Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 August 2018
Sec. Cognition

The Role of Item-Specific Information for the Retrieval Awareness of Performed Actions

  • 1School of Education Science, Jiangsu Normal University, Xuzhou, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Northeast Normal University, Changchun, China

Research on action memory has been pursued for more than 30 years, but it is still unclear what drives the recollection process of performed actions. In this study, we used the remember/know paradigm and designed two experiments to examine the relation between item-specific processing and retrieval awareness of subject-performed tasks (SPT). The results showed that SPT allows remember responses in remember-know judgments more easily; that is, SPT can enhance the frequency of recalling re-collective experience. Item-specific processing can improve the memory performance and the proportion of remember judgments of verbal tasks (VT), but it does not improve the memory performance and proportion of remember judgments of SPT, indicating that SPT can enhance item-specific processing, which leads to more remember responses in judgment. The relation between item-specific processing and retrieval awareness of SPT is also discussed.

Introduction

Memory for simple action phrases (such as “pick up the pencil” or “break the stick”) are retained better when the participants are instructed to learn the phrases while enacting them rather than just listening or reading. The excellent memory performance in subject-performed tasks (SPT), as compared to verbal tasks (VT), has been called the “enactment effect” or “SPT effect” (for reviews see Cohen, 1989; Engelkamp, 1998; Zimmer et al., 2007). This effect is reliable and robust and has been observed in a variety of encoding and test conditions. It is observed whether the subjects are performing actions with actual objects or only using gestures (for reviews see Madan and Singhal, 2012a), whether a recall or recognition test is used (Cohen, 1989), and whether the subjects are young children or old people (Feyereisen, 2009; Mecklenbräuker et al., 2011). In a large sample (N = 1,000) analysis, about 88% of subjects showed significant SPT effects, and the degree of memory enhancement following encoding enactment is substantial, often in the range of 20–30% (Nyberg et al., 2002). Although the SPT effect has been studied for more than 30 years, it has not been determined what drives the recollection process for performed actions. In the present study, we investigate this issue.

Retrieval Awareness of Performed Actions

Episodic memory is the ability to consciously remember one's experiences of events that occurred at a particular time and place and is typically characterized by temporal and spatial details (Tulving, 1985). Tulving (2002, 1985), associated different levels of memory systems with different levels of consciousness and connected episodic memory with the highest level of human consciousness—auto-noetic consciousness. Tulving (2002) pointed out that the individual who is engaged in the subjective re-experience of the event, accompanied by a personal emotional state, is undergoing auto-noetic consciousness. Semantic memory, conversely, involves the general concepts, and knowledge of the world that are shared by others; thus, the memory is not accompanied by personal feelings but only by noetic consciousness.

Previous studies have often adopted the remember/know paradigm to test auto-noetic consciousness and noetic consciousness (Tulving, 1985; Gardiner, 1988, 2001). The procedure involves a recognition test, where people are asked to identify the basis on which they judged that an item had been previously studied—whether they “remembered” its prior occurrence or simply “knew” on some other basis that it was old (Tulving, 1985; Gardiner, 2001). A remember response is taken to indicate a state of auto-noetic awareness, defined as the subjects being consciously aware of some aspects of what they experienced when the item was first learned (Tulving, 1985; for a more detailed discussion see Gardiner, 2001). A know response is taken to indicate a state of noetic awareness, defined as recognition that the item had been presented earlier but without the ability to recollect consciously anything about its actual occurrence or what the subject experienced at that time (Tulving, 1985; for a more detailed discussion see Gardiner, 2001).

This aspect is often overlooked while researching into the SPT effect. Although Kormi-Nouri (1995) proposed the episodic integration theory to explain it and suggested that a good self-involvement level accompanied by enactment caused the effect, this study did not provide evidence. To our knowledge, only a handful of studies have examined the retrieval awareness in SPT. The first study of the re-collective experience of SPT was reported by Conway and Dewhurst (1995). In their experiment, the subjects studied a series of phrases by performing, watching, or imagining them. Then in a recognition test, they were asked to judge whether their recognitions were made on the basis of conscious re-collective experience (remember responses), or on some other basis such as familiarity (know responses). The authors found that the proportion of correct remember responses was much higher for performed actions than for observed actions (see also Manzi and Nigro, 2008). However, because both studies presented corresponding objects during the performing actions, it is not possible to determine whether the remember responses occurred more frequently for performed actions because of the enactment or because of the abundant sensory information brought by the objects. Engelkamp and Dehn (1997) solved this problem and compared the relative contributions of familiarity and recollection to recognition following SPT and VT without the presence of the relevant objects. They found an increase in remember judgments after SPT as compared to VT and concluded that the SPT effect is due mainly to an enhanced recollection process after SPT (see also Lövdén et al., 2002).

