds S. Hurley, N. Chater, S. Hurley, and N. Chater (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 55–77.
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: a foundation for social cognition. Dev. Sci. 10, 126–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x
Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 491–500. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1261
Nadel, J. (2014). Réhabiliter scientifiquement l'imitation au bénéfice de l'autisme. L'inform. Psychiatr. 90, 835–842. doi: 10.1684/ipe.2014.1276
Nadel, J., and Potier, C. (2002). “Imiter et être imité dans le développement de l'intentionnalité,” in Imiter Pour Découvrir l'Humain - Psychologie, Neurobiologie, Robotique et Philosophie de l'esprit (Paris), 232.
Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., and Dissanayake, C. (2013). Object-directed imitation in children with high-functioning autism: testing the social motivation hypothesis: autism, overimitation, and synchronic imitation. Autism Res. 6, 23–32. doi: 10.1002/aur.1261
Pan, C. Y., Tsai, C. L., and Chu, C. H. (2009). Fundamental movement skills in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1694–1705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0813-5
Rinehart, N. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Tonge, B. J., Brereton, A. V., Howells-Rankin, D., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2006). An examination of movement kinematics in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder: further evidence for a motor planning deficit. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 757–767. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0118-x
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., and Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Dev. 67, 2060–2073. doi: 10.2307/1131609
Rogers, S. J., Cook, I., and Meryl, A. (2005). Imitation and play in autism. Handb. Autism Pervasive Dev. Disord. 1, 382–405. doi: 10.1002/9780470939345.ch14
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., and Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 44, 763–781. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00162
Rogers, S. J., and Williams, J. H. G. (2006). Imitation and the Social Mind - Autism and Typical Development. New York, NY: Guildford Press.
Rogers, S. J., Young, G. S., Cook, I., Giolzetti, A., and Ozonoff, S. (2010). Imitating actions on objects in early-onset and regressive autism: effects and implications of task characteristics on performance. Dev. Psychopathol. 22, 71–85. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990277
Rumiati, R., and Tessari, A. (2002). Imitation of novel and well-known actions. Exp. Brain Res. 142, 425–433. doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0956-x
Salowitz, N. M., Eccarius, P., Karst, J., Carson, A., Schohl, K., Stevens, S., et al. (2013). Brief report: visuo-spatial guidance of movement during gesture imitation and mirror drawing in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 985–995. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1631-8
Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthélémy, C., and Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: deficit of anticipatory function? Neurosci. Lett. 348, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00644-X
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., Bashford, A., Lansing, M. D., and Marcus, L. M. (1990). Individualied Assessment of Autistic and Developmentally Disabled Children: Psychoeducational Profile Revised (PEP-R). Austin TX.
Sevlever, M., and Gillis, J. M. (2010). An examination of the state of imitation research in children with autism: issues of definition and methodology. Res. Dev. Disabil. 31, 976–984. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.014
Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., et al. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 15220–15225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416797111
Thirioux, B., Jorland, G., Bret, M., Tramus, M. H., and Berthoz, A. (2009). Walking on a line: a motor paradigm using rotation and reflection symmetry to study mental body transformations. Brain Cogn. 70, 191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.006
Trevarthen, C., and Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2013). Autism as a developmental disorder in intentional movement and affective engagement. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:49. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00049
Van Boxtel, J. J., and Lu, H. (2013). A predictive coding perspective on autism spectrum disorders. Front. Psychol. 4:19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00019
Vanvuchelen, M., Roeyers, H., and De Weerdt, W. (2011). Do imitation problems reflect a core characteristic in autism? Evidence from a literature review. Res. Autism Spectrum Disord. 5, 89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.07.010
Vivanti, G., Barbaro, J., Hudry, K., Dissanayake, C., and Prior, M. (2013). Intellectual development in autism spectrum disorders: new insights from longitudinal studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:354. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00354
Vivanti, G., and Hamilton, A. (2014). “Imitation in autism spectrum disorders,” in Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Vol 1: Diagnosis, Development, and Brain Mechanisms, 278–302. doi: 10.1002/9781118911389
Vivanti, G., McCormick, C., Young, G. S., Abucayan, F., Hatt, N., Nadig, A., et al. (2011). Intact and impaired mechanisms of action understanding in autism. Dev. Psychol. 47, 841–856. doi: 10.1037/a0023105
Vivanti, G., Trembath, D., and Dissanayake, C. (2014). Mechanisms of imitation impairment in autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 1395–1405. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9874-9
Von der Lühe, T., Manera, V., Barisic, I., Becchio, C., Vogeley, K., and Schilbach, L. (2016). Interpersonal predictive coding, not action perception, is impaired in autism. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150373. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0373
Werner, J. M., Cermak, S. A., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2012). Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: the mirror neuron system hypothesis. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 258–268. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2012.22029
Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., and Gowen, E. (2010). The influence of goals on movement kinematics during imitation. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 353–360. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2034-8
Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., and Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1739–1749. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1417-4
Wilkinson, M. R., and Ball, L. J. (2012). Why studies of autism spectrum disorders have failed to resolve the theory theory versus simulation theory debate. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 3, 263–291. doi: 10.1007/s13164-012-0097-0
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., and Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 285–299. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000029551.56735.3a
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 287–295. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00014-8
Williams, J. H. G. (2018). “Action evaluation and discrimination as indexes of imitation fidelity in autism,” in Autism: The Movement-Sensing Perspective, eds E. B Torres and C. Whyatt (CRC Press), 89–103.
Williams, J. H. G., Casey, J. M., Braadbaart, L., Culmer, P. R., and Mon-Williams, M. (2014). Kinematic measures of imitation fidelity in primary school children. J. Cogn. Dev. 15, 345–362. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.771265
Xavier, J., Bursztejn, C., Stiskin, M., Canitano, R., and Cohen, D. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders: an historical synthesis and a multidimensional assessment toward a tailored therapeutic program. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 18, 21–33. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.06.011
Xavier, J., Magnat, J., Sherman, A., Gauthier, S., Cohen, D., and Chaby, L. (2017). A developmental and clinical perspective of rhythmic interpersonal coordination: from mimicry toward the interconnection of minds. J. Physiol. 110, 420–426. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2017.06.001
Xavier, J., Tilmont, E., and Bonnot, O. (2013). Children's synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: toward a developmental model of empathy. J. Physiol. 107, 291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.012
Keywords: imitation, interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination, motor control, autism spectrum disorder, developmental coordination disorder
Citation: Xavier J, Gauthier S, Cohen D, Zahoui M, Chetouani M, Villa F, Berthoz A and Anzalone SM (2018) Interpersonal Synchronization, Motor Coordination, and Control Are Impaired During a Dynamic Imitation Task in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychol. 9:1467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01467
Received: 19 November 2017; Accepted: 25 July 2018;
Published: 03 September 2018.
Edited by:
Jessica Lindblom, University of Skövde, SwedenReviewed by:
Bruno Wicker, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), FranceMeredith Ria Wilkinson, De Montfort University, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2018 Xavier, Gauthier, Cohen, Zahoui, Chetouani, Villa, Berthoz and Anzalone. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jean Xavier, jean.xavier@aphp.fr