Skip to main content

DATA REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 24 July 2018
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic From Perception to Action: The Role of Auditory and Visual Information in Perceiving and Performing Complex Movements View all 19 articles

Musical Training in Congenital Hearing Impairment. Effects on Cognitive and Motor Skill in Three Children Using Hearing Aids: Pilot Test Data

\r\nSara Ghiselli*Sara Ghiselli1*Elena CicirielloElena Ciciriello1Giovanni ManiagoGiovanni Maniago2Enrico MuzziEnrico Muzzi1Sandra PellizzoniSandra Pellizzoni3Eva OrzanEva Orzan1
  • 1Department of ENT and Audiology, Institute for Maternal and Child Health IRCCS Burlo Garofolo, Trieste, Italy
  • 2Prematuri Si Nasce Association, Pordenone, Italy
  • 3Department of Life Science, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy

Introduction

Congenital hearing impairment (CHI) is one of the most common sensory deficits (Davis and Davis, 2011) with significant repercussions on cortical brain development and behavior (e.g., Pisoni et al., 2008; Kral, 2013). Studies have shown that cortical auditory pathways develop largely on the basis of sensory experience (Kral et al., 2000; Sharma et al., 2002; Kral and Eggermont, 2007). Late treatment and rehabilitative intervention may therefore critically affect neuropsychological and language development, preventing full access to educational opportunities and hindering participation and self-reliance (Kral, 2013).

Restoration of auditory functions with current hearing device technology (i.e., digital hearing aids and cochlear implants) together with auditory training therapy are crucial to help children learn to understand how to interpret auditory signals, form meaningful sound representations, and develop their own listening strategies. Nevertheless, children with CHI still appear to lag behind their normal-hearing peers in terms of linguistic, cognitive, and motor skills (Kral and O'Donoghue, 2010).

Recent findings indicate that brain regions involved in the processing of music and language at cortical (Tillmann et al., 2006; Koelsch et al., 2009) and subcortical (Strait and Kraus, 2014) level tend to overlap, thus showing a significant degree of affinity between music and language (Patel, 2008). Music training has therefore been increasingly offered to both children with typical development (Besson et al., 2011; Kühnis et al., 2013) and children with various types of atypical development, including individuals with auditory impairments (Yucel et al., 2009; Torppa et al., 2014). Research in this field yielded the following results: (1) children with implants or hearing aids display poorer music perception skills compared to their peers (Scorpecci et al., 2012) but this seem to be related to differences in acoustical hearing prior to cochlear implantation (Hopyan et al., 2012); (2) however, when offered musical training their perception of musical acoustic features improves (e.g., Chen et al., 2010; Stabej et al., 2012; Dastgheib et al., 2013; Rochette et al., 2014; Good et al., 2017). This transfer effect of musical training seems to affect not only language (Yucel et al., 2009; Torppa et al., 2014), but also other cognitive functions (Moreno et al., 2011; Rochette et al., 2014; Benz et al., 2016). Studies mentioned so far present the following limits; (1) their main focus is on primary school children and (2) they investigate the effect of musical training on phonetic awareness, language, and memory (auditory or digit span task), disregarding all other cognitive abilities potentially influenced by music. “Even if the effect of music training on non-auditory findings are preliminary and in need of further corroboration, musical program seem to enhance working memory (Bergman Nutley et al., 2014; Zuk et al., 2014), motor and visual-spatial skills (Benz et al., 2016), and visual attention (Roden et al., 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2014).” This case report focuses on non-instrumental musical training offered to three pre-school children with CHI with the aim of investigating its effects on complex cognitive functions governing language development, such as memory, attention, and motor skills.

Background

The study was carried out at the Audiology Unit of the Institute for Maternal and Child Health—IRCCS “Burlo Garofolo” of Trieste, Italy. Inclusion criteria for patients were set as follows: (1) CHI with mean auditory threshold between 50 and 70 dB HL, corrected with bilateral digital hearing aids; (2) age between 2 and 4 years; (3) absence of cognitive impairment; (4) permanent abode in the area of the Friuli-Venezia-Giulia region. The selection process resulted in the identification of seven eligible candidates for the study, although only three of them took part in the actual activities. The other four lived too far away from the rehabilitation center to be able to attend the training sessions. The Ethical committee of the Institute approved the project (protocol number 614/2016) and informed consent was signed by both parents of each participating child. The three participants were two girls (L.D. and S.V.I.) and one boy (N.C.). At the beginning of their musical training all three of them were aged between 35 and 44 months, had attended nursery school, and had been admitted to their first year of kindergarten. The three participants were also receiving speech-language therapy, which was not interrupted during the study.