In spite of this, it is still not clear what drives the recollection process of SPT. Some researchers pointed out that the characteristics of the encoding shape the recollection process of SPT (Zimmer et al., 2001; Magnussen and Helstrup, 2007), but these studies were all reviews and did not provide empirical support. Gardiner et al. (2006) found that the recognition performance and the remember responses were not enhanced by SPT in a case of developmental amnesia in which the episodic memory was impaired. More recently, Zhao et al. (2016) pointed out that enactment unites the components of actions so that familiarity can support associative recognition following SPT; however, their study did not provide behavioral evidence. Therefore, the purpose of the present study is to use the remember/know paradigm and make VT a control condition to explore what drives the recollection process of SPT.

Item-Specific Processing of Performed Actions

Item-specific information involves the characteristics of an item that makes it unique and discriminable from other items that are studied together with it. Most of the research suggests that SPT promotes item-specific knowledge, making the item more specific and distinguishable (e.g., Steffens et al., 2006, 2009; Kubik et al., 2016). The item-specific information accompanied by SPT can be especially effective in leading to the memory advantage of encoding by enacting (Seiler and Engelkamp, 2003; Schatz et al., 2011). The assumption of enhanced item-specific processing is based on the result that the enactment effect is much more notable in recognition and cued-recall than in free-recall (Steffens et al., 2006, 2009). These findings have been explained by the fact that different ways of testing depend on different processing mechanisms. Recognition and cued-recall tests mainly examine, respectively, the feeling of familiarity with the item and the degree of connection between the clue and the target word, which reflects item-specific information. However, free-recall tests also require relational information, which is generally not heightened by SPT over VT when no action-related objects are presented (Engelkamp et al., 2005).

For example, using lists of independent items, Steffens et al. (2009) discovered that the parameter measuring the item-specific processing was enhanced. They argued that SPT is more complete in the semantic-related properties of action phrases. Engelkamp (1995) pointed out that the participants focus their attention on the enactment task and ignore irrelevant environmental details while the action is being performed. As a result, they gain more details. Kormi-Nouri (1995) suggested that during the execution of the action, participants focus on the verb, the noun, and the whole action phrase, so that the information in the action phrase is fully processed by enactment. Moreover, on this basis, Steffens et al. (2006) argued that enactment can enhance item-specific information mainly in the following three respects: (1) the item-specific information of the noun, (2) the item-specific information of the verb, and (3) the integration of the verb and the noun (which they called “retrieval”; see Steffens et al., 2006, 2009, for more details). These results have been supported by subsequent studies (Kubik et al., 2014a,b; Schult et al., 2014; Mulligan and Peterson, 2015).

As mentioned above, most of the studies of item-specific processing focused on whether SPT enhances item-specific information, and no studies have explored the relationship between the item-specific processing and retrieval awareness. For this reason, the current study did not present the corresponding objects in the experiments, and used VT as the comparison group, to explore the retrieval awareness of SPT. Experiment 1 used the remember/know paradigm to verify that SPT is accompanied by more re-collective retrieval. Experiment 2 examined the hypothesis that item-specific processing leads to more remember responses.

Experiment 1

The aim of Experiment 1 was to use the remember/know paradigm to verify the existing research conclusions. We assumed that a reliable SPT effect would be obtained and more remember responses would occur for SPT than for VT.

Methods

Subjects and Experimental Design

Prior to the experiment, we used G*Power (Erdfelder et al., 1996) to calculate the sample size, which was sufficient to obtain 0.80 power for a 0.05 probability error. In this experiment, we expected an effect size of 0.45 and a correlation among repeated measures of 0.5. The sample size was 19 subjects. We recruited 20 students (7 males and 13 females, M age = 22.50, SD = 1.20) as participants from Jilin University in China. A single within-subjects design was used for the study. The protocol was approved by the Ethnic Committee of Jilin University, and written consent was obtained from all the participants before conducting the test.

Materials

We selected 96 action phrases (e.g., “open the textbook,” and “pick up the pencil”, see Table A1) that were roughly matched in word length (2–4 Chinese characters). Half the phrases (48) were used for study, and the other half (48) were used for counter-balanced distractors. The phrases used in the study were independent of parts of the body (e.g., “scratching the head”) and did not refer to objects in the laboratory (e.g., “banging on the table”), because these types of phrases would have clear retrieval clues and could be recalled easily, thereby interfering with the SPT effect (Steffens et al., 2007).

We randomly recruited 17 additional students to assess the familiarity of the phrases before the experiment on a 7-point scale (1 indicating lowest familiarity, 7 indicating highest familiarity). One-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess the results; the difference in familiarity between the learning materials (M = 4.26, SD = 0.85) and the interfering materials (M = 4.17, SD = 0.79) was not significant, F(1, 16) = 1.26, MSE = 0.057, p = 0.286, and η2p = 0.07.

Procedure

The experimental procedure had two stages, learning and testing, which were compiled by E-prime 2.0. Each study trial began with a fixation cross (+) displayed at the center of the screen for 2,000 ms. An action phrase was then centrally displayed against a white background for 6,000 ms. The subjects were told to study the items for the subsequent recognition test. Half the learning words (24) were studied in SPT: the subjects were asked to read the phrases silently first and then pretend to perform the actions with imaginary objects. The other half (24) were studied in VT: the subjects were asked to read the phrases silently but were prohibited from making unnecessary hand movements. For the learning sequence, half of the subjects first studied in SPT and then in VT, while the other half took the reverse order. After the last word was presented, the subjects were told to solve four funny math problems in 5 min, and then the recognition test was conducted.

In the test phase, old and new items were randomly presented to the subjects on the computer screen. The subjects were asked to determine whether the items were new (key labeled X) or old (key labeled J). If they were sure that the item was old, they were required to make a remember/know judgment by pressing the “R” or “K” key within 6 s. They were told to press “R” if they consciously remembered the items and could recall the details of learning it (such as the number of the silent reading, the magnitude of the action, the learning mood, the items before or after it, and if the phrase let the subject re-experience the past). They were told to press “K” if they were sure that the word had been presented but could not recollect its actual occurrence or any related details. If they were sure that the word was new, the remember/know judgment was not applicable, and they were required to press the “X” key. All the subjects were required to keep their fingers on the keys throughout the experiment and to respond to the tasks as quickly as possible without sacrificing accuracy. The entire experiment lasted for about 15–20 min.

Data Calculation Method

This study adopted the remember/know recognition paradigm and used the method of signal detection to calculate hit rates and false-alarm rates. In all the following analyses, a hit was defined as responding “old” to a repeated word, and a false alarm as responding “old” to a new word. We could not calculate the false-alarm rates under every specific encoding condition because of the within-subjects design. Therefore, we only calculated the false-alarm rates of the recognition test and the remember/know judgment.

Results

Table 1 presents the mean proportions of hits and false alarms under different study conditions and response types.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean proportion of hits and false alarms as a function of study conditions and response type in Experiment 1.

A one-way ANOVA was done to explore the order effect (SPT – VT vs. VT – SPT) on the recognition performance of SPT and VT, respectively. The order effect did not have a significant difference on SPT and VT. The corresponding values were, respectively, F(1, 18) = 2.31, p = 0.15, η2p = 0.11, and F(1, 18) = 0.98, p = 0.36, η2p = 0.05. For completeness, we also adopted a non-parametric statistical approach and found similar results.

One-way repeated measures ANOVA was used for recognition performance. The result showed that the main effect of the encoding condition was significant, F(1, 19) = 20.37, MSE = 0.009, p = 0.001, and η2p = 0.52, thereby indicating that the performance of SPT was significantly better than that of VT.