Measures

Individual neuropsychological evaluation was carried out before, immediately after and 6 months after the conclusion of the musical training program (MTP). Testing took place in a quite room of the hospital during 60-min sessions. In the following paragraphs each tested task is illustrated, together with the respective evaluation tool.

Attention

Attention was measured using the Leiter-R Attention Sustained task (Roid and Miller, 1997), which ensures good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.83) and good test-retest reliability (r = 0.85).

Memory

Memory abilities were measured using the NEPSY tool, a neuropsychological assessment tool designed for children of 3–12 years of age. NEPSY psychometric properties have proved satisfactory (Korkman et al., 2011). The following NEPSY tasks were selected for our study:

(1) Memory for Designs: this subtest is designed to assess spatial memory by presenting new visual material. It uses a 1–20 scale (M = 10, SD = 3), with good psychometric properties as regards validity and reliability (see Korkman et al., 2011).

(2) Narrative Memory: This subtest is designed to assess recognition and (free or cued) recollection of organized verbal material. It uses a 1–20 scale (M = 10, SD = 3), with good psychometric properties as regards validity and reliability (see Korkman et al., 2011).

Motor Skills

The NEPSY task used was the Manual Motor Sequences subtest, which is designed to assess the ability to imitate a series of rhythmic movement sequences using one or both hands. It uses a 1–20 scale (M = 10, SD = 3), with good psychometric properties as regards validity and reliability (see Korkman et al., 2011).

Evaluation was integrated with two musical ability surveys, administered to the children's parents and music teacher, respectively. The two surveys are described in the following paragraphs.

Musical Ability Survey Administered to the Children's Parents

The survey is a 26-item questionnaire; responses were rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 points (always). Questions concern five areas of musical ability: (a) general reaction and awareness of sounds; (b) music exposure; (c) melody and dynamic variations; (d) rhythm variations; (e) emotional response (Yucel et al., 2009). The total score was calculated as the sum of questionnaire responses over the full range of the scale, with the highest possible score set at 155 points.

Musical Ability Survey Administered to the Children's Music Teacher

The survey is a 15-item questionnaire designed by the music teacher. Responses were rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 points (always). Questions concern various areas of musical training (posture, synchronization, distinction between binary and ternary forms, identification of duration, reading rhythmic notation). The total score was calculated as the sum of questionnaire responses over the full range of the scale, with the highest possible score set at 75 points (Piatti, 1993; Gordon, 2003).

Musical Training

Musical training was organized in two separate sessions with a 2-month break in-between; each session consisted of 10 individual encounters (25 min) and 10 group encounters (45 min). Encounters took place twice per week, for a total of 20 individual encounters and 20 group encounters. This training relied primarily on listening activities and did not involve the acquisition of any specific instrumental program. Activities provided a combination of motor, perceptual, and cognitive tasks, and included training in movement, rhythm, melody, sound-language production, and basic musical concepts.

In the following paragraphs each clinical case is presented individually.

Clinical Cases

N.C. Clinical Case

N.C. was diagnosed with bilateral sensorineural CHI of moderate-to-severe degree. He had been fitted with behind-the-ear digital hearing aids since he was 3 months old. N.C. started MTP when he was 44 months old and attended 36 encounters out of 40. Before the MTP, N.C. had scored 107 in the Short Leiter QI index (Roid and Miller, 1997). Results of the neuropsychological tests administered at the beginning of the MTP (pre-MTP), immediately after (post-MTP), and during the follow-up visit (follow-up) are illustrated in Figure 1 and described in the following paragraphs.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cognitive and motor abilities performed by N.C. before the training (pre-MTP), just after the training (post-MTP) and 6 months after the training (follow-up). (M = 10; SD = 3).

Pre-MTP: N.C. performed very poorly in the narrative memory neuropsychological test of recognition and recall. Results obtained in the memory for drawings and motor sequences test were within the mid-lower average score. Conversely, N.C. performed well in the visual attention test, with results within the higher average bracket. Musical ability surveys returned a score of 78 points out 155 and 5 out of 75 when filled out by the parents and the music teacher, respectively.

Post-MTP: all weighted scores displayed an increase of at least two points. N.C. still performed poorly in the narrative memory test, although his post-MTP results were within the lower average score. Results obtained in the memory for drawings and motor sequences test were significantly higher in the post-MTP phase, ranking within the higher average score. N.C.'s performance in the visual attention test was above the average. Musical ability surveys returned a score of 97 points out 155 and 57 out of 75 when filled out by the parents and the music teacher, respectively, bearing further evidence of the child's improvement.