One-way repeated measures ANOVA was used for remember responses. The results showed a significant main effect of the encoding condition, F(1, 19) = 17.12, MSE = 0.029, p < 0.01, and η2p = 0.47, thereby demonstrating that more remember responses were given to the items in SPT than in VT. However, the effect was reversed for know responses: more “K” responses were given to the items in VT than in SPT, F(1, 19) = 4.95, MSE = 0.017, p < 0.05, and η2p = 0.21.

Discussion

For the recognition performance, the study demonstrated a significant SPT effect. This supports the previous results that the SPT effect is stable, and this stability does not depend on the test method or on the experimental design type (Roediger and Zaromb, 2010). For the remember/know judgment, more remember responses were given to the items in SPT than in VT. This is also similar to the previous results. Earlier studies have shown that SPT engenders remember responses more easily than Experimenter Performed Tasks (EPT) (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995; Manzi and Nigro, 2008) and VT (Engelkamp and Dehn, 1997), and that different encoding types can stably influence the frequency of recalling re-collective experiences. In this study, the difference between SPT and VT was that subjects in SPT were required to perform corresponding actions while those in VT were not. Therefore, we suggest that performing an action constitutes an episodic event, which allows participants to retrieve more specific information. If the specific information is more abundant in the encoding, there will be more retrieval cues.

Experiment 2

This experiment was conducted to verify the results of Experiment 1. However, we adopted the remember/know paradigm to evaluate the hypothesis that item-specific processing leads to more remember responses. Specifically, the experiment was based on the following two considerations: First, because of the within-subjects design of Experiment 1, we could not calculate the false-alarm rates under each encoding condition. To overcome this problem, in Experiment 2 we used a between-subjects design, and this also allowed us to verify whether the experimental design affects the results. Second, the remember/know judgment was based directly on whether the subject had specific information about the learned item, and this paradigm was more straightforward for measuring item-specific information. To enhance the item-specific processing, the subjects were asked to rate the pleasantness of each action phrase; this technique is frequently used in orienting tasks to focus the subject on item-specific information (e.g., Seiler and Engelkamp, 2003).

Remember responses are often associated with item-specific information. Therefore, it can be speculated that if the participants pay attention to item-specific information during the encoding, they will make more remember responses in the judgment. If SPT enhances item-specific processing, and the participants recall more details of the items, and their memory performance is better, then the additional item-specific processing cannot enhance the memory performance as well as the remember responses of SPT. On the other hand, the item-specific information is assumed to play a smaller role in VT, so the pleasantness rating should enhance the item-specific processing. Encoding and retrieval processes in VT should approximate those of SPT, and the memory performance as well as the remember responses should be improved in VT.

Methods

Experimental Design

A 2 (encoding conditions: SPT and VT) × 2 (instructional methods: standard instruction and pleasantness rating) between-subjects design was used.

Subjects and Materials

Because of the between-subjects design, we expected an effect size of 0.3. The sample size required was 80 subjects, with 20 in each condition. Therefore, we recruited 80 participants (47 males and 33 females, M age = 22.08, SD = 1.20) from Jilin University in China. The materials were same as those in Experiment 1.

Procedure

The procedure was basically the same as that in Experiment 1. However, we added the pleasantness rating task in addition to the standard instruction condition: the subjects were asked to evaluate the pleasantness of each action phrase on a five-point scale (ranging from 1 = unpleasant to 5 = pleasant) during the study, both in VT and SPT. They were required to make a snap decision. In SPT, the rating followed the enactment.

Data Calculation Method

The data analysis was also similar to that of Experiment 1, except that we adopted a between-subjects design. This let us calculate the rates of hits and false alarms under different conditions. The discriminability index (d′ = Z hit rates– Z false alarm rates) was then calculated as the dependent variable measuring performance on recognition and remember responses. The larger the d' value in the discriminability index, the higher the sensitivity and the better the performance of the subjects; the lower the d' value, the lower the sensitivity and the poorer the performance.

Results

Prior to dealing with the data, we first analyzed whether there was a significant difference in the pleasantness ratings between SPT and VT. A One-way ANOVA showed that there was no significant difference, F(1, 38) = 1.04, MSE = 0.20, p = 0.31, and ηp2 = 0.03

Recognition Results

A 2 × 2 (encoding method × instruction) between-subjects ANOVA was conducted to investigate the pattern of the discriminability index shown in Table 2. The main effect of the encoding method was significant, F(1, 76) = 8.76, MSE = 0.56, p = 0.004, and ηp2 = 0.10, with better performance for SPT than for VT. The main effect of instruction was not significant, F(1, 76) = 2.23, MSE = 0.56, p = 0.13, and ηp2 = 0.03. The interaction between the encoding and instruction conditions was significant, F(1, 76) = 9.33, MSE = 0.56, p = 0.003, and ηp2 = 0.11. Simple main-effect tests revealed that memory performance was significantly better in the pleasantness-rating instruction condition than in the standard condition for the VT group, F(1, 76) = 10.33, MSE = 0.56, p = 0.002, and ηp2 = 0.12. However, no such difference emerged in the SPT groups, F(1, 76) = 1.22, MSE = 0.56, p = 0.27, and ηp2 = 0.02. The above results indicate that the pleasantness-rating instruction improves the performance only in VT.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Table shows the mean proportion of hit rates and false-alarm rates, the discriminability index (d′ = Z hit rates– Z false−alarm rates), and the standard deviation in each study condition and response type.

Results for Remember/Know Judgment

A 2 × 2 (encoding × instruction) between-subjects ANOVA was conducted to explore the pattern of the discriminability index of the remember response, as shown in Table 3. There was a significant main effect of the encoding method, F(1, 76) = 8.18, MSE = 0.74, p < 0.005, and ηp2 = 0.10. The main effect of instruction was not significant, F(1, 76) = 1.36, MSE = 0.74, p = 0.25, and ηp2 = 0.02. The interaction between encoding and instruction conditions was significant, F(1, 76) = 11.61, MSE = 0.74, p = 0.01, and ηp2 = 0.13.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Table shows the mean proportions of hit rates and false-alarm rates, the discriminability index (d′ = Z hit rates– Z false−alarm rates), and the standard deviation in each study condition and response type.

Simple main-effect tests revealed that the participants made more remember responses under the pleasantness-rating instruction than under standard instruction in the VT group, F(1, 38) = 10.48.1, MSE = 0.74, p = 0.02, and η2p = 0.12, but not in the SPT group, F(1, 38) = 2.50, MSE = 0.74, p = 0.12, and η2p = 0.03. This shows that the pleasantness-rating instruction significantly increased the incidence of remember responses for VT group, but not for SPT group.

However, for the K response, the main effects of encoding method and instruction were not significant. The corresponding values were F(1, 76) = 0.78, MSE = 0.62, p = 0.38, and η2p = 0.01; and F(1, 76) = 0.25, MSE = 0.62, p = 0.62, and η2p = 0.03. The interaction between the encoding method and instruction was not significant, F(1, 76) = 1.46, MSE = 0.62, p = 0.23, and η2p = 0.02.

Discussion

The aim of Experiment 2 was to verify the results of Experiment 1 and examine the hypothesis that item-specific processing leads to more remember responses in SPT. The results confirmed this expectation. On the one hand, SPT produced better recognition performance and engendered more remember responses under standard instruction, independent of design type. The recognition performance was consistent with previous results showing that the SPT effect is reliable. It was observed whether a between-subjects or within-subjects design was used, and whether the control group was VT or EPT (Engelkamp and Dehn, 2000). On the other hand, the pleasantness-rating instruction significantly increased the incidence of remember responses and the recognition performance for the VT group but not the SPT group. This suggests that SPT already provides an excellent item-specific encoding that can hardly be improved on (Seiler and Engelkamp, 2003; Mulligan and Peterson, 2015; Kubik et al., 2014a,b).

For the know response, the results of Experiments 1 and 2 were not consistent. Experiment 1 showed that VT is more likely to engender know responses than SPT, but no such difference was evident in Experiment 2. It may be that because of the within-subjects design used in Experiment 1, the subjects tended to compare the items they learned in VT with those in SPT at the time of judgment. If the details of learning were less obvious in VT than in SPT, people might have tended to make know responses. However, there is no such situation in the between-subjects design.

General Discussion

In this study, we combined an SPT paradigm with a remember/know judgment paradigm and conducted two experiments to investigate the relations between item-specific processing and retrieval awareness. Experiment 1 used a remember/know paradigm to verify that SPT was accompanied by more re-collective retrieval. Experiment 2 examined whether item-specific processing improved the proportion of subsequent remember responses.