Follow up: all scores displayed a decrease, with the sole exception of results obtained in the motor coordination test. More specifically, N.C.'s performance in the narrative memory test of recognition and visual attention regressed to their pre-MTP values. Weighted scores obtained in the narrative memory test of recollection and memory for drawings test registered a slight decrease. On the contrary, N.C. obtained 133 points out of 155 in the musical ability survey administered to his parents, thus showing a consistent learning trajectory by indirect evaluation.

L.D. Clinical Case

L.D. was diagnosed with bilateral sensorineural CHI of severe degree. She had been wearing hearing aids since she was 3 months old. L.D. started her MTP when she was 35 months old and attended only 26 encounters out of 40. Before the MTP, L.D. had scored 105 in the Short Leiter QI index (Roid and Miller, 1997). Results of the pre- and post-MTP neuropsychological tests are illustrated in Figure 2 and described in the following paragraphs.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Cognitive and motor abilities performed by L.D. before the training (pre-MTP), just after the training (post-MTP) and 6 months after the training (follow-up). (M = 10; SD = 3).

Pre-MTP: L.D.'s initial deficit was so severe that the narrative memory test of recognition and recollection could not be carried out. Results obtained in the memory for drawings and motor sequences test were within the average, whereas L.D.'s scores in the visual attention test could only reach the lower average score. Musical ability surveys returned a parental score of 105 points out 155 while only 3 out of 75 points in the case of the music teacher scoring.

Post-MTP: all weighted scores displayed an increase of at least five points, with the sole exception of L.D.'s results in the memory for motor sequences test. L.D. performed poorly in the narrative memory test, with post-MTP recognition scores within the lower average score. However, her post-MTP recollection results were within the average. Results obtained in the memory for motor sequences test are consistent with scores obtained in the pre-MTP phase. L.D.'s performance in the visual attention test and memory for drawings test improved, with scores ranking within the higher average score. Musical ability surveys returned a score of 150 points out 155 and 32 out of 75 when filled out by his parents and the musical teacher, respectively, bearing further evidence of the child's improvement.

Follow up: all weighted scores display a decrease, although registered values remain within the average. L.D. obtained 133 points out of 155 in the musical ability survey administered to her parents, thus showing a consistent learning trajectory.

S.V.I. Clinical Case

S.V.I. was diagnosed with bilateral sensorineural CHI of moderate degree. She had been wearing hearing aids since she was 3 months old. S.V.I. started the MTP when she was 38 months old and attended 40 encounters out of 40. Before the MTP, S.V.I. had scored 123 in the Short Leiter QI index (Roid and Miller, 1997). Results of the pre- and post-MTP neuropsychological tests are illustrated in Figure 3 and described in the following paragraphs.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Cognitive and motor abilities performed by S.V.I. before the training (pre-MTP), just after the training (post-MTP) and 6 months after the training (follow-up). (M = 10; SD = 3).

Pre MTP: results obtained in the memory for drawings and motor sequences test were within the mid-lower average bracket. S.V.I. obtained average scores in all the other tests. Musical ability surveys returned a parental score of 155 points out 155 and 19 out of 75 points in the case of the music teacher scoring.

Post-MTP: all weighted scores were in the mid-higher average score. S.V.I.'s performance in the memory for drawings and motor sequences test, which had obtained the lowest scores in the pre-MTP phase, registered the most significant improvement, with an increase of six points. Musical ability surveys confirmed a score of 155 points out 155 in the case of S.V.I.'s parents, while the music teacher reported an improvement with a scoring of 54 out of 75.

Follow up: S.V.I.'s weighted scores obtained in the memory for drawings and motor sequence test registered a slight increase compared to her post-M.T. values. Results obtained in the visual attention test and narrative memory test of recognition registered an improvement consistent with the scores obtained in the post-M.T. phase. S.V.I.'s performance in the narrative memory test of recollection indicates a stability in her learning process, with scores lower than those obtained in the pre-M.T. phase, although still within the higher average score. Parent again confirmed a scoring of 155 points out of 155 in the musical ability survey.