The results verified the hypothesis. Experiment 1 showed a stable SPT effect and that SPT engenders more remember responses, which means that SPT can increase the frequency of recalling re-collective experience. Experiment 2 confirmed these results and revealed that the item-specific processing could improve the recognition performance and increase the incidence of remember responses in the VT condition, but could not improve the memory performance or the incidence of remember responses under the SPT condition, which means that SPT can enhance item-specific processing.

Irrespective of whether a within-subjects or between-subjects design was used, both experiments obtained the result that SPT engenders more remember responses in the judgment, which is consistent with previous results (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995; Lövdén et al., 2002; Manzi and Nigro, 2008); that is, the SPT group more easily recalled re-collective experiences than the VT group. We conclude that the state of retrieval awareness is closely related to the attentional state in the encoding process. Previous studies have shown that people are more involved in events when performing actions (Kormi-Nouri, 2001). This is mainly reflected in the following two aspects: First, SPT requires participants to perform the action represented by the phrase, so they associated the phrase with themselves. Second, to perform actions better, people will pay attention only to action-related information and ignore unrelated environmental data (Engelkamp, 1995). This focused attention gives people more specific information and makes the memory traces more distinct, thereby leading people to remember performed actions more easily (Schatz et al., 2011).

However, the present result is not consistent with Gardiner et al's (2006) research showing that recognition performance and remember responses were not enhanced by SPT. One difference between our research and Gardiner's is that we used normal subjects, as opposed to developmental amnesiac subjects whose episodic memory was impaired and who failed to carry out the actions as effectively as the participants in the control group did.

The results of Experiment 2 supported the view that the item-specific processing leads to the retrieval awareness of SPT. First, previous studies have suggested that people's attention focuses on the verb, the noun, and the integration of the two in an action phrase to make the phrase distinct from other items (Steffens et al., 2006). The more specific information is encoded, the richer the clues that are retrieved. Thus, SPT can increase the frequency of recalling re-collective experience.

Second, VT can equally use item-specific and relational information, which leads to bad performance. However, when the VT group is given pleasantness-rating instruction, it breaks the balance of the use of item-specific and relational information maintained by VT under standard instruction, making VT use too much item-specific information and hindering the use of relational information. Previous studies have suggested that the item-specific processing can provide optimal encoding, thereby letting people achieve high memory performance without additional encoding strategies (Schatz et al., 2011) such as the conceptual strategy (Zimmer and Engelkamp, 1999). We have found that the pleasantness-rating task enhances the recognition performance in VT but not in SPT, indicating that SPT relies mainly on item-specific information.

Third, the incidence of remember responses was affected by the item-specific processing instruction. SPT exhibits a higher incidence of remember responses under standard instruction, whereas under pleasantness-rating instruction, VT and SPT show a similar incidence, which suggests that SPT contains item-specific processing already, and additional item-specific processing does not improve the incidence of remember responses.

Performing actions can enhance the processing of item-specific information. First, for a simple noun-verb phrase, subjects need to understand the meaning of the phrase and then physically enact it. Previous studies have suggested that producing words from definitions can lead to elaborative processing (MacLeod and Daniels, 2000), and generating actions from phrases is similar to this process. Second, the enactment can activate the corresponding mirror neurons (Glover and Dixon, 2013; Goldin-Meadow and Alibali, 2013) and form a motor imagery, which makes the action phrase more specific and distinct (Madan and Singhal, 2012b). Finally, the performing actions accompanied by a higher self-involvement level makes people focus their attention on action-relevant information rather than contextual information (Engelkamp, 1995; Kormi-Nouri, 1995), leading to more distinct memory traces. Hence, people can re-experience the original performed actions more frequently.

The present study does not support the result that SPT depends on relational information processing. If SPT depended on relational information processing, the item-specific processing would be missing and additional processing would be able to improve the performance of SPT like that of VT. However, this is not the case; performance in both recognition and remember/know judgment was unaffected by the additional item-specific processing. This is because, when the action is performed, the individual needs to focus on the action, and the situation cannot be similar to that of VT, where they can examine the associations among the phrases. Therefore, this research supports the view that SPT does not enhance or even hinder relational information processing (Engelkamp, 1995; Steffens, 2007).