Discussion

Our case report is an initial attempt at quantifying the beneficial effects of non-instrumental musical training on pre-school children with CHI. Tests aimed at investigating various cognitive functions, including memory, attention, and motor skills. Musical training was specifically designed to enhance children's attentional and auditory skills, focusing on the recognition of sound-related aspects, such as timbre, intensity, duration, and pitch, as well as the reproduction of rhythmic models and the combination of rhythm and melody. Investigated learning patterns also included neural-motor skills and sound-gesture coordination skills. Musical training proved beneficial in all the analyzed cognitive areas. More specifically, narrative memory, whose deficit was most significant in all three patients due to CHI repercussions on language production, registered significant improvements, with post-MTP scores ranking within the average and/or its lower scores. Previous studies indicate that patients with CHI greatly rely on their visual skills; our study confirms these results, as all three children registered a significant post-MTP improvement in the visual attention test and memory for drawings test. Such results corroborate the hypothesis according to which visual skills may serve as an effective starting point for future learning. Post-MTP results concerning memory for motor sequences were either preserved or improved, thus bearing witness of MTP's beneficial potential in this area, under-studied in the literature. Moreover, our results provide corroborating evidence of the need for promoting the development of specific motor skills in hearing-impaired children, which may result in severe motor and balance deficits (Gheysen et al., 2008) and difficulties in motor sequencing (Conway et al., 2011), when left untreated.

The report limits lay in the reduced dimension of the analyzed sample of participants, as well as in the fluctuating nature of their performance in time. We strongly believe that these preliminary data need to be further confirmed by new studies with a larger sample and with specific control groups. At the same time, however, these data seem to highlight the need for a recurrent administration of the proposed activities to ensure retention and consistent improvement (Moreno et al., 2015).

Conclusive Remarks

This case report is a preliminary observation that seems to show the efficacy of a multimodal training involving cognitive and motor skills as an effective clinical and rehabilitative tool offered to very young children with CHI using a hearing device. A combined approach may in fact enhance the child's overall skills. Further research with better methodological approach is needed to confirm the extent of benefit from specific musical training activities.

Author Contributions

SG designed the scientific work, drafted the first version of the manuscript and gave the final approval of the manuscript. EC followed the testing procedure of the children and drafted the first version of the manuscript. GM programmed and developed the musical training. EM designed and programmed the musical training. SP critically interpreted the data and gave the final approval of the manuscript. EO supervised the scientific work, provided a critical revision of the work and gave the final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

This paper was supported by MHO, under the internal project RC n° 17/2017.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Association Prematuri si Nasce and the families who participated in the study.

References

Benz, S., Sellaro, R., Hommel, B., and Colzato, L. S. (2016). Music makes the world go round: the impact of musical training on non-musical cognitive functions – a review. Front. Psychol. 6:2023. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergman Nutley, S., Darkiand, F., and Klingberg, T. (2014). Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:926. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besson, M., Chobert, J., and Marie, C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and speech: common processing, attention and memory. Front. Psychol. 2:94. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. K., Chuang, A. Y. C., McMahon, C., Hsieh, T.-H., and Li, L. P.-H. (2010). Music training improves pitch perception in prelingually deafened children with cochlear implants. Pediatrics 125, 793–800. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, C. M., Karpicke, J., Anaya, E. M., Henning, S. C., Henning, S. C., Kronenberger, W. G., et al. (2011). Non verbal cognition in deaf children following cochlear implantation: motor sequencing disturbances mediate language delays. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 237–254. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dastgheib, S. S., Riyassi, M., Anvari, M., and Ghasemi, M. M. (2013). Music training program: a method based on language development and principles of neuroscience to optimize speech and language skills in hearing-impaired children. Iran. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 25, 91–95.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Davis, A. S., and Davis, A. S. (2011). “Descriptive epidemiology of childhood Hearing impairment,” in Comprehensive Handbook of Pediatric Audiology 2Edn, eds R. Seewald and A. M. Tharpe (San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing Inc.), 85–111.

Google Scholar

Gheysen, F., Loots, G., and Van Waelvelde, H. (2008). Motor development of deaf children with and without cochlear implants. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 13, 215–224. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enm053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, A., Gordon, K. A., Papsin, B. C., Nespoli, G., Hopyan, T., Peretz, I., et al. (2017). Benefits of music training in deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 38, 455–464. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, E. (2003). A Music Learning Theory for Newborn and Young Children. Milano: Curci ed.