Conclusions

The conclusions of this study are as follows:

(1) The SPT effect was obtained under standard instruction, but not under the pleasantness-rating instruction; that is, the pleasantness-rating instruction only improved the recognition performance for VT, suggesting that SPT provides an excellent item-specific encoding that can hardly be enhanced.

(2) SPT allows remember responses more easily in remember/know judgment; that is, SPT can enhance the frequency of recalling re-collective experience. The pleasantness rating instruction significantly increased the incidence of remember responses for the VT group but not the SPT group, thereby suggesting that the greater number of remember responses in SPT was caused by item-specific processing.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of jilin university academic ethics committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the ethics committee.

Author Contributions

LW and GL designed research and wrote the paper. GL collected and analyzed the data.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 31371022), Natural Science Foundation of Jiangsu Province (grant number BK20170235), and Social Science Fund in Colleges and Universities of Jiangsu Province (2016SJB190010).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer DB and handling editor declared their shared affiliation at time of review.

References

Cohen, R. L. (1989). The effects of interference tasks on recency in the free recall of action events. Psychol. Res. 51, 176–180. doi: 10.1007/BF00309145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A., and Dewhurst, S. A. (1995). Remembering, familiarity, and source monitoring. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 48, 125–140. doi: 10.1080/14640749508401380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J. (1995). Visual imagery and enactment of actions in memory. Br. J. Psychol. 86, 227–240. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02558.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for Actions. Hove: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J., and Dehn, D. M. (1997). Strategy and consciousness in remembering subject-performed actions. Sprache Kognit. 16, 94–109.

Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J., and Dehn, D. M. (2000). Item and order information in subject-performed tasks and experimenter-performed tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 671–682. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.3.671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J., Seiler, K. H., and Zimmer, H. D. (2005). Differential relational encoding of categorical information in memory for action events. Mem. Cogn. 33, 371–379. doi: 10.3758/BF03193055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., and Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior research methods, instruments, and computers, 28, 1–11. doi: 10.3758/BF03203630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feyereisen, P. (2009). Enactment effects and integration processes in younger and older adults' memory for actions. Memory 17, 374–385. doi: 10.1080/09658210902731851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of re-collective experience. Mem. Cogn. 16, 309–313. doi: 10.3758/BF03197041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, J. M. (2001). Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a first–person approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 1351–1361. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, J. M., Brandt, K. R., Vargha-Khadem, F., Baddeley, A., and Mishkin, M. (2006). Effects of level of processing but not of task enactment on recognition memory in a case of developmental amnesia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 23, 930–948. doi: 10.1080/02643290600588442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, S., and Dixon, P. (2013). Context and vision effects on real and imagined actions: support for the common representation hypothesis of motor imagery. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 1352–1364. doi: 10.1037/a0031276.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin-Meadow, S., and Alibali, M. W. (2013). Gesture's role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 257–283. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kormi-Nouri, R. (1995). The nature of memory for action events: an episodic integration view. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 337–363. doi: 10.1080/09541449508403103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kormi-Nouri, R. (2001). “The motor component is not crucial,” in Memory for Action: A Distinct form of Episodic Memory, eds H. D. Zimmer, J. D. Cohen, M. S.Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. kormi-Nouri, and M. A. Foley (New York, NY: Oxford), 97–111.

Google Scholar

Kubik, V., Obermeyer, S., Meier, J., and Knopf, M. (2014a). The enactment effect in a multi-trial free-recall paradigm. J. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 781–787. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.959018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubik, V., Olofsson, J. K., Nilsson, L.-G., and Jönsson, F. U. (2016). Putting action memory to the test: testing affects subsequent restudy but not long-term forgetting of action events. J. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 209–219. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1111378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubik, V., Söderlund, H., Nilsson, L. G., and Jönsson, F. U. (2014b). Individual and combined effects of enactment and testing on memory for action phrases. Exp. Psychol. 61, 347–355. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lövdén, M., Rönnlund, M., and Nilsson, L. G. (2002). Remembering and knowing in adulthood: effects of enacted encoding and relations to processing speed. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 9, 184–200.

Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M., and Daniels, K. A. (2000). Direct versus indirect tests of memory: directed forgetting meets the generation effect. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 7, 354–359. doi: 10.3758/BF03212993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, C. R., and Singhal, A. (2012a). Using actions to enhance memory: effects of enactment, gestures, and exercise on human memory. Front. Psychol. 3:507. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, C. R., and Singhal, A. (2012b). Motor imagery and higher-level cognition: four hurdles before research can sprint forward. Cogn. Process. 13, 211–229. doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0438-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnussen, S., and Helstrup, T. (eds.). (2007). Everyday memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Manzi, A., and Nigro, G. (2008). Long-term memory for performed and observed actions: retrieval awareness and source monitoring. Memory 16, 595–603. doi: 10.1080/09658210802070749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mecklenbräuker, S., Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., and Goergens, N. K. (2011). Interactive context integration in children? Evidence from an action memory study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 747–761. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, N. W., and Peterson, D. J. (2015). Negative and positive testing effects in terms of item-specific and relational information. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 859–871. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Persson, J., and Nilsson, L. G. (2002). Individual differences in memory enhancement by encoding enactment: relationships to adult age and biological factors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 835–839. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00074-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L., and Zaromb, F. M. (eds.). (2010). Memory for actions: How different? New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Schatz, T. R., Spranger, T., Kubik, V., and Knopf, M. (2011). Exploring the enactment effect from an information processing view: what can we learn from serial position analyses? Scand. J. Psychol. 52, 509–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00893.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schult, J., von Stülpnagel, R., and Steffens, M. C. (2014). Enactment versus observation: item-specific and relational processing in goal-directed action sequences (and lists of single actions). PLoS ONE 9:e99985. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seiler, K. H., and Engelkamp, J. (2003). The role of item-specific information for the serial position curve in free recall. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 29, 954–964. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffens, M., Buchner, A., Wender, K., and Decker, C. (2007). Limits on the role of retrieval cues in memory for actions: Enactment effects in the absence of object cues in the environment. Mem. Cogn. 35, 1841–1853. doi: 10.3758/BF03192919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffens, M. C. (2007). Memory for goal-directed sequences of actions: Is doing better than seeing? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 1194–1198. doi: 10.3758/BF03193112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., and Mecklenbräuker, S. (2009). Decomposing the memory processes contributing to enactment effects by multinomial modelling. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 61-83. doi: 10.1080/09541440701868668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., Mecklenbräuker, S., and Thompson, E. M. (2006). Decomposing retrieval and integration in memory for actions: a multinomial modeling approach. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 557-576. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canad. Psychol. Psychol. Canad. 26, 1–12. doi: 10.1037/h0080017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 53, 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, M. F., Zimmer, H. D., Zhou, X., and Fu, X. (2016). Enactment supports unitisation of action components and enhances the contribution of familiarity to associative recognition. J. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 932–947. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1229321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer, H. D., Cohen, R., Guynn, M. J., Engelkamp, J., Kormi-Nouri, R., and Foley, M. A. (Eds.). (2001). Memory for Action: A Distinct form of Episodic Memory? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Zimmer, H. D., and Engelkamp, J. (1999). Levels-of-processing effects in subject-performed tasks. Mem. Cogn. 27, 907–914. doi: 10.3758/BF03198543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer, H. D., Helstrup, T., and Nilsson, L.-G. (2007). “Action-events in everyday life and their remembering,” in Everyday Memory, eds T. Helstrup and S. Magnussen (London: Psychology Press-Routledge), 57–92.

Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. The phrases used in the study.

Keywords: subject-performed task, verbal task, remember/know judgments, retrieval awareness, item-specific information

Citation: Li G and Wang L (2018) The Role of Item-Specific Information for the Retrieval Awareness of Performed Actions. Front. Psychol. 9:1325. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01325

Received: 19 January 2018; Accepted: 10 July 2018;
Published: 14 August 2018.

Edited by:

Lionel Brunel, Paul Valéry University, Montpellier III, France

Reviewed by:

Denis Brouillet, Paul Valéry University, Montpellier III, France
Christopher R. Madan, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2018 Li and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lijuan Wang, d2FuZ2xqNjk5QG5lbnUuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.