Google Scholar

Hopyan, T., Peretz, I., Chan, L. P., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. (2012). Children using cochlear implants capitalize on acoustical hearing music percetion. Front. Psychol. 3:425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Schulze, K., Sammler, D., Fritz, T., Müller, K., and Gruber, O. (2009). Functional architecture of verbal and tonal working memory: an FMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 859–873. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korkman, M., Kirk, U., and Kemp, S. (2011). NEPSY– II. Italian Adaptation, 2nd Edn. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Kral, A. (2013). Auditory critical periods: a review from system's perspective. Neuroscience 247, 117–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kral, A., and Eggermont, J. J. (2007). What's to lose and what's to learn: development under auditory deprivation, cochlear implants and limits of cortical plasticity. Brain Res. Rev. 56, 259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., and Heide, S. (2000). Congenital auditory deprivation reduces synaptic activity within the auditory cortex in a layer-specific manner. Cereb. Cortex 10, 714–726. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.7.714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kral, A., and O'Donoghue, G. M. (2010). Profound deafness in childhood. N. Engl. J. Med. 363, 1438–1450. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0911225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnis, J., Elmer, S., Meyer, M., and Jäncke, L. (2013). The encoding of vowels and temporal speech cues in the auditory cortex of professional musicians: anEEGstudy. Neuropsychologia 51, 1608–1618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, G., Cepeda, N. J., and Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1425–1433. doi: 10.1177/0956797611416999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Lee, Y., Janus, M., and Bialystok, E. (2015). Short-term second language and music training induces lasting functional brain changes in early childhood. Child Dev. 86, 394–406. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press.

PubMed Abstract

Piatti, M. (1993). Progettare l'Educazione Musicale. Bologna: Ed Cappelli.

Pisoni, D. B., Conway, C. M., Kronenberger, W. G., Horn, D. L., Karpicke, J., and Henning, S. C. (2008). “Efficacy and effectiveness of cochlear implants in deaf children,” in Deaf Cognition: Foundations and Outcomes, eds M. Marschark and P. Hauser (NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press), 52–101.

Google Scholar

Rochette, F., Moussard, A., and Bigand, E. (2014). Music lessons improve auditory perceptual and cognitive performance in deaf children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:488. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roden, I., Könen, T., Bongard, S., Frankenberg, E., Friedrich, E. K., and Kreutz, G. (2014). Effects of music training on attention, processing speed and cognitive music abilities – findings from a longitudinal study. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 545–557. doi: 10.1002/acp.3034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, A. C., Loureiro, M., and Caramelli, P. (2014). Visual memory in musicians and non-musicians. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:424. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roid, G. H., and Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Col.

Google Scholar

Scorpecci, A., Zagari, F., Mari, G., Giannantonio, S., D'Alatri, L., Di Nardo, W., et al. (2012). Investigation on the music perception skills of Italian children with coclea implants. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 76, 1507–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., and Spahr, A. J. (2002). A sensitive period for the development of the central auditory system in children with cochlear implants; implications for age of implantation. Ear Hear. 23, 532–539. doi: 10.1097/00003446-200212000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stabej, K. K., Smid, L., Gros, A., Zargi, M., Kosir, A., Vatovec, J., et al. (2012). The music perception abilities of prelingually deaf 322 children with cochlear implants. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol. 76, 1392–1400. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strait, D. L., and Kraus, N. (2014). Biological impact of auditory expertise across the life span: musicians as a model of auditory learning. Hear. Res. 308, 109–121. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillmann, B., Koelsch, S., Escoffier, N., Bigand, E., Lalitte, P., and Friederici, A. D. (2006). Cognitive priming in sung and instrumental music: activation of inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage 31, 1771–1782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torppa, R., Faulkner, A., Huotilainen, M., Järvikivi, J., Lipsanen, J., Laasonen, M., et al. (2014). The perception of prosody and associated auditory cues in early implanted children: the role of auditory working memory and musical activities. Int. J. Audiol. 53, 182–191. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2013.872302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yucel, E., Sennaroglu, G., and Belgin, E. (2009). The family oriented musical training for children with cochlear implants: speech and musical perception results of two year follow-up. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 73, 1043–1052. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuk, J., Benjamin, C., Kenyon, A., and Gaab, N. (2014). Behavioral and neural correlatesofexecutivefunctioninginmusiciansandnon-musicians. PLoSONE 9:e99868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: congenital hearing impairment, hearing aid, early intervention, cognitive ability, musical training

Citation: Ghiselli S, Ciciriello E, Maniago G, Muzzi E, Pellizzoni S and Orzan E (2018) Musical Training in Congenital Hearing Impairment. Effects on Cognitive and Motor Skill in Three Children Using Hearing Aids: Pilot Test Data. Front. Psychol. 9:1283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01283

Received: 27 February 2018; Accepted: 04 July 2018;
Published: 24 July 2018.

Edited by:

Penny McCullagh, California State University, East Bay, United States

Reviewed by:

Frank A. Russo, Ryerson University, Canada
Laura Traverso, University of Genoa, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Ghiselli, Ciciriello, Maniago, Muzzi, Pellizzoni and Orzan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Ghiselli, sara.ghiselli@burlo.trieste.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.